Lokaverkefni til BS prófs í sálfræði. Separating the effects of selection and non-selection on subsequent PoP trials

Similar documents
ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research

Priming in visual search: Separating the effects of target repetition, distractor repetition and role-reversal

The role of priming. in conjunctive visual search

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

(This is a sample cover image for this issue. The actual cover is not yet available at this time.)

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set

Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search

Priming of Pop-out provides reliable measures of target activation and distractor inhibition in selective attention q

Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Rejecting salient distractors: Generalization from experience

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group?

Short-term and long-term attentional biases to frequently encountered target features

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis

Bottom-Up Guidance in Visual Search for Conjunctions

UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund

Piloting the use of the Modified Attention Network Task in Children

Cross-trial priming in visual search for singleton conjunction targets: Role of repeated target and distractor features

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

The impact of item clustering on visual search: It all depends on the nature of the visual search

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load

Cross-Trial Priming of Element Positions in Visual Pop-Out Search Is Dependent on Stimulus Arrangement

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features

Effect of Attentional Bias in Discrimination of Negative, Positive and Neutral Facial Expressions in Foraging Tasks.

Optimal exploration strategies in haptic search

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY

The synergy of top-down and bottom-up attention in complex task: going beyond saliency models.

Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use

CONNERS K-CPT 2. Conners Kiddie Continuous Performance Test 2 nd Edition C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

Ben Gurion University

Feature Integration Theory Revisited: Dissociating Feature Detection and Attentional Guidance in Visual Search

Aging, Emotion, Attention, and Binding in the Taboo Stroop Task: Data and Theories

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Intentional versus unintentional use of contingencies between perceptual events

IMPRINT models of training: Update on RADAR modeling

The Effects of Action on Perception. Andriana Tesoro. California State University, Long Beach

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them

VISUAL PERCEPTION OF STRUCTURED SYMBOLS

A Return to the Gorilla What Effects What

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance

Distinct, but top-down modulable color and positional priming mechanisms in visual pop-out search

Taming the White Bear: Initial Costs and Eventual Benefits of Distractor Inhibition

SENSATION AND PERCEPTION KEY TERMS

The path of visual attention

Motion onset really does capture attention

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Article in press, Journal of Vision. December Rapid and selective updating of the target template in visual search

(SAT). d) inhibiting automatized responses.

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention

Efficient Visual Search without Top-down or Bottom-up Guidance: A Putative Role for Perceptual Organization

Enhanced discrimination in autism

(In)Attention and Visual Awareness IAT814

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces

Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks

Vision Research. Task-irrelevant stimulus salience affects visual search q. Dominique Lamy *, Loren Zoaris. abstract

PSYC20007 READINGS AND NOTES

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the processes underlying the object advantage

Interaction Between Social Categories in the Composite Face Paradigm. Wenfeng Chen and Naixin Ren. Chinese Academy of Sciences. Andrew W.

Early, Involuntary Top-Down Guidance of Attention From Working Memory

Feature Integration Theory

From concept to percept: Priming the perception of ambiguous figures

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients

Effects of Task Relevance and Stimulus-Driven Salience in Feature-Search Mode

Awareness in contextual cuing with extended and concurrent explicit tests

AVATARS AND VASES: THE AUTOMATIC PROCESSING OF WHAT OTHER PEOPLE SEE 1

Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm

Processing Emergent Features in Metaphor Comprehension

Researchers have investigated visual search behaviour for almost a century. During that

Appendix: Instructions for Treatment Index B (Human Opponents, With Recommendations)

Target contact and exploration strategies in haptic search

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

CHAPTER 6: Memory model Practice questions at - text book pages 112 to 113

The observation that the mere activation

Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift of Visual Attention Induced by Averted Gaze

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Evidence for distinct mechanisms underlying attentional priming and sensory memory for bistable perception

RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions

Stressing the mind: The effect of cognitive load and articulatory suppression on attentional guidance from working memory

Transcription:

Lokaverkefni til BS prófs í sálfræði Choice induced priming Separating the effects of selection and non-selection on subsequent PoP trials Elva Bergþóra Brjánsdóttir Sverrir Rolf Sander Júní 2015 HÁSKÓLI ÍSLANDS HEILBRIGÐISVÍSINDASVIÐ SÁLFRÆÐIDEILD

