Aircraft Noise Effects on Sleep: Final Results of DLR Laboratory and Field Studies of 2240 Polysomnographically Recorded Subject Nights

Similar documents
The sleep disturbance index a measure for structural alterations of sleep due to environmental influences

NORAH Sleep Study External Comment Mathias Basner, MD, PhD, MSc

ProPG PLANNING &NOISE. Noise Events. New Residential Development. Dani Fiumicelli. Birmingham. 22nd June 2017

NORAH. Knowledge No. 10. NORAH noise impact study. Aviation noise and nocturnal sleep. Results. Quality of Life Health Development NORAH

Aircraft noise effects on sleep: A systematic comparison of EEG awakenings and automatically detected cardiac arousals

Pilot study examining the effects of aircraft noise on sleep in communities near Philadelphia International Airport

Design for a U.S. Field Study on the Effects of Aircraft Noise on Sleep

NORAH (Noise Related Annoyance, Cognition, and Health): Questions, designs, and main results

INTRODUCTION. 12th ICBEN Congress on Noise as a Public Health Problem

Milena Pavlova, M.D., FAASM Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital Assistant Professor of Neurology, Harvard Medical School Medical

Noise-related sleep disturbance

Night-time noise events and awakening

European Wind Energy Association Oxford 12th December 2012

12th ICBEN Congress on Noise as a Public Health Problem

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep

Experimental studies on sleep disturbances due to railway and road traffic noise

Simplest method: Questionnaires. Retrospective: past week, month, year, lifetime Daily: Sleep diary What kinds of questions would you ask?

87 pages 25 figures 7 tables 30 references

STAcoustics. Noise and Health: Tools for Assessing Night Noise Impact. Stephen Turner, MA, MSc, HonFIOA. Director Stephen Turner Acoustics Limited

Appendix E: Basics of Noise. Table of Contents

Appendix 1. Practice Guidelines for Standards of Adult Sleep Medicine Services

Graduate school of Science and Technology, Kumamoto University, Japan. Corresponding author's address:

Basics of Polysomnography. Chitra Lal, MD, FCCP, FAASM Assistant professor of Medicine, Pulmonary, Critical Care and Sleep, MUSC, Charleston, SC

The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events

Webinar Q&A Report Noninvasive, Automated Measurement of Sleep, Wake and Breathing in Rodents

Arousal detection in sleep

Aircraft Noise, Sleep Disturbance and Health Effects: A Review

Effects of Traffic Noise on Sleep: A Review

[ L] Environmental Impacts, Threshold Levels and Health Effects

LEARNING MANUAL OF PSG CHART

Fragmenting Sleep Diminishes Its Recuperative Value

COMPARATIVE EVALUATION OF SLEEP DISTURBANCE DUE TO NOISES FROM AIRPLANES, TRAINS AND TRUCKS

Noise, nuance and nuisance: an introduction to sound and sleep. Kevin Morgan Clinical Sleep Research Unit Loughborough University

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Recognition of Sleep Dependent Memory Consolidation with Multi-modal Sensor Data

Indoor Noise Annoyance Due to Transportation Noise

Periodic Leg Movement, L-Dopa, 5-Hydroxytryptophan, and L-Tryptophan

Nocturnal road traffic noise and sleep: Day-by-day variability assessed by actigraphy and sleep logs during a one week sampling. Preliminary findings

Study «CLIMSOM & Sleep Quality»

ICANA - Frankfurt 12 to 13 November External speaker: Dr Ian H Flindell, University of Southampton

Procedures in the Sleep Laboratory

EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples. A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association

Disclosures. Acknowledgements. Sleep in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Window to Treatment and Etiology NONE. Ruth O Hara, Ph.D.

Aircraft Noise as a Public Health Problem

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle

Measuring sleep and sleepiness with mobile devices

H-Reflex Suppression and Autonomic Activation During Lucid REM Sleep: A Case Study

An active unpleasantness control system for indoor noise based on auditory masking

Western Hospital System. PSG in History. SENSORS in the field of SLEEP. PSG in History continued. Remember

Noise 101. Sources Metrics Noise Modeling Federal Statutes. O Hare Noise Compatibility Commission. June 16, 2017

