How to differentiate hemianesthesia from left tactile neglect: A preliminary case report

Similar documents
battery assessing general cognitive functioning (Mini-Mental State Examination, MMSE),

Pain is more than an unpleasant feeling associated with a somatosensory sensation.

Space amputation in neglect?

Left hand movements and right hemisphere activation in unilateral spatial neglect: a test of the interhemispheric imbalance hypothesis

Explicit versus Implicit Processing of Representational Space in Neglect: Dissociations in Accessing the Mental Number Line

Unilateral Spatial Neglect USN

Validity of the italian telephone version of the mini-mental state examination in the elderly healthy population

B rain damaged patients may report only the stimulus

Dominant Limb Motor Impersistence Associated with Anterior Callosal Disconnection

LOST IN SPACE THE FATE OF MEMORY REPRESENTATIONS FOR NON- NEGLECTED STIMULI

Disorders of Object and Spatial perception. Dr John Maasch Brain Injury Rehabilitation Service Burwood Hospital.

Behavioral and Brain Functions

H emispatial neglect is a common disabling

Sensorimotor Functioning. Sensory and Motor Systems. Functional Anatomy of Brain- Behavioral Relationships

Spatial working memory capacity in unilateral neglect

Voxel-based Lesion-Symptom Mapping. Céline R. Gillebert

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex


10/10/2011. Tools for Visualization and Labeling. SPM MRIcron MRIcroGL xjview MARINA FreeSurfer Talairach Daemon SPM Anatomy Toolbox WFU Pickatlas

Motor intentional disorders in right hemisphere stroke

Auditory Peripersonal Space in Humans: a Case of Auditory Tactile Extinction

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

SPECIAL INTEREST GROUP MEETING

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Effects Of Attention And Perceptual Uncertainty On Cerebellar Activity During Visual Motion Perception

fmri and Voxel-based Morphometry in Detection of Early Stages of Alzheimer's Disease

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism

Impaired perception of temporal order in auditory extinction

Two eyes make a pair: facial organization and perceptual learning reduce visual extinction

Our senses provide us with wonderful capabilities. If you had to lose one, which would it be?

Higher Cortical Function

what do the numbers really mean? NIHSS Timothy Hehr, RN MA Stroke Program Outreach Coordinator Allina Health

Excellent Network Courses. Department of Neurology Affiliated hospital of Jiangsu University

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:.

Quantitative Neuroimaging- Gray and white matter Alteration in Multiple Sclerosis. Lior Or-Bach Instructors: Prof. Anat Achiron Dr.

Chapter 14: The Cutaneous Senses

Unilateral spatial neglect due to right frontal lobe haematoma

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia

Spatial attention: normal processes and their breakdown

Research Article Long-Term Efficacy of Prism Adaptation on Spatial Neglect: Preliminary Results on Different Spatial Components

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m.

Left unilateral neglect or right hyperattention?

Brief communication Contralateral visual search deficits following TMS

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Coding of Far and Near Space in Neglect Patients

fmri Evidence for Modality-Specific Processing of Conceptual Knowledge on Six Modalities

Visual Selection and Attention

Cortical Control of Movement

Overview of Questions

Unilateral neglect (ULN) (or neglect ) is a common behavioral

Learning Objectives.

Storage and processing working memory functions in Alzheimer-type dementia

Funding: NIDCF UL1 DE019583, NIA RL1 AG032119, NINDS RL1 NS062412, NIDA TL1 DA

The Effect of Constraint-Induced Movement Therapy on Upper Extremity Function and Unilateral Neglect in Person with Stroke

Sensory and response contributions to visual awareness in extinction

Figure 1. Excerpt of stimulus presentation paradigm for Study I.

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

Identification of Neuroimaging Biomarkers

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Evoked Potential Correlates of Conscious and Unconscious Vision in Parietal Extinction Patients

Repetition-priming effect: a cognitive task for the definition of a clinical assessment

Supplementary Information

Author's response to reviews

Stroke School for Internists Part 1

Supplementary Online Content

Uni- and cross-modal temporal modulation of tactile extinction in right brain damaged patients

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience

University of Manitoba - MPT: Neurological Clinical Skills Checklist

It Doesn t Take A Lot of Brains to Understand the Brain: Functional Neuroanatomy Made Ridiculously Simple

Cognitive Neuroscience Attention

Supplementary Online Content

Anatomy of Spatial Attention: Insights from Perfusion Imaging and Hemispatial Neglect in Acute Stroke