Choice induced priming Separating the effects of selection and non-selection on subsequent PoP trials Elva Bergþóra Brjánsdóttir Sverrir Rolf Sander Lokaverkefni til BS-gráðu í sálfræði Leiðbeinandi: Árni Kristjánsson Sálfræðideild Heilbrigðisvísindasvið Háskóla Íslands Júní 2015

Ritgerð þessi er lokaverkefni til BS gráðu í sálfræði og er óheimilt að afrita ritgerðina á nokkurn hátt nema með leyfi rétthafa. Elva Bergþóra Brjánsdóttir og Sverrir Rolf Sander, 2015 Prentun: Svansprent ehf. Reykjavík, Ísland 2015

Objective: Traditional priming-of-popout (PoP) experiments involve an observer whose goal is to specify an oddball stimulus (target) among irrelevant items (distractors). Research has shown that repeated PoP trials result in reduced response times (RTs). Recent studies added free choice trials intermixed with traditional PoP. Results of these studies show that preceded items are favoured over those previously ignored. Separation of the effects of the target and distractors emphasize these findings. Moreover, selection seems to affect subsequent PoP RTs. There, the separate roles of selected (target) and non-selected (distractor) items have not been investigated. This was the purpose of our study. Method: Nine students participated in the experiment. We intermixed six variations of 600 PoP trials where one target was presented among two distractors with three variations of 1600 free choice trials with two dissimilar targets. A neutral stimulus was introduced to separate the effects of the target and distractors. Results: Conditions where the selected stimulus became a neutral item on the following trial yielded the fastest RTs. Also, repetition of distractor items reduced RTs further. Conclusion: Neutral items have an attention grabbing effect which either facilitates or inhibits recognition. Furthermore, results highlight the importance of distractor repetition in priming.

Preface This BSc thesis was written under Dr. Árni Kristjánsson and we would like to thank him for his patience and understanding. Our experimental paradigm was designed by our colleague Jóhann Pálmar Harðarson. We would like to thank him for lots of great inspiration, ideas, comments and an endless stream of positive vibes. His selfless contribution of personal time and wisdom is invaluable to us. Writing a thesis takes long hours, hard work and dedication but without the two mentioned above this paper never would have become a reality. Thank you.

Table of Contents Abstract... 2 Preface... 3 Introduction... 5 This experiment... 8 Methods... 8 Apparatus and stimuli... 8 Observers... 9 Results and discussion... 10 Reaction times on target vs. distractor vs. neutral conditions... 10 Expanding the conditions in relation to color variations... 11 Comparing the six variations to separate the effects of selected versus non-selected items on subsequent PoP trials... 13 General discussion... 15 References... 18 4

In everyday life, our attention is inevitably directed to a certain something. This happens whether we are completely aware of it or not. With repeated exposure to a stimulus, we find it easier to identify that specific item and distinguish it from an array of different stimuli (Kristjánsson & Campana, 2010). This can be of great benefit in everyday functioning. Imagine playing a casual ball game divided into two teams. Whilst thinking of where to pass the ball, a teammate appears in your visual field. He immediately catches your attention, even though he is surrounded by members of the opposing team. Once the game is underway you will be better at differentiating between your teammates and the opponents, which is vital if the goal of the game is to keep the ball within the team. In recent years a similar phenomenon has gained attention in the field of psychophysics and has become known as priming of attention shifts (Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1994; Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1996). The concept of priming in perception has been defined as the unconscious effects that a recent stimulus has on altering current perception (Kristjánsson & Campana, 2010). In visual perception, priming research is particularly concerned with repeated exposure to an oddball stimulus presented among a multitude of irrelevant stimuli, such as the ball game in the example mentioned above. This has become known as priming of pop-out (PoP) (Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1994; Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1996). The priming paradigm generally features one target stimulus presented repetitively among a group of distractors where the role of the observer is, for example, to specify if the target is present on each trial (Kristjánsson & Driver, 2008; Kristjánsson, Wang & Nakayama, 2002). Research has shown that with repeated exposure of the target on successive trials, it becomes more distinctive (Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1994; Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1996). Moreover, the observer manages to suppress the distractors more effectively, independent of whether a target was present or absent (Kristjánsson & Driver, 2008). In combination, this results in more rapid recognition and better accuracy on repeated versus novel trials (Kristjánsson & Driver, 2008). The process of PoP is generally thought of as automatic, implicit and short-term (Maljkovic & Nakayama, 2000). The effects of PoP have been shown to increase gradually with repeated trials when the features of the target persist through runs of trials, resulting in reduced response times (RTs). This indicates that exposure to a certain target enhances its priming effect progressively (Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1994; Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1996). Likewise, Kristjánsson & Driver (2008) demonstrated the importance of role reversal where the target 5