Issues in the measurement of sleep-related noise events in road traffic streams

Effect of background noise levels on community annoyance from aircraft noise

THE EFFECT OF CARPAL TUNNEL SYNDROME PAIN ON SLEEP ARCHITECTURE

How to Forecast Community Annoyance in Planning Noisy Facilities

linkedin.com/in/lizziehillsleeptechservices 1

Supplement. Aircraft Noise Terminology & Metric

[ V] Environmental Impacts, Threshold Levels and Health Effects

Sleep Stages Solution v0.1

Blood Pressure and Heart Rate During Continuous Experimental Sleep Fragmentation in Healthy Adults

ROAD TRAFFIC NOISE ANNOYANCE IN RELATION TO AVERAGE NOISE LEVEL, NUMBER OF EVENTS AND MAXIMUM NOISE LEVEL

TOPIC H: NOISE. Richmond State of the Environment 2001

Effects of Aircraft Noise on Student Learning

The Impact of Patient-Ventilator. Karen J Bosma, MD, FRCPC Critical Care Medicine and Respirology

NeuRA Sleep disturbance April 2016

Traffic noise effects on autonomic arousals during sleep

Portable Noise Monitoring Report August 15 - October 11, 2013 Woodland Park Elementary School. Vancouver Airport Authority

Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound: Do We Need a Fixed SWS Amount per Night?

CHAPTER 6 INTERFERENCE CANCELLATION IN EEG SIGNAL

NATIONAL COMPETENCY SKILL STANDARDS FOR PERFORMING POLYSOMNOGRAPHY/SLEEP TECHNOLOGY

The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events

THE EFFECT OF WHOLE-BODY VIBRATION ON HUMAN PERFORMANCE AND PHYSIOLOGICAL FUNCTIONS

Non-auditory: 9th International Congress on Noise as a Public Health Problem (ICBEN) 2008, Foxwoods, CT

Sleep Questionnaire. If yes, what? If yes, how would you describe it? Please explain? If yes, what times are these?

WYETH LABORATORIES SITE HUNTERCOMBE LANE SOUTH TAPLOW ENVIRONMENTAL NOISE SURVEY & ASSESSMENT REPORT 4390/ENS. 25 March 2011 Revision Number: 0

Sleep and Sleep Hygiene in an Occupational Health & Safety Context

LEARNING DURING SLEEP: AN INDIRECT TEST OF THE ERASURE-THEORY OF DREAMING

Before taking field measurements, it is important to determine the type of information required. The person making the measurement must understand:

The Efficacy of a Restart Break for Recycling with Optimal Performance Depends Critically on Circadian Timing

The effects of aircraft noise exposure on subjective sleep quality: the results of the DEBATS study in France

The Residence Inn Hotel at 2 nd and Mace

How is Sleep Quality and Noise Sensitivity of Residents Living in Proximity to Largest Wind Farm of Iran?

Community response to Shinkansen noise and vibration: a survey in areas along the Sanyo Shinkansen Line

What Is the Moment of Sleep Onset for Insomniacs?

Classification of Pre-Stimulus EEG of K-complexes using Competitive Learning Networks

Automated Sleep Stage Analysis Using Hybrid Rule-Based and Case-Based Reasoning

NORAH Overview. Rainer GUSKI. Ruhr-University Bochum, Germany

Project: COOK ISLAND CHRISTIAN CHURCH. Prepared for: Cook Island Christian Church 34 Dunbeath Crescent Kew Invercargill Attention: Tina Maine

Sleep Stage Estimation By Evolutionary Computation Using Heartbeat Data and Body-Movement

SLEEP POLYSOMNOGRAPHIC VARIABLES IN PATIENTS WITH LOW BACK PAIN. A COMPARATIVE STUDY IN TWO DIFFERENT CONDITIONS.

OVERVIEW. Somnuva: a new way to sleep

Aircraft Noise and Quality of Life around Frankfurt Airport

Effects of speaker's and listener's environments on speech intelligibili annoyance. Author(s)Kubo, Rieko; Morikawa, Daisuke; Akag

Appendix D-1. Introduction to Aircraft Noise and Its Effects

Low Frequency Noise and Its Assessment and Evaluation

Physiology of Normal Sleep: From Young to Old

AGING AND SLEEP STAGE EFFECTS ON ENTROPY OF ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAM SIGNALS

ASSOCIATIONS OF ACOUSTIC ENVIRONMENT WITH PHYSIOLOGICAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL STATES AMONG HOSPITALIZED PATIENTS IN GENERAL HOSPITAL WARDS

