Physiological measures of the precedence effect and spatial release from masking in the cat inferior colliculus.

Similar documents
Neural correlates of the perception of sound source separation

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

Sound localization psychophysics

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

Modeling Physiological and Psychophysical Responses to Precedence Effect Stimuli

Neural Correlates and Mechanisms of Spatial Release From Masking: Single-Unit and Population Responses in the Inferior Colliculus

21/01/2013. Binaural Phenomena. Aim. To understand binaural hearing Objectives. Understand the cues used to determine the location of a sound source

Neural Recording Methods

P. M. Zurek Sensimetrics Corporation, Somerville, Massachusetts and Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02139

HST.723J, Spring 2005 Theme 3 Report

B. G. Shinn-Cunningham Hearing Research Center, Departments of Biomedical Engineering and Cognitive and Neural Systems, Boston, Massachusetts 02215

Representation of sound in the auditory nerve

Binaural Hearing. Steve Colburn Boston University

Accurate Sound Localization in Reverberant Environments Is Mediated by Robust Encoding of Spatial Cues in the Auditory Midbrain

Publication VI. c 2007 Audio Engineering Society. Reprinted with permission.

The neural code for interaural time difference in human auditory cortex

William A. Yost and Sandra J. Guzman Parmly Hearing Institute, Loyola University Chicago, Chicago, Illinois 60201

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening

Binaurally-coherent jitter improves neural and perceptual ITD sensitivity in normal and electric hearing

The Auditory Nervous System

Juha Merimaa b) Institut für Kommunikationsakustik, Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Germany

Processing in The Superior Olivary Complex

Functional role of the human inferior colliculus in binaural hearing

The Coding of Spatial Location by Single Units in the Lateral Superior Olive of the Cat. I. Spatial Receptive Fields in Azimuth

INTRODUCTION. 475 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (1), January /98/103(1)/475/19/$ Acoustical Society of America 475

Spatial hearing and sound localization mechanisms in the brain. Henri Pöntynen February 9, 2016

Effect of spectral content and learning on auditory distance perception

Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive. Sasha Devore 1,2,3 and Bertrand Delgutte 1,2,3 1

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 THE DUPLEX-THEORY OF LOCALIZATION INVESTIGATED UNDER NATURAL CONDITIONS

Isolating mechanisms that influence measures of the precedence effect: Theoretical predictions and behavioral tests

The Structure and Function of the Auditory Nerve

On the influence of interaural differences on onset detection in auditory object formation. 1 Introduction

How is the stimulus represented in the nervous system?

Neural System Model of Human Sound Localization

Neural Responses in the Inferior Colliculus to Binaural Masking Level Differences Created by Inverting the Noise in One Ear

3-D SOUND IMAGE LOCALIZATION BY INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES AND THE MEDIAN PLANE HRTF. Masayuki Morimoto Motokuni Itoh Kazuhiro Iida

J Jeffress model, 3, 66ff

ABSTRACT. Figure 1. Stimulus configurations used in discrimination studies of the precedence effect.

The Central Auditory System

Influence of acoustic complexity on spatial release from masking and lateralization

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080

Hearing II Perceptual Aspects

I. INTRODUCTION. NL-5656 AA Eindhoven, The Netherlands. Electronic mail:

Lauer et al Olivocochlear efferents. Amanda M. Lauer, Ph.D. Dept. of Otolaryngology-HNS

Trading Directional Accuracy for Realism in a Virtual Auditory Display

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 102 (3), September /97/102(3)/1739/7/$ Acoustical Society of America 1739

An Auditory System Modeling in Sound Source Localization

Systems Neuroscience Oct. 16, Auditory system. http:

Spike-Timing Codes Enhance the Representation of Multiple Simultaneous Sound-Localization Cues in the Inferior Colliculus

Auditory Phase Opponency: A Temporal Model for Masked Detection at Low Frequencies

A NOVEL HEAD-RELATED TRANSFER FUNCTION MODEL BASED ON SPECTRAL AND INTERAURAL DIFFERENCE CUES

Sound Localization PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 31. Audition

I. INTRODUCTION. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113 (3), March /2003/113(3)/1631/15/$ Acoustical Society of America

Brian D. Simpson Veridian, 5200 Springfield Pike, Suite 200, Dayton, Ohio 45431

The use of interaural time and level difference cues by bilateral cochlear implant users

Linguistic Phonetics. Basic Audition. Diagram of the inner ear removed due to copyright restrictions.