and distractors switch roles between trials. Observers faced with such conditions displayed a particularly increased reaction time compared to other conditions. These findings highlight the fact that both the target and distractors have separate effects on attentional acuity in visual search tasks. Earlier research focused almost exclusively on numerous PoP trials and their cumulative effect in relation to RTs. Brascamp, Blake & Kristjánsson (2011) designed an experimental paradigm where they added free choice trials to the traditional PoP visual search task. This enabled them to investigate the effect of voluntary selection with regards to conventional fixed choice trials where the observer was obstructed to specify a predesignated target. Their results showed that observers tended to select the color they had recently attended to and that this selection bias progressed in relation to the number of PoP trials preceding the free choice ones. In this way, Brascamp et al. (2011) established that PoP not only influences reaction time but also results in more homogeneous selection on the subsequent free choice trial. Thus, they established that recent past has an impact on present preference. Furthermore, free choice tends to induce conventional PoP RTs, suggesting that these trials are in fact two manifestations of the same cognitive operations. However, the design of Brascamp et al. (2011) did not allow them to separate the influence that selection and non-selection on traditional PoP trials have on choice trials. Harðarson (2014) introduced a new implementation based on the research of Brascamp et al. (2011). In order to separate these effects, Harðarson (2014) added a third type of stimulus whose role was to act as a neutral item in addition to the traditional combination of a target and distractors. The study revealed that the previous target stimulus was generally preferred to both neutral and distractor items on free choice trials. Moreover, neutral items were favoured over distractor items. This effect was cumulative with additional PoP trials, thus emphasizing the role that priming plays in selection. The purpose of this study was to expand on the findings of Harðarson (2014) and Brascamp et al. (2011). Instead of focusing on the effect that traditional PoP trials have on the subsequent free choice trial, we investigated the role of voluntary selection on RTs in priming-of-popout. This modification was obtained by using the experimental method designed by Harðarson (2014), while reversing their ratio of PoP versus free choice trials. In this way, we were able to separate the effects of freely selected and non-selected items. In addition, we wanted to explore possible effects that runs of the same color being selected have 6

on RTs, whereas former studies have suggested that repetition of target and distractor on PoP trials is beneficial (Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1994; Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1996; Kristjánsson & Driver, 2008). 7

Experiment: How selection and non-selection influences RTs on traditional PoP Methods The experiment consisted of two types of visual task trials. In both trials, a set of either two or three diamonds (red, green and blue) were presented to observers. Each diamond had one of their four corners (top, right, bottom and left) randomly cut off. The cut varied between the diamonds on each trial, but was otherwise randomly determined. This design enabled the observer to select a distinctive cut-off point and thereby specify which color of diamond was selected. To indicate their selection, observers pressed one of four different keyboard keys by using their dominant hand. The first type contained free choice trials with no predesignated target (left part of Figure 1). Two diamonds of different colors were presented on screen and the role of the observer was to choose the one they preferred. Throughout the experiment the terms selected and non-selected were used interchangeably with target and distractor when referring to the free choice trials, since the selected diamond was believed to function as a target and vice versa. The second type of trial was a traditional PoP (right part of Figure 1). Three diamonds were presented, a single predesignated target alongside two distractor items that shared a single color. Here, the observer had to select the diamond that was of distinct color. These two types of trials were intermixed so that a single PoP trial always followed a free choice run. The length of the free choice runs were random and could last 1, 2, 3 or 4 trials. Therefore, there were more free choice trials per session compared to PoP trials, with approximately 1600 free choice trials and 600 PoP trials (circa 2200 trials per observer). On each trial, only two colors out of the three were presented. PoP trials consisted of six possible color variations (e.g. a red diamond among two blue diamonds), and choice trials of three variations (e.g. a green diamond and a red diamond). In this way, we were able to separate the effects of target selection (selecting the same color on a run of free choice trials) and distractor (not selecting the same color on a run of free choice trials) on subsequent RTs on traditional PoP trials. Variations of both types of trials were randomly balanced so that each would appear approximately the same number of times for each observer. Apparatus and stimuli A 17 inch CRT screen (85 hz refresh rate) was used to present the stimuli. They consisted of two or three diamonds (2 deg diameter) that could be green, blue or red, appearing on a black 8

