Using Peripheral Processing and Spatial Memory to Facilitate Task Resumption

Similar documents
Ratwani & Trafton: Task Resumption. Spatial Memory Guides Task Resumption. Raj M. Ratwani

Raj M. Ratwani Master of Arts George Mason University, Director: J. Gregory Trafton, Professor Department of Psychology

THE ATTENTIONAL COSTS OF INTERRUPTING TASK PERFORMANCE AT VARIOUS STAGES

INTERRUPTION PRACTICE REDUCES ERRORS

Further Investigations into Post-completion Error: the Effects of Interruption Position and Duration

The Effect of Interruption Duration and Demand on Resuming Suspended Goals

Resumption Lag at Interruptible Timing might not be short in Actual Environment

Further investigations into post-completion error: the effects of interruption position and duration

Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory

A memory for goals model of sequence errors

LEARNING TO MORE EFFECTIVELY MANAGE INTERRUPTIONS OVER REPEATED EXPOSURES: WHEN, HOW, AND WHY?

A memory for goals model of sequence errors

Working Memory Load Affects Device-Specific but Not Task-Specific Error Rates

Examining the Qualitative Gains of Mediating Human Interruptions during HCI

Reminders, Alerts and Pop-ups: The Cost of Computer- Initiated Interruptions

Spatial Transformations in Graph Comprehension

A Perceptual Process Approach to Selecting Color Scales for Complex Visualizations

The spacing and lag effect in free recall

Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance

A PROCESS MODEL OF TRUST IN AUTOMATION: A SIGNAL DETECTION THEORY BASED APPROACH

An Activation-Based Model of Routine Sequence Errors

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

The role of attention in processing configural and shape information in 3D novel objects

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Introduction. A Complex Task Environment

Single Operator, Multiple Robots: An Eye Movement Based Theoretic Model of Operator Situation Awareness

The roles of encoding, retrieval, and awareness. in change detection.

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Task Interruption: Resumption Lag and the Role of Cues

Interrupting Problem Solving: Effects of Interruption Position and Complexity

Unified Modeling of Proactive Interference and Memorization Effort: A new mathematical perspective within ACT-R theory

An Activation-Based Model of Routine Sequence Errors

The Color of Similarity

A negative compatibility effect in priming of emotional faces

Towards Ambient Notifications

Running head: THE EFFECT OF STIMULUS LENGTH AND TYPE

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Cognitive Models of the Influence of Color Scale on Data Visualization Tasks

Notification, Disruption, and Memory: Effects of Messaging Interruptions on Memory and Performance

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Satiation in name and face recognition

The allocation of visual short-term memory capacity: Evidence for a flexible storage mechanism

When facts go down the rabbit hole: Contrasting features and objecthood as indexes to memory

RADAR Oxford Brookes University Research Archive and Digital Asset Repository (RADAR)

Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making

The e#ect of temporal uncertainty on measurements: The case of priming e#ects. Rika MIZUNO and Takao MATSUI (Chubu University)

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks

University of Groningen. Understanding and managing interruptions Katidioti, Ioanna

Acta Psychologica 141 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources

Exam 2 PS 306, Fall 2005

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER

On the failure of distractor inhibition in the attentional blink

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Effects of gaze-aversion on visual-spatial imagination. Lucy Markson. Kevin B. Paterson. School of Psychology. University of Leicester

Augmented Cognition for Tactical Tomahawk Weapon Control System Operators

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Human Control of Multiple Unmanned Vehicles: Effects of Interface Type on Execution and Task Switching Times

Why do we look at people's eyes?

New objects do not capture attention without a sensory transient

Preventing Postcompletion Errors: How Much Cue Is Enough?

Shifting Covert Attention to Spatially Indexed Locations Increases Retrieval Performance of Verbal Information

Cognitive Modeling Reveals Menu Search is Both Random and Systematic

Perceptual grouping in change detection

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention

A Cognitive Model of Social Influence

How do we read YAH maps? An Eye-Tracking Study

Mini symposium on pain and suffering

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features

THE EFFECTS OF MOMENTARY VISUAL DISRUPTION ON HAZARD ANTICIPATION IN DRIVING. Massachusetts, USA

Measuring the Effects of Interruptions on Task Performance in the User Interface

The Effects of Observers Expectations and the Probability of a Change Occurring on Change Detection Performance

Exploring the Impact of Instant Messaging (IM) on User Performance and Perceived Workload