Rhythm and Rate: Perception and Physiology HST November Jennifer Melcher

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Week 5. The peripheral auditory system: The ear as a signal processor

Hearing in the Environment

What Is the Difference between db HL and db SPL?

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Acoust Soc Am. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 April 29.

Localization in the Presence of a Distracter and Reverberation in the Frontal Horizontal Plane. I. Psychoacoustical Data

Binaural interference and auditory grouping

The development of a modified spectral ripple test

Auditory System & Hearing

Masker-signal relationships and sound level

Discrimination and identification of azimuth using spectral shape a)

Lateralized speech perception in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners and its relationship to temporal processing

Evidence for a neural source of the precedence effect in sound localization

3-D Sound and Spatial Audio. What do these terms mean?

Lab 4: Compartmental Model of Binaural Coincidence Detector Neurons

The Effects of Hearing Loss on Binaural Function

Binaural processing of complex stimuli

Spectral processing of two concurrent harmonic complexes

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization

Role of F0 differences in source segregation

Sound Localization in Multisource Environments: The Role of Stimulus Onset Asynchrony and Spatial Uncertainty

Information conveyed by inferior colliculus neurons about stimuli with aligned and misaligned sound localization cues

Pitfalls in behavioral estimates of basilar-membrane compression in humans a)

Linguistic Phonetics Fall 2005

Effect of mismatched place-of-stimulation on the salience of binaural cues in conditions that simulate bilateral cochlear-implant listening

Speech segregation in rooms: Effects of reverberation on both target and interferer

Effect of microphone position in hearing instruments on binaural masking level differences

Review Auditory Processing of Interaural Timing Information: New Insights

Frequency refers to how often something happens. Period refers to the time it takes something to happen.

The role of spatial cues for processing speech in noise

Phase Locking of Auditory-Nerve Fibers to the Envelopes of High-Frequency Sounds: Implications for Sound Localization

Convergent Input from Brainstem Coincidence Detectors onto Delay-Sensitive Neurons in the Inferior Colliculus

INTRODUCTION. 494 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (1), January /98/103(1)/494/13/$ Acoustical Society of America 494

Auditory nerve model for predicting performance limits of normal and impaired listeners

Chapter 40 Effects of Peripheral Tuning on the Auditory Nerve s Representation of Speech Envelope and Temporal Fine Structure Cues

9/29/14. Amanda M. Lauer, Dept. of Otolaryngology- HNS. From Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics, Green & Swets (1966)

Hearing. Juan P Bello

Spectrograms (revisited)

Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology. Psychoacoustics of hearing impairment

Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey

The masking of interaural delays

USING AUDITORY SALIENCY TO UNDERSTAND COMPLEX AUDITORY SCENES

Spatial processing in adults with hearing loss

Transcription:

Physiological measures of the precedence effect and spatial release from masking in the cat inferior colliculus. R.Y. Litovsky 1,3, C. C. Lane 1,2, C.. tencio 1 and. Delgutte 1,2 1 Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Eaton Peabody Laboratory 243 Charles St. oston M 2114, US 2 Harvard-MIT Speech and Hearing Sciences Program Cambridge, M, US 3 oston University, Hearing Research Center 44 Cummington St., oston M 2215, US litovsky@bu.edu 1. Introduction Human listeners localize and recognize auditory objects in complex acoustic environments. Little is known about the neural mechanisms underlying this ability, which is often degraded in the hearing impaired. We investigated responses of single-units in the anesthetized cat inferior colliculus (IC) for two stimulus situations that have some characteristics of complex environments and that depend on the location of the sound sources: (1) two brief stimuli that simulate a direct sound and a single reflection, and (2) a pair of simultaneous sounds, one of which may mask the other. The IC is an obvious target for these investigations because most IC neurons are sensitive to source direction and individual localization cues such as interaural time (ITD) and level (ILD) differences. (For a review, see Irvine, 1992). The first stimulus paradigm simulates the precedence effect (PE), which is the observation that two sounds occurring in rapid succession are perceived as a single auditory object localized near the leading sound (Litovsky et al., 1999). Single-unit studies in the IC (Yin, 1994; Fitzpatrick et al., 1995; Litovsky & Yin, 1998a, b) have identified a possible neural correlate of the PE in that the response to the lagging sound is suppressed for delays in which the PE occurs. key question is whether these effects are due to a general suppressive mechanism akin to forward masking, or whether the neural suppression is specifically directional. To address this question, we characterize the relationship between the directional neural responses to the leading sound and the lagging sound and how this relationship depends on individual localization cues. The second paradigm simulates a phenomenon called spatial release from masking (SRM), in which a signal is more easily detected when separated in space from a masker (e.g., Saberi et al., 199). Single-unit studies of the IC have found that the neural detectability of a tone in noise can be improved if the signal ITD differs from the masker s (Jiang et al., 1997). We extend these results to neural masking release in simulated free field, which depends on other localization cues (ILD and spectral features) besides ITD (ronkhurst and Plomp, 1988).