background (Figure 1). Stimuli turned up on an imaginary ring (8 deg radius) in random positions around a white fixation point and were equally spaced. On the PoP trials, the three diamonds were always spaced 120 degrees apart and on the choice trials the two diamonds were 180 degrees apart. The stimuli were present until the observer pressed one of the four keyboard keys possible on both types of trial. In between each trial there was a random delay ranging from 1.5 to 2 seconds until a new trial started. Observers Eleven observers participated in the experiment, unaware of its purpose. Two of them did not follow the instructions given so their results were excluded from the experiment. Thus, of the total eleven, nine observers (5 women) ranging between 21 and 28 years (M = 24 years) in age were included in the data. All of them had previously participated in a psychophysical experiment and reported normal or corrected to normal vision and no color vision deficits. Observers were asked to submit their answers as quickly as possible without sacrificing accuracy. Participation in the experiment was part of the curriculum in a course at the faculty of Psychology in the University of Iceland. Figure 1. (Left) One of three possible choice trials where observers were told to report the missing corner of a diamond of their choice. (Right) One of six possible variations on pop-out trials. Observers were instructed to report the missing corner of the odd-colored diamond. Items are not drawn to scale 9

Results and discussion Reaction times on target vs. distractor vs. neutral conditions To give a better overview of the results, we begin by comparing the three possible role changes of the target between a free choice trial and the subsequent PoP trial. They focus on selection, non-selection will be explored further in the next section. For this analysis RTs faster than 100 ms and three standard deviations above the mean were excluded from the experiment. In addition, error responses on free choice (0,01%) and PoP (0,7%) trials were excluded as well. In the first condition, the preferred diamond color on a free choice trial became the designated target on the following PoP trial ( target condition). In the second condition, the color of the target became a distractor on the subsequent PoP trial. Thus, the predesignated target held the color of what was not chosen on the previous trial ( distractor condition). Lastly, the color that was not presented on the free choice trial became the predesignated target on the subsequent PoP trial, such as when faced with the option of either red or blue, the following PoP trial had a predesignated target of the color green ( neutral condition). When the three conditions were compared, a difference in RTs was revealed (Figure 2; repeated-measures ANOVA with mean RTs of the conditions as the within-subject factor: F(2,16) = 13.267 p <.001). Further comparison between the conditions indicated that RTs were faster when the neutral condition (M = 846 ms) was compared to the target condition (M = 871 ms; Figure 2; Fisher LSD post-hoc tests with mean RTs of the neutral condition against the target condition: p <.05), or the distractor condition (M = 882 ms; Figure 2; Fisher LSD post-hoc tests with mean RTs of the neutral condition against the distractor condition: p <.01). This suggests that voluntary selection did in fact have an effect on RTs on a subsequent PoP trial. Here, the most crucial finding is that the neutral condition yielded the fastest RTs. Moreover, this difference was true for every observer in the experiment. 10

950 Reaction time (ms) 900 850 800 Target Distractor Neutral Condition Figure 2. The three possible roles that the target could hold on a subsequent PoP trial. The reaction times are displayed in milliseconds. The neutral condition yielded the fastest RTs, followed by the target condition. Error bars represent the standard error of the sample mean Expanding the conditions in relation to color variations Each defined condition is generated from two color variations, resulting in six conditions overall (see Table 1). The color variations specify possible changes of the roles of selected and non-selected items between a free choice trial and its subsequent PoP trial. We defined the six color variations in the following manner. For the target condition, selected color maintained its color between a free choice trial and the following PoP trial, but the non-selected color on the subsequent PoP trial could either persist as a distractor (color variation one ) or become a neutral item (color variation two ). For the distractor condition, the previously non-selected color always became the predesignated target on the following PoP trial. However, the PoP distractor could either hold the color of the selected item in the previous trial (color variation three ) or be a neutral color relative to the preceding trial (color variation four ). For the neutral condition, the target converted into a neutral item between trials, while distractors either obtained the color of the target from the previous trial (color variation five ), or did not change at all (color variation six ). These six color variations were compared both in relation to RTs and possible runs of the selected target prior to the single PoP trial. 11