Assessment of Somatosensory

Thalamus and Sensory Functions of Cerebral Cortex

FUNCTIONAL MAGNETIC RESONANCE EVIDENCE OF CORTICAL ALTERATIONS IN A CASE OF REVERSIBLE CONGENITAL LYMPHEDEMA OF THE LOWER LIMB: A PILOT STUDY

Supporting Information

VIII. 10. Right Temporal-Lobe Contribution to the Retrieval of Family Relationships in Person Identification

Via Giustiniani, 5, Padova; Phone ; Fax

Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex. 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations. 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage

NeuroImage: Clinical

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

performance of verbal working memory tasks

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Notes

Homework Week 2. PreLab 2 HW #2 Synapses (Page 1 in the HW Section)

This is a repository copy of Apathy in Mild Parkinson's Disease: Neuropsychological and Neuroimaging Evidence..

The origins of localization

The Neuropsychology of

Nicoletta Beschin a, Gianna Cocchini b c, Rory Allen b & Sergio Della Sala d a Neuropsychological Centre, Rehabilitation

Persisting asymmetries of vision after right side lesions

1. Processes nutrients and provides energy for the neuron to function; contains the cell's nucleus; also called the soma.

Unit 3: The Biological Bases of Behaviour

Attentional Control 1. Identifying the neural systems of top-down attentional control: A meta-analytic approach

Transcription:

Behavioural Neurology 26 (2013) 151 155 151 DOI 10.3233/BEN-2012-110225 IOS Press Clinical Note How to differentiate hemianesthesia from left tactile neglect: A preliminary case report Marco Pitteri a, Annalena Venneri a,b, Francesca Meneghello a and Konstantinos Priftis a,c, a Laboratory of Neuropsychology, IRCCS San Camillo Hospital, Venezia-Lido, Italy b Department of Neuroscience, University of Sheffield, and Sheffield Teaching Hospital Foundation NHS Trust, Sheffield, UK c Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy Abstract. When assessing for the presence of hemianesthesia, the examiner touches the body of the patients, and requests that they report verbally the location of the delivered tactile stimulus. Contralesional omissions of single tactile stimuli, however, might be due to either primary somatosensory deficits or to spatial attention impairment (i.e., neglect). In this preliminary study, we tested whether clinical assessment can be improved to differentiate between these two types of deficit by modifying the assessment procedure. K.L., a patient with left unilateral neglect, was asked to detect tactile stimuli delivered in two conditions: spatial attention distributed either to his left or to his right hand, and spatial attention focused only on his left hand. Note that K.L. did not receive double simultaneous tactile stimuli. In the distributed spatial attention condition, K.L. omitted most of the single tactile stimuli delivered to his left hand. In the focused attention condition, K.L. was asked to focus his spatial attention only on his left hand. Under this latter condition, his performance increased dramatically, suggesting that his omissions were not due to hemianesthesia, but rather reflected left tactile neglect. In line with the neuropsychological findings, voxel based analysis of his grey and white matter damage confirmed significant loss in areas associated with left-sided neglect, but sparing of the primary somatosensory cortex. This result suggests that standard somatosensory assessment and differential diagnosis between hemianesthesia and tactile neglect may be more accurate when neuropsychology-based procedures are incorporated in the standard neurological examination. Keywords: Tactile neglect, hemianesthesia, spatial attention, neurological examination, lesion mapping 1. Introduction Up to 65% of stroke patients show contralesional somatosensory loss (i.e., hemianesthesia), which can affect their functional outcome [1]. To test for the presence of hemianesthesia, in the standard neurological assessment procedures, typically the examiner touches both hemibodies of the patient, and asks the patient to report verbally the presence, location, and/or intensity of the delivered tactile stimulus. Contralesional omissions of tactile stimuli, however, can be due to either somatosensory or attention deficits. For instance, Corresponding author: Konstantinos Priftis, Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Italy. E-mail: Konstantinos. priftis@unipd.it. patients with left unilateral neglect (LUN) following right-hemisphere damage fail to report,orient to, or verbally describe stimuli in the left side of space [2]. LUN is normally due to the patients failure to orient automatically their spatial attention towards the contralesional side of space [3]. LUN has been observed for each sensory modality (visual, auditory, tactile, etc.). Patients with LUN are able, however, to orient voluntarily their spatial attention focus towards the contralesional side of space when receiving an explicit cue [4]. One of the major diagnostic challenges in the assessment of patients with right hemisphere lesions is ascertaining whether omissions of contralesional stimuli reflect a primary sensory disorder or are the expression of LUN. If patients with right hemisphere damage fail to detect single tactile stimuli delivered on the left limb, ISSN 0953-4180/13/$27.50 2013 IOS Press and the authors. All rights reserved