SLEEP STAGING AND AROUSAL. Dr. Tripat Deep Singh (MBBS, MD, RPSGT, RST) International Sleep Specialist (World Sleep Federation program)

COMMUNITY ANNOYANCE AND SLEEP DISTURBANCE FROM RAILWAYNOISE IN KOREA ABSTRACT

states of brain activity sleep, brain waves DR. S. GOLABI PH.D. IN MEDICAL PHYSIOLOGY

Transcription:

The 33 rd International Congress and Exposition on Noise Control Engineering Aircraft Noise Effects on Sleep: Final Results of DLR Laboratory and Field Studies of 2240 Polysomnographically Recorded Subject Nights M. Basner, H. Buess, U. Müller, G. Plath, A. Samel Institute of Aerospace Medicine, German Aerospace Center (DLR), 51170 Köln, Germany mathias.basner@dlr.de Abstract [500] OBJECTIVES Sleep is vital for the recovery of physical and mental capacities. Environmental noise is a potential disrupter of the sleep process. In order to develop scientifically sound criteria for the operation and restriction of nocturnal air traffic, the DLR-Institute of Aerospace Medicine investigated the influence of nocturnal aircraft noise on sleep, mood and behavior in four representative laboratory and two field studies between 1999 and 2003. METHODS In the lab, 128 subjects aged 18-65 were investigated during 13 consecutive nights (total: 1,664 nights). Between 4 and 128 aircraft noise events with LAS,max between 45 and 80 db(a) were played back between 11 p.m. and 7 a.m. Results were compared to the findings of two field studies with 64 subjects and 576 nights in total. Here, sound pressure levels were simultaneously measured indoors and outdoors. Electrophysiological signals included polysomnography, EKG, plethysmography, respiration and actigraphy. Synchronous recording with acoustic data assured event related analysis. RESULTS Random effects logistic regression was used for the prediction of noise induced awakenings depending on LAS,max and the calculation of dose response curves. A comparison of the awakening probability between lab and field revealed striking differences. Based on these results, new criteria for the restriction of nocturnal air traffic were developed and will be presented. CONCLUSIONS The combination of a huge database and new statistical procedures allows the precise prediction of the influence of nocturnal aircraft noise on sleep, which again can be used for the proposal of advanced rules for the protection of residents living near airports. 1 INTRODUCTION Sleep is vital for the recovery of physical and mental capacities. Environmental noise is able to interfere with the sleep process. In order to develop scientifically sound criteria for the restriction of nocturnal air traffic, the DLR-Institute of Aerospace Medicine investigated the influence of nocturnal aircraft noise on sleep, mood and behavior in four representative laboratory and two field studies between 1999 and 2003. By use of the electroencephalogram (EEG), the electrooculogram (EOG) and the electromyogram (EMG) and after dividing the night into 30-second segments each segment of sleep can be classified into the wake state and five distinct sleep stages [1]: stages 1-4 and REM sleep. Stage 1 and stage 2 sleep are called light sleep, whereas stage 3 and stage 4 are known as deep or slow wave sleep (SWS). REM-sleep and especially deep sleep seem to be very important for the process of memory consolidation, whereas stage 1 sleep seems to contribute only little if all to the recuperative value of sleep. 1/8