Table 1. The conditions were expanded in relation of the possible role change of both the target and distractors between the two types of trials. Each condition was split into two possible color variations Condition Variation Change between trials Target condition Distractor condition Neutral condition Color variation 1 Target persists Distractor persists Color variation 2 Target persists Distractor becomes neutral Color variation 3 Target becomes distractor Distractor becomes target Color variation 4 Target becomes distractor Distractor becomes neutral Color variation 5 Target becomes neutral Distractor becomes target Color variation 6 Target becomes neutral Distractor persists When comparing the six color variations it was evident that a difference in relation to RTs was apparent (Figure 3; two-factor ANOVA with the mean RTs of the six color variations as within-subject factor: F(1.84,14.73) = 9.185, p <.005). In addition, when the same item was chosen repeatedly on free choice trials, RTs on the following PoP trial were generally faster (Figure 3; two-factor ANOVA with repeat or non-repeat as within-subject factor: F(1,8) = 6.667, p <.05). However, the interaction between runs and RTs for the color variations was just short of significance (Figure 3; two-factor ANOVA with within subject factors as the mean RTs of the six color variations and repeat or non-repeat: F(5,40) = 2.251, p =.068). 915 Reaction time (ms) 890 865 840 815 One Two Three Four Five Six Color variation Figure 3. The six color variations that were used to separate the effects of selected versus non-selected items on a subsequent PoP trial. The reaction times are displayed in milliseconds. The neutral color variations yielded the fastest response times. Conversely, the distractor variations produced the slowest RTs. Error bars represent the standard error of the sample mean 12

Comparing the six variations to separate the effects of selected versus non-selected items on subsequent PoP trials This section outlines the separate roles of the previous target and distractors from choice trials on RTs in traditional PoP, giving us a more thorough understanding of the results as a whole. First, both color variations five (M = 842 ms) and six (M = 850 ms) yielded faster RTs than color variations three (M = 881 ms) and four (M = 884 ms). The difference was significant in all cases (Figure 3; Fisher LSD post-hoc tests with mean RTs of color variation five against color variation three: p <.005; Fisher LSD post-hoc tests with mean RTs of color variation six against color variation three: p <.001; Fisher LSD post-hoc tests with mean RTs of color variation five against color variation four: p <.02; Fisher LSD post hoc tests with mean RTs of color variation six against color variation four: p <.005). Also, for color variation six, a difference was revealed in relation to both color variation one (M = 867 ms) and two (M = 875 ms), whereas variation six again yielded faster RTs (Figure 3; Fisher LSD post-hoc tests with mean RTs of color variation six against color variation one: p <.03; Fisher LSD post-hoc tests with mean RTs of color variation six against color variation two: p <.01). Therefore, we can conclude that when the target becomes a neutral item on the following PoP trial, it induces more rapid RTs. Conversely, the neutral colored relative oddball as a PoP distractor may have played a part in slowing down RTs. Thus, when the target was repeated, as in color variation two, repetition of a target from a free choice trial paired with a neutral color on the following PoP trial did not benefit from repeated runs of the same color chosen (two-tailed paired t test testing mean RTs of color variation two against repeat or non-repeat: t(8) = 0.182, p =.860). Furthermore, RTs for that variation were similar to when the previous target became a distractor as in variations three and four (Fisher LSD post-hoc tests with mean RTs of color variation two against color variation three: p =.11; Fisher LSD post-hoc tests with mean RTs of color variation two against color variation four: p =.405). Also, color variation one where both the target and distractors were repeated, yielded faster RTs than variation three and four (Fisher LSD post-hoc tests with mean RTs of color variation one against color variation three: p <.002; Fisher LSD post-hoc tests with mean RTs of color variation one against color variation four: p <.004), highlighting the inhibiting effect of role reversal in relation to the previously selected item. 13