Response (Sp/Stim) 2. Methods Delay (ms) zimuth ( ) 1 6 36 12 4 1 C 8 6 - - -6-8 Similar procedures and stimulus paradigms were employed in the PE and SRM 1 1 Time (ms) 5 1 15 25 3 35 45 5 Time (ms). D 1..5 Lead Lead Lag Lag -9-45 45 9 Lead zimuth Fig. 1: Responses of an IC neuron to PE stimuli. : Lead and lag are positioned at 45 and delay is varied. : Stimulus configuration for measuring directionally dependent suppression. The lag is held constant at 45 where the response in absence of lead is robust, and the lead azimuth is varied. C: Temporal discharge patterns for lead response (left) as azimuth is varied and lag response (right) as a function of changes in lead azimuth. D: Summary of data shown in C; spike rates for lead and lag are plotted as a function of the lead azimuth. rrow denotes lag azimuth. studies. Methods for recording from single units in the IC of anesthetized cats were described by Delgutte et al. (1999). Free-field stimuli were simulated by filtering signals through head-related transfer functions (HRTFs) measured in one cat by Musicant et al. (199). ll stimuli contained energy from 3 Hz to 3 khz. PE stimuli consist of two delayed clicks, the lead and lag. For the SRM experiments, a 1-Hz click train or a -Hz chirp train was presented in continuous noise. Stimulus levels, when fixed, were chosen to produce a robust yet unsaturated response, usually 1- d above a unit's threshold. In both experiments, we were primarily concerned with how the azimuth of the lead/masker affects responses to a fixed lag/signal. 3. Results 3.1 Precedence Effect Responses to PE stimuli were initially measured for lead and lag stimuli both positioned at +45 contralateral to the recording site with delay varying from 1 to 1 ms. The temporal discharge patterns in Fig. 1 show strong responses to both stimuli at long delays and suppressed responses to the lagging stimulus at shorter delays. The halfmaximal delay, or delay at which the lag response recovers to 5% of the maximum, is 3 ms. Half-maximal delays range from 3 to 55 ms in our data and are highly consistent with those reported in the anesthetized cat for actual free-field stimulation (Yin, 1994; Litovsky and Yin, 1998a). To measure directional dependence of suppression, a delay slightly below the halfmaximal delay was chosen, the lagging stimulus held at 45 contralateral, and the lead azimuth varied from -9 to +9 in the frontal hemifield (Fig. 1). For the neuron in Fig. 1C-D, responses to the leading source vary with lead azimuth. If there were no suppression, the lag response would equal the response at +45 in the absence of a lead. The observed reduction in lag response indicates suppression resulting from the presence of the lead. Temporal discharge patterns for a 1-msec delay (Fig. 1C) show pronounced directionally dependent lag suppression; suppression is strongest for contralateral (positive) azimuths, where the lead evokes a response, and weaker for ipsilateral (negative) azimuths, where the lead evokes no response. plot of spike rates for both stimuli against lead azimuth (Fig. 1D) makes this relationship between lead excitation and lag suppression evident.