Environmental noise is a potential disruptor of the sleep process. It may lead to decreases in total sleep time (TST) and changes in sleep structure, to more frequent awakenings and to activations of the vegetative nervous system. The process of sleep stage classification is time consuming and therefore expensive. Hence, there have only been a few and small studies in the past using polysomnography (EEG, EOG, EMG) for the investigation of the effects of environmental noise on sleep. 2 METHODS 2.1 Laboratory Studies 128 volunteers free of intrinsic sleep disorders and aged 18-65 (mean age: 38) were examined for thirteen consecutive nights in our soundproof sleep facility, which is situated in the basement of the institute. 8 separate sleep cabins allowed the simultaneous observation of 8 volunteers. 16 subjects served as a control group and did not receive aircraft noise at all during the 13 nights. Electrophysiological variables were measured continuously during the night and included: Electroencephalogram (EEG), Electromyogram (EMG) and Electrooculogram (EOG) Electrocardiogram (EKG) Finger pulse amplitude (plethysmography) Respiration (movements of the rib cage and airflow at mouth and nose) Control variables were gender, age, educational, psychological and medical status, personal attitude towards aircraft noise and aircraft noise annoyance. Each laboratory study consisted of 4 groups of 8 volunteers each who were examined over a period of 8 weeks (4 x 13 nights). Nights 1 and 2 as well as nights 12 and 13 served as adaptation, baseline and recovery nights respectively. Lights were turned off at 11 p.m. and again on at 7 a.m. In the noise nights, aircraft noise events (ANE) with varying frequencies of occurrence (4, 8, 16, 32, 64 or 128 events per night with minimum intervals of 3, 7, 14, 30, 60 and 120 min respectively) and noise levels ranging from 45 to 80 db L AS,max (L AS,eq(3) 31.0-54.5 db, 8 hours, background L AS,eq(3) 30 db) were equidistantly presented by loudspeakers. The combinations of frequency and L AS,max over the 9 noise nights were drawn in a random fashion. In each study night, always the same noise event with its characteristic L AS,max was presented to all 8 volunteers. The ANEs had been recorded at homes of residents living in the vicinity of Düsseldorf Airport. In the bedroom, the microphone was positioned near the pillow. The windows were closed or tilted. In order to guarantee realistic playback of ANEs, each sleep cabin was acoustically calibrated. A trigger signal was recorded simultaneously with the electrophysiological data allowing for an event correlated analysis with a resolution of 125 ms. In total, more than 33,000 ANEs were played back in the laboratory studies. 2.2 Field studies In the field studies, 64 volunteers aged 19-61 (mean age: 38) were investigated between September 2001 and November 2002 in the vicinity of Cologne Airport, which is one of Germany's airports with the highest number of nocturnal starts and landings. The ANEs occurring during the night were simultaneously recorded outside and inside the bedroom. In total, more than 15,000 ANEs were recorded. The same electrophysiological variables as in the laboratory were continuously sampled in the field. Again, the simultaneous recording of electrophysiological data and a trigger signal allowed for an event correlated analysis with a resolution of 125 ms. 2/8

2.3 Analysis Sleep stage classification Sleep stage classification [1] was performed visually. All data from one volunteer were analyzed by the same scorer. Prior to the analysis, the data files were renamed and the sequence of the nights was changed in a randomized fashion. In this way, the scorer was blinded, so that he or she did not know if and how many aircraft noise events were played back (laboratory) or recorded (field) during the particular night. In that way, the problem of inter-rater variability was eliminated for intra-individual analyzes as well. In both the laboratory and the field studies, a total of 1,922,194 30-second sleep epochs were visually scored. Comparison of sleep stage fractions The fractions of the different sleep stages were compared intra-individually between baseline and noise nights. The comparison was restricted to the time from sleep onset to the end of time in bed (TIB) of the shorter of both nights. The Wilcoxon signed rank test was used to test for differences in the fractions of sleep stages. A significant result was assumed at alpha = 0.05. Event correlated analysis An event correlated analysis directly connects the incidence of an ANE and the reaction of the sleeper to the ANE. This was made possible by the simultaneous recording of electrophysiological and acoustical data. Changes to sleep stage 1 or awakenings (both from now on simply called 'awakenings') were chosen as the dependent variable of multivariable regression analyzes. Since the dependent variable was dichotomous (awakenings occur (1) or do not occur (0)) logistic regression was used instead of linear regression. Data were not independent, because every subject provided more than one data point (ANEs) for the analysis. Hence, random effects logistic regression was used (EGRET software, version 2.0.31). Awakenings following an ANE are in that way unspecific, that awakenings are part of normal undisturbed sleep, i.e. they occur spontaneously as well. The probability (P) of noise induced awakenings (risk attributable to aircraft noise alone) can be calculated as: P(induced) = P(aircraft noise) P(spontaneous) (1) The time window after the onset of an ANE was chosen in a way maximizing the probability of noise induced awakenings P(induced). This time window turned out to be 60 seconds for the laboratory and 90 seconds for the field studies. In the field studies, every noise event occurring in the sleeping room (e.g. aircraft noise, snoring, car noise, etc.) was identified and flagged. In that way undisturbed ANEs could be differentiated from ANEs disturbed by other noise events. Only undisturbed ANEs were used for the analysis. 3 RESULTS 3.1 Comparison of sleep stage fractions For the laboratory, Figure 1 shows the differences of the time spent in the different sleep stages between all noise nights (n = 966) and the baseline nights (n = 112) during the course of the night irrespective of the number and maximum SPL of the ANEs played back during the night. 3/8