152 M. Pitteri et al. / Tactile neglect vs. hemianesthesia this often leads to a diagnosis of left hemianesthesia ignoring the possibility that failure to report contralesional stimuli might be the expression of LUN. Indeed, performance of some of these patients might improve substantially when their left hand is positioned within the right side of space [5,6], suggesting that impairments in detecting contralesional stimuli can be due not only to primary somatosensory deficits, but also to LUN. Improvements cannot be attributed to changes in primary somatosensory deficits, because the innervation of the left limb remains the same independently of its position in space (i.e., left or right hemispace). On the contrary, improvements of this kind can only be explained by a change in the orientation of spatial attention, since changing the position of the left limb by moving it away from the neglected side of space (i.e., left) into the intact one (i.e., right) alters spatial attention and consequent processing of the limb. By delivering tactile stimuli to the left limb when positioned either in the left side of space or in the right side of space, the examiner would be able to differentiate between a left sensory deficit due to hemianesthesia and one due to tactile LUN. Moving of the left limb, however, is not always possible or practical because patients can be affected by spastic hemiparesis, pain, or proprioceptive deficits that prevent them from moving towards and/or maintaining their left limb in the right side of space. A possible alternative to overcome this problem might be to ask patients with LUN to orient voluntarily their spatial attention to the left limb during assessment. We tested this hypothesis in the present preliminary study by asking a patient with LUN (K.L.) and apparent left hemianesthesia to detect, while blindfolded, single tactile stimuli in two conditions: a divided spatial tactile attention condition and a left-oriented spatial tactile attention condition. The divided spatial tactile attention condition was similar to that included in the standard neurological examination for left hemianesthesia. That is, we delivered sequentially (not simultaneously as it is the case in the examination of tactile extinction) single tactile stimuli either to his left or to his right hand randomly, to make him distribute voluntarily his spatial tactile attention to both hands. In the left-oriented spatial tactile attention condition, K.L. was informed that single tactile stimuli would be delivered exclusively to his left hand. This verbal cue allowed him to focus voluntarily his spatial tactile attention to his left hand only. We hypothesized that if the detection of single tactile stimuli delivered to his left handwas influenced by spatial tactile attention deficits, his stimulus detection rate would be higher in the leftoriented condition than in the distributed condition. In contrast, if K.L. s detection of left tactile stimuli was not altered by spatial attention cueing, then his left tactile stimulus omissions would reflect the presence of hemianesthesia. 2. Methods 2.1. Case history K.L., a 54-year-old, right-handed man, with 10 years of education, suffered a right hemisphere hemorrhagic stroke in April 2010. The lesion was localized in the territory of the right medial cerebral artery. He had negative neurological and psychiatric history. During hospitalization, K.L. received neuropharmacologicaltreatment and motor therapy for his left hemiparesis. At the time of testing, K.L. was partially able to walk for short distances by himself with the help of a tripod, but he mainly used a wheelchair for long distance mobility. He gave his informed consent to take part in this study, which was conducted accordingly to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki (revision 2008). 2.2. Neuropsychological assessment Patient K.L. underwent formal neuropsychological evaluation in June 2010. He was alert and cooperative. K.L. was oriented to time, space, and personal information. His social, behavioral, and emotional control remained normal. General cognitive abilities were preserved and he achieved a score of 25/30 on the Mini Mental State Examination [7]. He had no memory deficits (as tested by digit span [8] and verbal paired-associates learning [9]). His performance on the verbal judgment task [10] was borderline but still within the normal range and his non-verbal reasoning (Raven s Colored Progressive Matrices [11]) was preserved. K.L. showed, however, behavioral signs of LUN including spontaneous head and gaze deviation towards the ipsilesional side of space (i.e., right). His performance on the Behavioral Inattention Test [12] and on the Bells test [13] confirmed the presence of LUN for the visual modality. K.L. was able, however, to explore haptically his left hemibody (Fluff test [14]). K.L. s neuropsychological data are shown in Table 1. 2.3. Procedure During examination, K.L. was blindfolded and sat in front of a table. Both his left and his right arms were