Lastly, a benefit of repetition on free choice trials was noticeable for color variation six (MD = 36 ms; two-tailed paired t test testing mean RTs of color variation six against repeat or non-repeat: t(8) = 2.96, p <.05) and variation one (MD = 31.9 ms; two-tailed paired t test testing mean RTs of color variation one against repeat or non-repeat: t(8) = 2.45, p <.05). However, this was not the case for variation five where the distractor item became the target between the two types of trials (two-tailed paired t test testing mean RTs of color variation five against repeat or non-repeat: t(8) = -0.040, p =.969), suggesting that the benefit of repetition between trials might consist in the effects of the distractors. Summarizing these results, it is clear that the neutral color has an attention grabbing effect. This was evident when the target on a previous choice trial became a neutral item on the following PoP trial and the distractor persisted its role, it yielded significantly faster responses than all other variations. Conversely, when presented as a distractor, neutral color can also have an inhibiting effect. Thus, when the target persisted between the two types of trials but the distractor changed to a neutral item, RTs were on par with the role reversal variations, which were the slowest in the experiment. This again favours the possible attention grabbing effect of the neutral distractor, whereas the neutral item attracts attention from the predesignated target and induces slower RTs, even though the target maintained its role between trials. Separating the effects of the target and distractors in relation to runs, RTs seemed beneficial to distractor repetition but not target repetition, whereas runs of color variations where the distractor persisted yielded faster RTs, compared to all other variations. 14

General discussion Earlier priming research focused on how preceding stimuli may accelerate our visual perception in the present. That is, what has been previously attended to will catch our attention more rapidly than items that have been ignored (Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1994; Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1996). Also, other priming research has revealed the importance of neglecting stimuli which has been shown to be vital in visual search tasks (Kristjánsson & Driver, 2008). More recent studies have investigated the effect of voluntary selection on our current perception. They have established how selection in the past has an impact on goaldirected visual search tasks, whereas observers strongly preferred items they had previously attended to compared to what they had neglected (Brascamp et al. 2011). To explore this effect further, Harðarson (2014) added a neutral item to the experimental paradigm introduced by Brascamp et al. (2011). This enabled Harðarson (2014) to separate the effect of target and distractor repetition in relation to target selection. Thereby, they highlighted that what has been previously attended to is preferred to what has been neglected in the past. Furthermore, they established that observers preferred a neutral stimulus over a previously neglected item. Our research addressed the question of how voluntary selection influences the time it takes to recognize a predesignated target on a following PoP trial. By combining the experimental paradigm of Brascamp et al. (2011) and Harðarson (2014), we were able to explore this effect in relation to what observers had previously selected and neglected. Our main goal was to see whether selection and non-selection of free choice would affect the speed of recognition on the following PoP trial. This assumption was made due to prior findings of Brascamp et. al (2011) and Kristjánsson & Driver (2008), which suggested that freely selecting a target would induce traditional PoP RTs and that repeating a distractor plays a large role in priming. Moreover, we wanted to confirm the findings of Harðarson (2014) and Brascamp et al. (2011) that repeatedly selecting a target would yield faster RTs compared to when no such repetition took place. The results showed that the neutral condition yielded the fastest RTs. This came as a surprise since recent priming research indicates that RTs should be enhanced when the target persists between trials (Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1994; Maljkovic & Nakayama, 1996; Brascamp et al., 2011; Kristjánsson & Driver, 2008) and that observers favour selecting a previous target over a neutral one (Harðarson, 2014). However, here it seemed that when the target on free choice trials took a neutral color on the following PoP trial, it grabbed the 15