Cumulated Sleep Stage in Minutes (Difference Noise - Basieline) 5 4 3 2 1 0-1 -2-3 Awake&S1 (+2.3 min) S2 (+0.6 min) SWS (-4.1 min) REM (+1.3 min) 0 60 120 180 240 300 360 420-4 -5 Elapsed Sleep Time Since Sleep Onset in Minutes Figure 1: Differences in sleep stage distributions between noise and baseline nights depending on elapsed sleep time (for explanations see text) On average, 7 hours and 24.5 minutes after sleep onset were compared. Totals sleep time was non significantly reduced by only 1.8 minutes (p = 0.262). Nevertheless, changes in the structure of sleep were obvious. The amount of slow wave sleep was reduced by 4.1 minutes (p =0.057) especially in favor of the time spent awake or in sleep stage 1 (+2.3 minutes, p = 0.132). The amounts of SWS were further analyzed depending on number and maximum SPL of ANEs. In 21 of 30 different combinations a reduction in SWS amount compared to noise free baseline nights was seen (expected if aircraft noise did not influence SWS: 15). The differences varied between the extremes of +7.7 minutes at 4 x 55 db(a) and 18.0 minutes at 32 x 60 db(a). Even these extremes represented non significant changes after Bonferroni correction of alpha (0.0017). No significant differences in sleep stage amounts could be seen between noisy and less noisy nights (median split) during the field studies. 3.2 Event correlated analysis Laboratory studies The final analysis consisted of 25,408 ANEs and of 112 subjects. Figure 2 shows the results of a multivariable random effects logistic regression model with the statistically significant independent variables: maximum SPL of the ANE maximum SPL * maximum SPL time after sleep onset (TaSO) sleep stage prior to the start of the ANE (PSS) 4/8

In Figure 2, TaSO was chosen as the middle of the more sensitive second half of the night (601 epochs). PSS was chosen as the most sensitive sleep stage S2. 100% 90% 80% Percent Awakened 70% 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% 45 50 55 60 65 70 75 80 Maximum SPL L AS,max in db Figure 2: Random effects logistic regression model based on the data of the laboratory study (includes 95% confidence intervals, for explanations see text) Awakening probability rises with increasing maximum SPL in a non-linear fashion. The model predicts an awakening probability of 13.3% at 45 db(a) and 71.5% at 80 db(a) respectively. The precision can be read from the width of the 95% confidence intervals. Because of the large sample size the precision of the prediction is very high and varies between 3.2% at 49.5 db(a) and 7.5% at 80 db(a). Field studies We were able to use 10,658 ANEs of 61 subjects for the final analysis. Figure 3 shows the results of a multivariable random effects logistic regression model with the following statistically significant independent variables: maximum SPL of the ANE background noise level (L AS,eq ) one minute prior to the start of an ANE (BNL) maximum SPL * BNL (interaction) time after sleep onset (TaSO) sleep stage prior to the start of the ANE (PSS) For Figure 3, TaSO was chosen as the middle of the more sensitive second half of the night (601 epochs). PSS was chosen as the most sensitive sleep stage S2. Background noise level (BNL) was set to 27.1 db(a), which equals the median BNL found in the field. 5/8

100% 90% 80% Percent Awakened 70% 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70 75 Maximum SPL L AS,max in db at the sleeper's ear Figure 3: Random effects logistic regression model based on the data of the field study (includes 95% confidence intervals, for explanations see text) Like in the laboratory studies, awakening probability rises with increasing maximum SPL, but in a much less steeper way. The model predicts an awakening probability of 7.7% at 27.1 db(a) and 18.4% at 73.2 db(a) (laboratory: 54%), respectively. The differences between laboratory and field were observed over the whole range of maximum SPL values and rose with increasing SPL values. The probability of spontaneous awakenings was calculated with a separate random effects logistic regression model, based on so called virtual ANEs [2]. Using similar values for TaSO and PSS, a spontaneous awakening probability of 8.6% was estimated. This value is surpassed only by ANEs with maximum SPLs higher than 32.7 db(a), which means that a threshold value of 32.7 db(a) was found in the field. In the laboratory, up to values of 45 db(a) no threshold was found (spontaneous awakening probability in the laboratory = 6.3%). The precision can be read from the width of the 95% confidence intervals. Because of the large sample size the precision of the prediction is high and varies between 3.1% at 39 db(a) and 10.5% at 73.2 db(a). 4 DISCUSSION Total sleep time in the laboratory studies was only marginally influenced by aircraft noise. Possible reasons are adaptation mechanisms, leading to changes in sleep structure between two ANEs or the influence of an elevated sleep pressure induced by aircraft noise in preceding nights. Nonetheless, changes in sleep structure in form of reductions of SWS amounts were obvious, although not statistically significant. With actigraphy alone, it would not have been possible to observe these subtle changes in sleep structure. Dose-response relationships between maximum SPL of ANEs and awakening probability revealed striking differences between laboratory and field studies. Reactions were much less frequent in the 6/8