For the neuron in Fig. 1, both the lead and lag responses are strongly modulated with lead azimuth. However, within the population, there are several types of relationships between lead and lag responses. Fig. 2 shows examples of 4 response types: () Strong modulation of both lead excitation and lag suppression, and a strong predictability of the lag response from the lead based on a linear regression analysis. This unit type represents about half of the population and suggests a simple relationship between excitatory and inhibitory mechanisms. () Strong modulation of both lead and lag responses, but no predictability of the lag suppression from the lead excitation. bout one-third of neurons fall into this category. For this unit type, excitation and suppression do not have the same directional dependence. (C) Weak modulation in the lead response but strongly modulated lag suppression. (D) Strongly modulated lead excitation and weak or no Response (sp/stim) 2. 1.. 2. 1.5 1..5. (1 ms) C (2 ms) 171-17-6-9 -45 45 9 Lead zimuth 1..5. 187-31-7 182-15. 187-29-7 2. 1. ( ms) D (1 ms) -9-45 45 9 Lead zimuth Fig. 2: Modulation response types. Lag stimulus was positioned at +45 and rates are plotted as a function of the lead azimuth. Lead-lag delay is indicated in each panel. Four types of neurons are shown, which exhibit different relationships between lead excitation and lag suppression (see text). Each panel plots lead response ( ) and lag response (o). In and the solid line represents a prediction of lag responses from lead responses using linear regression. modulation in the lag suppression. bout one tenth of neurons fall into each of C and D. These responses indicate a de-coupling between the directional mechanisms mediating excitation and suppression. Our interest lies in identifying which localization cues determine the directional patterns of lead excitation and lag suppression. We used the approach of Delgutte et al. (1995), where stimuli were digitally manipulated so that some cues were varied with azimuth as in free field, while others were held constant. Fig. 3-C shows results for a high-cf neuron (6.2 khz). In response to the normal stimulus ( full cue, ), the unit s lead and lag responses are strongly modulated, and the lag response can be predicted from the lead response, the response type shown in Fig. 2. When the lead ILD is held constant (), the unit loses modulation of both the lead and lag responses, while when only lead ILD is varied (C), the responses resemble those seen with the full-cue stimuli. For this unit, then, ILD is a potent cue for both excitatory and suppressive responses. Fig. 3D-F shows results for a lower-cf (1.9 khz) unit where responses showed a dependence on multiple cues. The full-cue response in D again shows modulations in both the lead and lag responses. If only the lead ITD is varied (E), both lead and lag responses maintain their strong modulations with azimuth. Holding ITD constant (F) renders the lead response unmodulated, but leaves the lag suppression modulated to some degree. This result points to a dissociation between the mechanisms that mediate excitation in response to the lead and those mediating suppression of the lag response. While excitation depends primarily on ITD, suppression is mediated by a combination of ITD and other cues. Similar manipulations of directional cues were conducted in 33 neurons. For one-third of the neurons, the most potent cues for lead excitation and lag suppression were distinct. For these neurons, the lead excitation and the lag suppression may be pro-