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Modeling Human Behavior from Low-Level Input Analytics

Validity of Haptic Cues and Its Effect on Priming Visual Spatial Attention

Unintended Effects: Varying Icon Spacing Changes Users Visual Search Strategy

Eye Movements During Mental Imagery are Not Reenactments of Perception

Evaluating Eyegaze Targeting to Improve Mouse Pointing for Radiology Tasks

Cognition 113 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Cognition. journal homepage:

Observational Coding Assignment Instructions for doing the assignment

The path of visual attention

Layout Geometry in Encoding and Retrieval of Spatial Memory

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

Rapid fear detection relies on high spatial frequencies

Age of distraction study

The Effect of Interruptions on Primary Task Performance in Safety-Critical Environments

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words

Using the method of adjustment to enhance collision warning perception

Adaptive Memory: Enhanced Location Memory After Survival Processing

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

Transcription:

Using Peripheral Processing and Spatial Memory to Facilitate Task Resumption Raj M. Ratwani 1,2, Alyssa E. Andrews 2, Malcolm McCurry 1, J. Gregory Trafton 1,2, Matthew S. Peterson 2 Naval Research Laboratory 1 George Mason University 2 Washington, D.C. Fairfax, VA Theories accounting for the process of primary task resumption following an interruption have focused on the suspension and retrieval of a specific goal (Altmann & Trafton, 2002). The ability to recall the spatial location of where in the task one was prior to being interrupted may also be important. We show that being able to maintain a spatial representation of the primary task facilitates task resumption. Participants were interrupted by an instant message window that either partially or fully occluded the primary task interface. Reaction time measures show that participants were faster at resuming in the partial occlusion condition. In addition, eye track data suggest that participants were more accurate at returning to where they left off, suggesting that they were able to maintain a spatial representation of the task and use this information to resume more quickly. INTRODUCTION Most studies examining the impact of interruptions on primary task performance have shown that interruptions can be detrimental to accomplishing the primary task (Altmann & Trafton, 2004; Czerwinski, Cutrell, & Horovitz, 2000; Monk, Boehm-Davis, & Trafton, 2004; Trafton, Altmann, Brock, & Mintz, 2003). Interruptions have increased the time required to accomplish the primary task, led to more errors, and elicited greater feelings of stress and anxiety. Altmann and Trafton (2002) have applied an activation based memory for goals model to account for the process of resuming an interrupted task. The focus of their theory has been on what specific goal needs to be retrieved in order to resume the primary task. We have argued that in addition to memory for the specific goal that was suspended, memory for spatial location is an important component to the resumption process (Brudzinski, Ratwani & Trafton, in press; Ratwani & Trafton, 2006). Knowing where you are spatially in a task can guide you to where you left off and can provide the context required to resume the suspended goal. More specifically, in previous experiments we have shown that after an interruption, participants do not start the task over again, but rather are able to return to the general area where they left off. Thus, people have the ability to remember the general location of where they are in the task interface, information that can be used to guide resumption. There has been support for the role of spatial memory in guiding task resumption in the visual search domain as well. First, it has been shown that the spatial representation of a task can be maintained over a delay (Lleras, Rensink & Enns, 2004, 2005). Further it has been suggested that spatial memory aids in resuming interrupted visual searches (Shen & Jiang, 2006). The goal of the experiment presented here was to examine whether a more intact spatial representation of the primary task facilitated task resumption. If remembering where you are spatially in a task is important to resumption, then having a more intact spatial representation of the primary task upon resumption should serve to facilitate the resumption process. In particular, an interrupting task that partially occluded the primary task screen was compared to one that completely occluded the primary task screen. A partially occluding