attention of observers and thus acted as a relative oddball stimulus which resulted in reduced RTs. These findings were somewhat unexpected. Since the role of priming is known as an automatic process that influences attention (Maljkovic & Nakayama, 2000), an observer should not benefit from making a conscious decision when faced with a relative oddball item that could disrupt an implicit habituation that priming might have evoked. Additionally, during debriefing of the experiment, observers reported that while submitting their responses little thought went into deciding which item to attend. Rather, they experienced that their eyes were automatically directed towards a particular item. This further suggests that responses were not under voluntary control. Thus, as the above-mentioned research has shown, priming should yield the fastest RTs when observers were faced with the two types of trials where neither target nor distractors altered rather than when the target changes to a neutral item. Surprisingly, this was not the case in our experiment. As has been presented, the neutral item seemed to be influential in relation to recognition speed. Furthermore, this was true regardless of which role the neutral item held, whether it was the target or a distractor. As mentioned above, this was beneficial if the previous target became a neutral item. However, when we expanded the conditions in relation to color variations, it was clear that when the distractor changed to a neutral item, it had an inhibiting effect on response speed. In fact, the RTs for this variation were similar to role reversal variation, which yielded the slowest RTs by far. This finding further indicates that the neutral item automatically grabbed the attention of the observers between the two types of trials, similar to how a loud noise might capture ones attention in a quiet library. Some results of Brascamp et al. (2011) were replicated in this experiment. Repeating run of target and distractor between trials resulted in faster RTs, suggesting the effect is cumulative. A new finding here is that faster RTs also appeared in the variation where a run of the same distractor in the free choice trials and the target diamond became a neutral item on the following PoP trial where the distractor persisted through more than one free choice trial as well as the subsequent PoP trial, but the target from the free choice trials changed its role to a neutral item. These findings lead us towards two possible conclusions. Firstly, when both of these results are combined, it highlights the cumulative priming effects of the distractors as was done by Kristjánsson & Driver (2008). This underlines the importance of the distractor, whereas more repetition enables the observer to suppress it with greater ease and thus 16

resulting in accelerated recognition of the target item. Secondly, when these results are compared to the main findings of the experiment, they reiterate the attention grabbing effect of the neutral item. Brascamp et al. (2011) revealed that free choice tends to induce conventional PoP RTs. This suggested that these two types of trials shared the same cognitive operations. Moreover, when Harðarson (2014) added a neutral item to their experimental paradigm, the results further supported this theory. However, it seems that we could not confirm these findings. Rather, our results imply a potential attention grabbing effect of the neutral stimulus. That is, when the target becomes a neutral item between the two types of trials, it becomes more salient which results in more rapid recognition than otherwise. This effect is further enhanced with repetition of distractor items. In summary, when separating the effects of selection and non-selection, we were unable to do so. Instead, it seems that the neutral item rather became more distinctive than the other two conditions, thereby yielding the fastest RTs in our experiment. A possible drawback has to do with the neutral diamond. Since it had been presented repeatedly throughout the experiment it was never unbiased, and therefore was only truly neutral in the first couple of trials. When designing the experimental paradigm, it was made sure it was random in relation to color variations of the diamonds. Thereby, each variation should have appeared approximately the same amount of times. Hence, observers might be familiar with each of the colors, irrelevant of their role. However, this element may not have been a large problem since it has been suggested that priming effects are short term (Maljkovic & Nakayama, 2000) and decay rapidly (Nakayama, Maljkovic & Kristjánsson, 2004). Moreover, it has been highlighted that the target, distractor and neutral items were not evenly favoured on free choice trials, suggesting that neutral diamonds evoke a different response than the other two items, even when they are only relatively novel (Harðarson, 2014). The effects of the neutral diamond could be investigated further in future research by designing an experimental paradigm where a diamond can become a true neutral item, making it possible to separate even further the effects of selection and non-selection with regards to priming. 17

References Brascamp, J. W., Blake, R., & Kristjánsson, Á. (2011). Deciding where to attend: Priming of pop-out drives target selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(6), 1700-1707. Harðarson, J. P. (2014). Choosing versus rejecting: Separating the effects of target and distractor repetition on target choice (Unpublished undergraduate thesis). University of Iceland, Reykjavík. Kristjánsson, Á., & Campana, G. (2010). Where perception meets memory: A review of repetition priming in visual search tasks. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72, 5-18. Kristjánsson Á., & Driver, J. (2008). Priming in visual search: Separating the effect of target repetition, distractor repetition and role-reversal. Vision Research, 48, 1217-1232. Kristjánsson, Á., Wang, D., & Nakayama, K. (2002). The role of priming in conjunctive visual search. Cognition, 85, 37-52. Maljkovic, V., & Nakayama, K. (1994). Priming of pop-out I: Role of features. Memory & Cognition, 22, 657-672. Maljkovic, V., & Nakayama, K. (1996). Priming of pop-out II: The role of position. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 977-991. Maljkovic, V., & Nakayama, K. (2000). Priming of pop-out III: A short-term implicit memory system beneficial for rapid target selection. Visual Cognition, 7, 571-595. Nakayama, K., Maljkovic, V., & Kristjánsson, Á. (2004). Short term memory for the rapid deployment of visual attention. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences III (pp. 397-408). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 18