duced by inputs from different subcollicular neural populations that would be sensitive to different directional cues. imulus Spikes / St 3.2 Spatial Release from Masking We measured masked threshold as a function of noise azimuth for 19 neurons with CFs ranging from 1 to khz. We studied neurons that responded to either a click train or a chirp train and whose responses were masked by intense noise. Most units met these criteria. The signal was held at a particular azimuth (usually in front at ), while noise azimuth was varied from -9 to +9 in the frontal hemifield. For each neuron, the signal level was fixed, and the noise level adjusted to determine masked threshold. The signal was ms in duration and delivered with a repetition rate of 2.5 Hz. Noise Level (d SPL) Rate (spikes/sec) L s 1.5 C 1..5. 1..5. 25 15 1 5 5 3 1 Lead Lag ILD unit 1.5 Lead Full Cue 1..5. Lead ILD C 1.5 Lead ILD θ N = -9 θ N = 5 1 3 Time (msec) S+N Lead zimuth % C 1..8.6.4.2 N. 1 3 5 6 Noise Level (d SPL) CF 6.2 khz Lag 45 (Full Cue) 2 msec Lag - Full Cue -9-6 -3 3 6 9 3 1 1 3 D Time (msec) 25 15 1 5 1..5. 1..5. 1..5. D E F S+N Lead Full Cue Lead ITD Lead ITD Mixed unit Lead zimuth % C.2 N. 1 3 5 6 Noise Level (d SPL) Fig. 4: For this unit (CF = 6 khz), an 8-d SPL chirp train (- ms) was held at with continuous noise at -9 (,C) or (,D)., : Dot rasters for increasing noise levels in 5 d steps with stimulus presentations for each level. C, D: Rate for the signal and noise (S + N, ), rate for the noise alone (N, ), and percent of stimulus presentations for which the number of spikes in S + N is greater than in N (%C, ). The smooth line is a fit cumulative Gaussian function. Masked threshold is the noise level where the fitted curve crosses 75% (dash-dot line). Masked threshold, L n Th, was defined in a manner similar to the psychophysical two-interval, two-alternative forced-choice paradigm where threshold is the level for which the subject gives the correct response 75% of the time. Here masked threshold was the noise level where the number of spikes in the signal-and-noise interval (- ms) is greater than in the noise-only interval (- ms) for 75% of the stimulus presentations (Fig. 4). In Fig. 4, there is a masking release of 21 d when the noise is moved from (L n Th = 17 d) to -9 (L n Th = 38 d). 1..8.6.4 CF 1.9 khz Lag 45 (Full Cue) msec -9-6 -3 3 6 9 % Correct Lag - Full Cue Fig. 3: Responses of lead ( ) and lag (o) plotted as a function of leading source azimuth. Lag stimulus was always at +45 and contained all directional cues. The lead was varied from ±9. For one neuron (CF 6.2 khz, left) lead stimulus either contained all cues (), or had fixed ILD () or only ILD varied (C). For a second neuron (CF 1.9 khz, right) lead was either full cue (D), or only ITD varied (E) or had ITD fixed (F).

Figs. 5-C show three neurons masked thresholds as a function of noise azimuth. The most effective masking azimuths, or the azimuths for which the lowest-level masker could mask the signal, are 18, 54, and 54 for these neurons, respectively. For the neurons studied, the most effective masking azimuths ranged from 54 to +54. Most (17/19) neurons had most effective masking azimuths contralateral to the recording site, the side that usually excites IC neurons. The distribution of most effective masking azimuths has a broad maximum around 36, roughly corresponding to the acoustic pinna axis (Phillips, 1982). The maximum release from masking is the difference in masked threshold for the least effective and most effective masking azimuths. In Fig. 5, these thresholds are 16 and 38 d, respectively; therefore, the release from masking is 22 d. The release from masking for all the neurons studied ranged from 3 to 44 d, with a mean of 17 d. These values are comparable to the 15-18 d range seen psychophysically for broadband stimuli (Gilkey and Good, 1995; Saberi et al., 1991). L n Th (d SPL) L n Th (d SPL) 4 17 3 1 9 54 18 18 54 9 Noise zimuth ( ) 5 2 3 1 9 54 18 18 54 9 Noise zimuth ( ) C D 6 2 3 1 Most Effective Masking zimuth ( ) 9 54 18 18 54 9 Noise zimuth ( ) 9 45 45 9 9 45 45 9 Most Sensitive zimuth ( ) Fig. 5: -C: Thick lines show masked threshold vs. noise azimuth for the signal at. Signal level is indicated by the bold circle. : Thin line shows masked threshold for signal at 45 contralateral (thin circle). C: Dashed lines show threshold in quiet for noise. The threshold in quiet is the noise level that elicits one spike every other stimulus presentation. D: Most effective masking azimuth vs. most sensitive masking azimuth for all the neurons that could be excited by continuous noise. ecause psychophysics shows that a masker is most effective when it is co-located with a signal, we measured masked thresholds for different signal azimuths in some neurons. For the neuron in Fig. 5, changing the signal azimuth from (thick line) to 45 (thin line) shifts the threshold curve vertically without changing its shape. Therefore, masking in this neuron did not depend on the separation between the signal and the masker, as seen in psychophysics; such dependence might be found by considering a population of neurons (see Discussion). s discussed above, suppression studied with the PE paradigm could be predicted from the lead excitation in many neurons. Masking could also be predicted from the excitation produced by the noise masker for some neurons. For example, Fig. 5C shows both a neuron s masked threshold for the signal at and the noise threshold in quiet as a function of noise azimuth. For this neuron, the threshold in quiet parallels the masked threshold curve. The most sensitive azimuth (the azimuth where threshold in quiet is lowest) coincides with the most effective masking azimuth at 54. Fig. 5D shows the most sensitive azimuth against the most effective masking azimuth for all the neurons that were excited by continuous noise (9/19). For this group, the most sensitive noise azimuth is strongly correlated with the most effective masking azimuth (ρ =.95, p <.1), indicating that the excitation pattern is related to the masking pattern.