interrupting task window allowed for peripheral access to the primary task screen and consequently may have resulted in a more intact spatial representation upon resumption of the primary task. If spatial memory is used to resume the task following an interruption, this more intact spatial representation should lead to faster resumption times. EXPERIMENT In the primary task, participants searched a column of numbers in a spreadsheet and transcribed only the odd numbers onto a separate list. While participants performed this task they were periodically interrupted by instant messages that either partially occluded the primary task screen or fully occluded the primary task screen. Reaction time and eye movement data were collected as participants performed the task. We were first interested in examining whether the partial occlusion interruption led to faster resumption times as compared to the full occlusion interruptions. Second, we were interested in examining whether participants were able to resume more accurately in the partial occlusion case, suggesting a more intact spatial representation of the primary task. Method Participants. Thirty-three George Mason University undergraduate students participated for course credit. Materials. Thirty Microsoft Excel spreadsheets were created; each sheet contained 10 three-digit numbers (see Figure 1 for an example). The numbers were randomly generated with the constraint that at least half of the numbers were odd. The numbers were listed in a single column on the left side of the spreadsheet in a random order. The distance between numbers was approximately 2.5 of visual angle. Design. A mixed design was used where interruption vs. control trials were manipulated within subjects, while partial vs. full occlusion instant messages were a between subjects variable. Each spreadsheet served as a trial; half of the spreadsheets had no interruptions (control) and half had one interruption randomly occurring during that trial. Each trial was randomly assigned as a control or interruption condition. Participants were randomly assigned to either the partial or full occlusion group. Procedure. The primary task required participants to type the odd numbers from the column on the left of the spreadsheet into a column labeled odd numbers. They began at the top of the original column in the first spreadsheet and typed the odd numbers into the designated column without leaving spaces between the cells (see Figure 1). They performed the same task on each spreadsheet until all the spreadsheets had been completed; the experiment was self-paced. The interrupting task was an instant message (IM) containing an addition problem with 5 wholenumber addends. In the partial occlusion condition the IM occluded less than 25% of the primary task screen (see Figure 1). In the full occlusion condition the IM occluded 100% of the primary task screen. Once the IM was received the participant was forced to cease work on the primary task and to mentally add the five numbers as quickly and accurately as possible. The participant then typed the answer in the IM window, sent the message, closed the IM window and finally resumed their primary task. The interruption lasted approximately 10 seconds and occurred only after an entire three digit number was entered in the odd numbers column. A single control and interruption trial served as practice trials. Figure 1. Primary task with partial occlusion IM.

Measures. The reaction time (RT) data were analyzed by computing the inter-action interval for the control trials and comparing this to the resumption lags from the interruption trials. The inter-action interval was the average time between entering numbers into the odd numbers column on the spreadsheet. The resumption lag was the average time from the end of the interrupting secondary task (closing the IM window) to the first action back on the primary task (entering an odd number). The eye track data were analyzed using ProtoMatch software (Myers & Scholles, 2005). ProtoMatch defines fixations as a minimum of 6 samples within a default 2 - of visual-angle window resolution. Each cell in the left hand column and the odd numbers column was defined as an area of interest for categorizing the location of fixations. when delivered by the full occlusion IM as compared to the partial occlusion IM as evidenced by the difference in resumption lags. One explanation for this finding might be that participants spent less time on the interruption in the partial occlusion case allowing them to resume faster. We examined the amount of time spent on the interruption (i.e. the interruption duration) and there was no statistical difference between the partial (M = 9.9 sec) and full occlusion conditions (M = 10.7 sec), F(1,31) = 1.4, p =.25. Participants spent the same amount of time on the interrupting task in both conditions. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Reaction Time Data We first sought to determine whether the interruptions were disruptive to primary task performance and whether this difference was influenced by the partial vs. full occlusion manipulation. The inter-action intervals from the control trials were compared to the resumption lags from the interruption trials for both the partial and full occlusion conditions. As Figure 2 suggests, the resumption lags (M = 2.7 sec) were significantly longer than the inter-action intervals (M = 1.2 sec), F(1,31) = 168.9, MSE =.23, p<.001. The main effect of partial vs. full occlusion was nonsignificant; however, there was a significant interaction, F(1,31) = 7.1, MSE =.23, p<.05. Tukey HSD post-hoc comparisons were performed to explore this interaction. There was no difference in the inter-action intervals for the partial (M = 1.2 sec) vs. full occlusion (M = 1.1 sec) conditions. However, the resumption lags in the partial occlusion condition (M = 2.5 sec) were significantly shorter than the resumption lags in the full occlusion condition (M = 2.9 sec), p<.05. As expected, the reaction time measures show that the interruptions were disruptive to primary task performance. Further, the interruptions were more disruptive to primary task performance Figure 2. Reaction time measures. Eye Track Data We examined the eye track data to determine how participants were able to resume more quickly in the partial occlusion interruption condition. The eye track data showed that only one participant fixated on the primary task during the interruption, and that this only occurred on a few of the trials; this amounted to less than 2% of the all the interruption trials for all the participants. Thus, the majority of participants were not making explicit fixations to the primary task during the interruption, suggesting that having peripheral access to the primary task may be influencing participants ability to resume faster. If having peripheral access to the primary task during the interruption facilitates memory for spatial location and consequently resumption of the primary task, there are several straightforward predictions at the perceptual level. First,