field than in the laboratory. These differences have already been reported as the result of a metaanalysis by Pearsons [3]. We were able to confirm these results in our study. Subjects sleeping in their familiar environment, including their own bed, is one possible reason for the differences observed in the laboratory and the field. Hume and Whitehead [4] showed in a study where subjects were exposed to ANEs via loudspeaker in the laboratory as well as in the field, that awakening probabilities at home were less pronounced than those observed in the laboratory. The different locations were the only reason for this difference, because the same ANEs were played back in both situations. The awakening probabilities at home were still higher than those reported by Pearsons [3] in the field, which may support the thesis that there is no general adaptation to aircraft noise, but rather an adaptation to the specific aircraft noise scenario experienced at home. 5 CONCLUSIONS The world's largest polysomnographic study on the effects of nocturnal aircraft noise on human sleep was carried out by the DLR-Institute of Aerospace Medicine between 1999 and 2003. 192 subjects and 2,240 subject nights provided extensive data for the development of precise doseresponse relationships between maximum SPL of ANEs and awakening probability. In connection with acoustical prognosis, these relationships will be used to precisely predict population groups who do not additionally wake up, exactly wake up one time, exactly wake up two times and so on by aircraft noise. This is shown exemplary for Frankfurt Airport in Figure 4. 5556 Frankfurt am Main 52 Offenbach 48 Erbenheim Kelsterbach Neu-Isenburg Heusenstamm Obertshausen 44 Eddersheim Zeppelinheim 40 36 Floersheim Raunheim Ruesselsheim Nauheim Walldorf Moerfelden Langen Egelsbach Dreieich 32 28 Worfelden Gross-Gerau Weiterstadt Buettelborn Darmstadt 24 20 5516 3448 52 56 60 64 68 72 76 80 84 88 3492 Figure 4: Combination of acoustical prognoses and the dose-response relationship between maximum SPL and awakening probability found in the field for the prognosis of areas in which more than 3, 2 or 1 awakenings induced by aircraft noise are expected to be observed at Frankfurt Airport (see text). 7/8

The green and yellow zones represent areas where an L AS,eq(3) of 50 db (green) and 55 db (blue) is expected to be exceeded. The red lines on the other hand represent areas outside of which less than 3 (inner area), less than 2 or less than 1 (outer area) additional awakenings induced by aircraft noise are expected. Clearly, qualitative differences of shape and size of the different areas can be seen. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We would like to thank Christian Piehler for the initiation of the study and for laying excellent prerequisites for the success of the experiment. Without the help of numerous assiduous colleagues of the Flight Physiology Department who sacrificed several weekends and tolerated countless sleepless nights this study would not have been possible. Many thanks to all of you. This project was partially financed by the Helmholtz Association of National Research Centres. REFERENCES [1] Rechtschaffen, A. et al.: A Manual of Standardized Terminology, Techniques and Scoring System for Sleep Stages of Human Subjects. (1968) Washington, D.C., Public Health Service, U.S. Government, Printing Office [2] Basner M. et al.: Aircraft Noise Effects Volume 1: Executive Summary (2004) DLR-FB-2004-07/E [3] Pearsons K et al.: Predicting noise-induced sleep disturbance. J.Acoust.Soc.Am. 1995;97:331-338 [4] Hume, K. and Whitehead, C. Sleep disturbance due to introduced aircraft noise. In: Proceedings of the 8th International Congress on Noise as a Public Health Problem. Editors: de Jong, R. G., Houtgast, T., Franssen, E. A. M., and Hofman, W., Schiedam, The Netherlands (2003): 199-200 8/8