The continuous noise masker did not excite the other half of the neurons. For these neurons, the response to the signal was masked through a suppressive mechanism, possibly synaptic inhibition. We also observed suppression of the signal response by the noise for some of the neurons that were excited by the noise (e.g. Fig. 4D). Therefore, the correlation between excitation and masking may not indicate that excitation is responsible for the masking per se, but instead that the excitation and suppression have similar directionality, not unlike the Group neurons in the PE experiments (Fig. 2). In the simplest case, masking patterns might be explained by the effective signalto-noise ratio in one ear. To test this idea, we created a model of the auditory periphery consisting of a Gamma-tone filter centered at the CF of each unit followed by an envelope detector. We passed the noise through the HRTF for one ear (the one that most excited the unit studied) and then through the model. The noise level was adjusted to obtain a certain energy level at the model output, and this energy level was chosen to best fit the data. The resulting noise level was the model masked threshold. Fig. 6 shows masked threshold curves for two neurons and the model. The model predicts the masked threshold curve for the neuron in Fig. 6, but greatly underestimates the change in masked threshold for the neuron in Fig. 6. Overall, we were able to predict about one-third of the neurons directional masking patterns with this simple monaural model. Presumably, binaural and temporal processing must be considered to predict masking patterns in the other two-thirds of the neurons. L n Th (d SPL) 5 3 1 6 2 5 9 54 18 18 54 9 9 54 18 18 54 9 Noise zimuth ( ) Noise zimuth ( ) L n Th (d SPL) 3 1 Masked Threshold Model Prediction 5 2 Fig. 6: Masked thresholds for two units with model predictions. The masked threshold in is predicted by the energy in the contralateral ear, but the masked threshold in is not predicted by this model. 4. Discussion and Conclusion We found that for a vast majority of IC neurons, lag suppression and masking vary with lead or masker azimuth, and this directionality depends in part on binaural interactions. Inhibitory mechanisms are likely to play an important role in these effects. There are further similarities between the PE results and the masking results if we make analogies between the lead and the masker on the one hand, and the lag and the signal on the other hand. For some neurons, masking and suppression appear to be fairly simple. For about half the neurons studied, suppression of the lag response was linearly predictable from the response to the lead (Fig. 2, 3). Similarly, directional masking patterns resembled noise thresholds in quiet for neurons that responded to the continuous masker (Fig. 5C,D). For these neurons, there is a close relationship between a sound s ability to excite and its directional pattern of masking or suppression. ecause the directional pattern of excitation depends only on the masker azimuth, this result suggests that masking does not depend on the interaction between the signal and the masker, but on each of the two sounds independently. This prediction was verified in a few neurons in which masking patterns were measured for different signal azimuths (e.g., Fig. 5), and similar results were observed with PE stimuli when the lag azimuth was varied (not shown). This separability of the effects of the signal and masker contrasts with psychophysical data, where masking is strongest when the signal and the masker occupy the