M. Pitteri et al. / Tactile neglect vs. hemianesthesia 153 Table 1 K.L. s scores on neuropsychological tests Test K.L. s score Cut-off MMSE (Folstein et al., 1979) 25/30 24 Digit span (Orsini et al., 1987) 4/4 0/4 Verbal paired-associates learning (Novelli et al. 1986) 4/4 0/4 Verbal judgments (Spinnler and Tognoni, 1987) 1/4 0/4 Raven s Coloured Progressive Matrices (Raven, 1947) 19.8/36 18.96/36 Behavioural Inattention Test (Wilson et al., 1987) Conventional part 122/146* 129/146 Behavioural part 61/81* 67/81 Bells test (Gauthier et al., 1989) 28/34* > 3 omissions Fluff test (Cocchini et al., 2001) 14/15 > 2 left-sided omissions Scorebelowthecut-off. placed parallel on the table, in a comfortable position. The examiner checked that K.L. kept his head straight for the duration of the examination. We assessed K.L. s ability to detect single tactile stimuli, by delivering a touch with a swab on the fleshy part of his fingertips, in two conditions: divided spatial tactile attention (e.g., I am going to touch either your left or your right hand ) and left-oriented spatial tactile attention (e.g., I am going to touch only your left hand ). Note that K.L. did not receive double simultaneous tactile stimuli. Each finger was touched three times (15 touches for each hand). Tactile stimuli were delivered randomly among the fingers in variable inter-stimulus intervals. 3. Results 3.1. Behavioral testing In the divided attention condition, K.L. showed an apparently severe left tactile hemianesthesia (correct detections of single tactile stimuli on his left hand: 1/15; correct detections of single tactile stimuli on his right hand: 15/15). His performance, however, improved dramatically in the left-oriented condition (correct detections: 13/15), χ 2 (1) = 19.29, p<0.001, suggesting that his apparently severe left tactile hemianesthesia was not due to a primary somatosensory disorder, but reflected tactile LUN. Note that K.L. s responses were always executed immediately following each touch, showing that he was not guessing. 3.2. Structural MRI acquisition, pre-processing, analysis, and results To verify whether the behavioral data were supported by structural sparing of the primary somatosensory cortex, a morphometric assessment of K.L. was carried out by comparing his grey and white matter values extracted from his 3D brain MRI with those of age-, education-, and gender-matched controls. Three dimensional T1-weighted MRI images of K.L. were acquired on a 1.5 T MRI scanner. Voxel dimensions were 0.937 0.937 1.6 mm 3.Thefield of view was 240 mm with a matrix size of 256 256 124. A number of pre-processing steps were followed to isolate the grey matter (GM) and white matter (WM) from the 3D T1-weighted structural scan before performing the statistical analysis using SPM8 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, UCL, London, UK). To correct for global differences in brain shape, structural images were warped to standard stereotactic space and segmented to extract GM, WM, and cerebral spinal fluid. The GM and WM segments were then modulated to correct for changes in volume, induced by nonlinear normalization, and were smoothed using a Gaussian filter set at 8 mm to reduce possible error from betweensubject variability in local anatomy and to render the data more normally distributed. Smoothed GM and WM segments were entered into the analyses. Anatomical regions were identified using the Talairach Daemon Client (http://www.talairach.org/), following conversion of the Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates extracted from the SPM analyses into Talairach coordinates using the Matlab function mni2tal (http:// imaging.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/downloads/mni2tal/mni2 tal.m). Height threshold was set at p<0.001 (uncorrected) with an extent threshold of at least 50 voxels. Only clusters surviving family-wise error correction were considered significant. Two voxel-based independent t-test comparisons were carried out, between K.L. s GM and WM segments and those of a group of 10 age-, education-, and gender-matched healthy controls (mean age 50.8, SD 9.32, mean education 12.8, SD 4.96), who were scanned with the same MRI protocol used for K.L. Sig-