participants should be more accurate at returning to where they left off in the primary task following the interruption in the partial occlusion condition. Second, because participants should have a more intact spatial representation and can return closer to where they left off after the interruption, they should be able to resume by making fewer fixations during the resumption lag. In order to examine spatial accuracy when resuming the primary task, we focused on where the participant was prior to being interrupted and compared this location to where they initially returned after completing the interrupting task. We calculated the Euclidean distance in pixels between the location of the pre-interruption fixation and the post-interruption fixation and compared this distance for the partial occlusion and full occlusion conditions. We found that participants were more accurate at returning to where they left off in the partial occlusion condition (M = 109.8 pixels) as compared to the full occlusion condition (M = 131.7 pixels), F(1,31) = 5.6, p<.05 (see Figure 3). Thus, the partial occlusion IM allowed participants to return more closely to where they left off as compared to full occlusion IM. partial occlusion IM (M = 5.8) as compared to a full occlusion IM (M = 7.3), F(1,31) = 6.2, p<.05, as Figure 4 suggests. The eye track data showed the partial occlusion IMs allowed for participants to be more accurate at returning to where they were in the primary task following the interruption and that participants were able to resume with fewer fixations. Importantly, participants were not explicitly fixating on the primary task during the interruption, and there was no difference in the amount of time spent on the interrupting task. Figure 4. Number of fixations during the RL. GENERAL DISCUSSION Figure 3.Distance between fixation points. Next, we examined the number of fixations during the resumption lag. If participants were more accurate at returning to where they left off following the partial occlusion interruption they should be able to resume the task with fewer fixations. We found that participants made fewer fixations during the resumption lag following a Most theories that attempt to account for the resumption process following an interruption focus on the specific goal that was being worked on in the primary task. We have focused on spatial memory and knowing where in the task one was prior to being interrupted. In the experiment presented here, we have shown that by allowing for peripheral access to the primary task interface during an interruption, a more intact spatial representation can be maintained. This spatial representation can facilitate resumption of the primary task upon completion of the interruption by allowing one to return more closely to where he or she was prior to the interruption. These results show that not only is spatial memory important to the resumption process, but also that maintaining a spatial representation of the primary task is possible using

peripheral vision, and this does not increase the amount of time required to handle the interrupting task. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This work was supported in part by grant number 55-8122-06 from the Office of Naval Research to Greg Trafton. We thank Chris Myers for his help with ProtoMatch. REFERENCES Altmann, E.M. & Trafton, J.G. (2002) Memory for goals: an activation-based model. Cognitive Science, 26(1), 39-83. Altmann, E. M., & Trafton, J. G. (2004). Task interruption: Resumption lag and the role of cues. In Proceedings of the 26th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society: Erlbaum. Brudzinski, M.E., Ratwani, R.M., & Trafton, J.G. (in press). What to Where: Goal Memory Needs Spatial Memory. In Proceedings of the 8 th International Conference on Cognitive Modeling. Czerwinski, M., Cutrell, E., & Horovitz, E. (2000). Instant messaging: Effects of relevance and time. In Proceedings of CHI 2000 Conference: ACM. Lleras, A., Rensink, R. A., & Enns, J. T. (2005). Rapid resumption of interrupted visual search. Psychological Science, 16(9), 684-688. Monk, C. A., Boehm-Davis, D. A., & Trafton, J. G. (2004). Recovering from interruptions: Implications for driver distraction research. Human Factors, 46(4), 650-663. Myers, C. W., & Schoelles, M. J. (2005). ProtoMatch: A tool for analyzing high-density, sequential eye gaze and cursor protocols. Behavior Research Methods, 37(2), 256-270. Ratwani, R.M. & Trafton, J.G. (2006). Now Where Was I? Examining the Perceptual Processes while Resuming and Interrupted Task. In Proceedings of the 28 th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Vancouver, Canada. Shen, Y. & Jiang, Y. (2006). Interrupted visual searches reveal volatile search memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. Trafton, J. G., Altmann, E. M., Brock, D. P., & Mintz, F. E. (2003). Preparing to resume an interrupted task: Effects of prospective goal encoding and retrospective rehearsal. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 58, 583-603.