same location (Gilkey and Good, 1995; Saberi et al., 1991). Similarly, several aspects of the PE are stronger when the two stimuli are close together (Litovsky and Shinn- Cunningham, ). For the simple neurons showing these effects, neural correlates of PE and SRM should be sought in the response of a population of neurons that respond preferentially to different spatial locations. minimum requirement for such a population code is that the entire range of azimuths must be represented in the population. The wide range of best azimuths in Fig. 5D (when combined with responses from the opposite IC) suggests that a population code may be viable in the IC. We also found neurons showing more complex forms of masking and echo suppression. For about half the neurons tested, the directional pattern of lag suppression could not be predicted from the response to the lead (Fig. 2-D). Further decoupling between lead excitation and lag suppression was revealed by manipulating localization cues (Fig 3D-F). Distinct directional patterns of lead excitation and lag suppression might be produced by excitatory and inhibitory inputs from different subcollicular nuclei onto IC cells. The observation of multiple phases of excitation and inhibition in intracellular recordings from the IC (Kuwada et al., 1998) is consistent with this hypothesis. Regardless of the underlying mechanisms, these complex directional patterns show that neural suppression is not simply a form of forward masking and may provide a directional component for the PE. Neurons of this type were less common in the masking study, but half the neurons in our small sample did not respond to continuous noise so that the relationship between excitation and masking was not tested. In summary, we have begun examining the neural mechanisms underlying the PE and SRM, two psychophysical phenomena likely to play a role when listening in complex acoustic environments. oth of these perceptual phenomena are known to depend on the relative directions of the first and second stimuli. Here we have shown that IC neurons show directional patterns of masking and suppression that correlate with these phenomena. However, a full understanding will require quantitative models for masking and suppression in populations of neurons. 5. References ronkhorst,.w., and Plomp, R. (1988). The effect of head-induced interaural time and level differences on speech intelligibility in noise. J. coust. Soc. m., 83, 158-1516. Delgutte,., Joris, P., Litovsky, R. and Yin, T.C.T. (1995). Relative importance of different acoustic cues to the directional sensitivity of inferior-colliculus neurons. In: G.. Manley, G.M. Klump, C. Koppl, H. Fastl, and H. Oeckinghaus (Eds.), dvances in Hearing Research. World Scientific, London. Delgutte,., Joris, P.X., Litovsky, R.Y., and Yin, T.C.T. (1999). Receptive fields and binaural interactions for virtual-space stimuli in the cat inferior colliculus. J. Neurophys. 81, 2833-2851. Fitzpatrick, D.C., Kuwada, S., atra, R., and Trahiotis, C. (1995). Neural responses to simple, simulated echoes in the auditory brainstem of the unanesthetized rabbit. J. Neurophys., 74, 2469-2486. Gilkey, R.H. and Good, M.D. (1995) Effects of frequency on free-field masking. Hum. Factors 37, 835-853. Irvine, D.R.F. (1992). Physiology of the uditory rainstem. In:.N. Popper and R.R. Fay (Eds.), The Mammalian uditory Pathway: Neurophysiology. Springer-Verlag, New York. Jiang, D., Mclpine, D., and Palmer,.R. (1997). Responses of neurons in the inferior colliculus to binaural masking level difference stimuli measured by rate-level functions. J. Neurophys. 77, 385-316. Litovsky, R.Y. and Yin, T.C.T. (1998a). Physiological studies of the precedence effect in the inferior colliculus of the cat: I. Correlates of psychophysics. J. Neurophys. 8, 132-1316. Litovsky, R.Y. and Yin, T.C.T. (1998b). Physiological studies of the precedence effect in the inferior colliculus of the cat: II. Neural Mechanisms. J. Neurophys. 8, 1285-131. Litovsky, R.Y., Colburn, H.S., Yost, W.., and Guzman, S. (1999). The precedence effect. J. coust. Soc. m. 16, 1633-1654.

Litovsky, R.Y. and Shinn-Cunningham,.G. (, in press). Investigation of the relationship between three common measures of precedence: fusion, localization dominance and discrimination suppression. J. coust. Soc. m. Musicant,.D., Chan, J.C.K., and Hind, J.E. (199). Direction-dependent spectral properties of cat external ear: New data and cross-species comparisons. J. coust. Soc. m. 87, 757-781. Phillips, D.P., Calford, M.., Pettigrew, J.D., itkin, L.M., and Semple, M.N. (1982). Directionality of sound pressure transformation at the cat s pinna. Hearing Res. 8, 13-28. Saberi, K., Dostal, L., Sadralodabai, T., ull, V., and Perrot, D.R. (1991). Free-field release from masking. J.coust. Soc. m. 9, 1355-137. Yin, T.C.T. (1994). Physiological correlates of the precedence effect and summing localization in the inferior colliculus of the cat. J. Neuroscience, 14, 517-5186. Research supported by Grant DC119 from the NIH-NIDCD.