154 M. Pitteri et al. / Tactile neglect vs. hemianesthesia Fig. 1. Note for readers of the printed version: grey matter differences are shown in dark grey, whereas white matter differences are shown in light grey. Grey and white matter differences between K.L. and the group of 10 neurologically healthy controls. Grey matter differences are shown in red; white matter differences are shown in green. nificant differences between the GM of the patient and that of the controls were found in the region of the right temporal-parietal junction, in the right thalamus, and in the brainstem (see Fig. 1). Similarly, the analysis of the WM segments showed WM loss in parietal and frontal areas of the right hemisphere (Fig. 1). No involvement of the right primary somatosensory cortex was detected by these comparisons, but significant differences were found in the secondary somatosensory cortex (i.e., the inferior part of the postcentral gyrus, BA 2). 4. Discussion K.L. s detection of tactile stimuli delivered to his left hand improved dramatically when his spatial attention was cued and focused on his left hand. The differential level of left tactile stimuli detection under the two assessment conditions suggests that K.L. s apparent left hemianesthesia was not due to a primary somatosensory deficit, but rather reflected tactile LUN. This finding suggests that orienting of spatial attention during assessment of tactile perception might be a useful technique for differentiating between a primary somatosensory deficit (i.e., hemianesthesia) and one originating from tactile LUN. The evidence from this single case study suggests that the standard neurological examination should be improved by including both mixed blocks of single tactile stimuli (i.e., single stimuli -not simultaneous- delivered either to the right or to the left hand) and pure blocks of single tactile stimuli (i.e., single stimuli delivered only to the contralesional hand). Our behavioral findings were supported by the evidence from the morphometric study which showed extensive damage of structures in the region of the right temporalparietal junction, a region associated with spatial attention and damage of which has been extensively shown to result in LUN [15]. No significant difference between K.L. and controls was found in the primary somatosensory cortex (BA 1), although significant differences were detected in the secondary somatosensory cortex (BA 2) in the postcentral gyrus, and in the region of the right parietal operculum, a region which plays an important role in tactile spatial attention [16]. The involvement of this cortical area in K.L. s brain damage is consistent with the observed behavioral deficits and the successful modulation of detection of tactile stimuli by overt orienting of spatial attention. The results of our preliminary study suggest that routine neurological examination and differential diagnosis of somatosensory deficits may reach a higher level of accuracy and be more useful for assessment and rehabilitation planning, when neuropsychology principles (e.g., spatial attention orienting) and short additional procedures (e.g., stimulating each hemibody in a separate session) are added to the standard neurological examination. To the best of our knowledge this approach (i.e., using also a pure block with only left-sided tactile stimuli) is not normally performed by clinicians and it is not described in clinical neurology textbooks. An extended group study, however, including both braindamaged patients and neurologically healthy controls is required for adding internal and external validity to our preliminary behavioral and anatomical findings. The present case study is only the first step in that direction. Acknowledgments We would like to thank K.L. and all controls for their time and effort. We are grateful to Luca Ghezzo, Mariano Trevisan, and dr. Luciano Foscolo for their assistance with the acquisition of MRI scans. References [1] C.E. Winward, P.W. Halligan and D.T. Wade, The Rivermead Assessment of Somatosensory Performance (RASP): Standardization and reliability data, Clin Rehabil 16 (2002), 523. [2] K.M. Heilman, R.T. Watson and E. Valenstein, Clinical Neuropsychology, (4th ed.), New York: Oxford University Press, 2003.

M. Pitteri et al. / Tactile neglect vs. hemianesthesia 155 [3] P. Bartolomeo and S. Chokron, Orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect, Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26 (2002), 217. [4] E. Làdavas, G. Menghini and C. Umiltà, A rehabilitation study of hemispatial neglect, Cogn Neuropsychology 11 (1994), 75. [5] S. Aglioti, N. Smania and A. Peru, Frames of reference for mapping tactile stimuli in brain-damaged patients, J Cognitive Neurosci 11 (1999), 67. [6] N. Smania and S. Aglioti, Sensory and spatial components of somaesthetic deficits following right brain damage, Neurology 45 (1995), 1725. [7] E. Magni, G. Binetti, A. Bianchetti and M. Trabucchi, Minimental state examination: A normative study in Italian elderly population, Eur J Neurol 3 (1996), 198. [8] A. Orsini, D. Grossi, E. Capitani, M. Laiacona, C. Papagno and G. Vallar, Verbal and spatial immediate memory span: Normative data from 1355 adults and 1112 children, Ital J Neurol Sci 8 (1987), 537. [9] G. Novelli, C. Papagno, E. Capitani, M. Laiacona, S.F. Cappa and G. Vallar, Tre test clinici di memoria verbale a lungo termine: Taratura su soggetti normali, Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr 47 (1986), 278. [10] H. Spinnler and G. Tognoni, Standardizzazione e taratura italiana di test neuropsicologici, Ital J Neurol Sci 8 (1987), 47. [11] A. Basso, E. Capitani and M. Laiacona, Raven s Colored Progressive Matrices: Normative values on 305 adult normal controls, Funct Neurol 2 (1987), 189. [12] B. Wilson, J. Cockburn and P. Halligan, Behavioral inattention test, Flempton: Thames Valley Test Company, 1987. [13] L. Gauthier, F. Dehaut and Y. Joanette, The bells test: A quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect, Int J Clin Neuropsyc 11 (1989), 49. [14] G. Cocchini, N. Beschin and M. Jehkonen, The Fluff Test: A simple task to assess body representation neglect, Neuropsychol Rehabil 11 (2001), 17. [15] D.J. Mort, P. Malhotra, S.K. Mannan, C. Rorden, A. Pambakian, C. Kennard et al., The anatomy of visual neglect, Brain 126 (2003), 1986. [16] S.B. Eickhoff, A. Schleicher, K. Zilles and K. Amunts, The human parietal operculum. I. Cytoarchitectonic mapping of subdivisions, Cereb Cortex 16 (2006), 254.