SAMENESS AND REDUNDANCY IN TARGET DETECTION AND TARGET COMPARISON. Boaz M. Ben-David and Daniel Algom Tel-Aviv University

Similar documents
Species of Redundancy in Visual Target Detection

Imperfect, Unlimited-Capacity, Parallel Search Yields Large Set-Size Effects. John Palmer and Jennifer McLean. University of Washington.

Commentary on The self-organizing consciousness, by Pierre Perruchet and Annie Vinter

CHRONOMETRIC ANALYSIS OF CLASSIFICATION 1

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Table 1 Patient characteristics Preoperative. language testing

THEORIES AND METHODS OF MEMORY PSYCHOPHYSICS: LINKS TO COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY

VARIATION IN MEASUREMENT OF HIV RNA VIRAL LOAD

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

Converging measures of workload capacity

Attention capacity and task difficulty in visual search

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Categorical Perception

The spacing and lag effect in free recall

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

Exam 2 PS 306, Fall 2005

observational studies Descriptive studies

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

11-3. Learning Objectives

The number line effect reflects top-down control

Introduction to Categorization Theory

UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations?

The Race Model Inequality: Interpreting a Geometric Measure of the Amount of Violation

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine

FACILITATION IN RECOGNIZING PAIRS OF WORDS: EVIDENCE OF A DEPENDENCE BETWEEN RETRIEVAL OPERATIONS 1

Stimulus-form effects in recognition memory

The role of modality congruence in the presentation and recognition of taskirrelevant stimuli in dual task paradigms.

Bar Ilan University Ramat-Gan, Israel. Ph.D. in Cognitive Psychology 1999.

Interaction Between Social Categories in the Composite Face Paradigm. Wenfeng Chen and Naixin Ren. Chinese Academy of Sciences. Andrew W.

Theoretical Note. Automatic and Controlled Processing Revisited

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli

The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract

The Effect of Target Repetition on Semantic Priming in a Three-Target RSVP Task

Running head: FEATURE INTEGRATION AND TASK SWITCHING 1. Feature Integration and Task Switching:

Distributed Cognition, Representation, and Affordance

Alternative Explanations for Changes in Similarity Judgments and MDS Structure

Satiation in name and face recognition

Chapter 7. Mental Representation

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article. MOTION ONSET CAPTURES ATTENTION Richard A. Abrams and Shawn E. Christ. Washington University.

VISUAL PERCEPTION OF STRUCTURED SYMBOLS

Foundations for a Science of Social Inclusion Systems

priming disappear, while stays intact? Why does long-term positive long-term negative priming

SCALAR TIMING (EXPECTANCY) THEORY: A COMPARISON BETWEEN PROSPECTIVE AND RETROSPECTIVE DURATION. Abstract

Automaticity of Number Perception

Evaluation of Cognitive Processing in Redundant Audio Visual Signals

When the visual format of the color carrier word does and does not modulate the Stroop effect

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

LAB 1: MOTOR LEARNING & DEVELOPMENT REACTION TIME AND MEASUREMENT OF SKILLED PERFORMANCE. Name: Score:

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity

AN ALTERNATIVE METHOD FOR ASSESSING LIKING: POSITIONAL RELATIVE RATING VERSUS THE 9-POINT HEDONIC SCALE ABSTRACT

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

The Influence of the Attention Set on Exogenous Orienting

Ideals Aren t Always Typical: Dissociating Goodness-of-Exemplar From Typicality Judgments

Logical-Rule Models of Classification Response Times: A Synthesis of Mental-Architecture, Random-Walk, and Decision-Bound Approaches

The e#ect of temporal uncertainty on measurements: The case of priming e#ects. Rika MIZUNO and Takao MATSUI (Chubu University)

Head Up, Foot Down. Object Words Orient Attention to the Objects Typical Location. Research Report

Case-sensitive letter and bigram frequency counts from large-scale English corpora

TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Forest before trees? It depends where you look

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1

The scope of perceptual content, II: properties

Processing Emergent Features in Metaphor Comprehension

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions

Conjunctive Search for One and Two Identical Targets

Stimulus-Response Compatibilitiy Effects for Warning Signals and Steering Responses

On the failure of distractor inhibition in the attentional blink

Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them

Visual Processing (contd.) Pattern recognition. Proximity the tendency to group pieces that are close together into one object.

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

The influence of anxiety on processing capacity for threat detection

OPTIC FLOW IN DRIVING SIMULATORS

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544

Chen, Z. (2009). Not all features are created equal: Processing asymmetries between

Influence of Frequency on Difference Thresholds for Magnitude of Vertical Sinusoidal Whole-Body Vibration

Feature encoding and pattern classifications with sequentially presented Markov stimuli*

Redundancy gains and coactivation with two different. targets: The problem of target preferences and the effects of display frequency

General recognition theory of categorization: A MATLAB toolbox

Effects of Music In Autistic Children and their Behavior Dr. T. MYTHILY Ph. D

James J. Gibson. Early Alignment with the Classical Tradition

STUDY OF THE BISECTION OPERATION

Recency order judgments in short term memory: Replication and extension of Hacker (1980)

Masked Primes Can Be Genuinely Semantically Processed

Attentional Window and Global/Local Processing

PSYC20007 READINGS AND NOTES

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research

Stimulus-Driven Attentional Capture and Attentional Control Settings

The role of parity, physical size, and magnitude in numerical cognition: The SNARC effect revisited

Spatial Orientation Using Map Displays: A Model of the Influence of Target Location

Set-Size Effects in Identification and Localization

Transcription:

SAMENESS AND REDUNDANCY IN TARGET DETECTION AND TARGET COMPARISON Boaz M. Ben-David and Daniel Algom Tel-Aviv University boazb@post.tau.ac.il Abstract Searching for targets, people perform better and reap larger redundancy gains when the targets comprise physically different stimuli than when they comprise replicates of the same physical stimulus. Comparing pairs of stimuli on sameness or difference, people are better when sameness is defined by shared physical features than by shared names or shared meaning. This seeming inconsistency of the effect of stimulus sameness and redundancy on behavior is elucidated. Two popular tasks of cognitive psychology center on the notions of sameness and redundancy, the Redundant Target Design (RTD), and the Same-Different Judgment task (SDJ). In the typical RTD experiment (Townsend and Nozawa, 1995), the stimulus ensemble comprises several items (say, the letters A, B, C and D) some of which are defined as targets (say, A and D), the others as distractors (B and C). On each trial, a pair of letters is presented and the participant's task is to make one response ("Yes") if at least one of the letters is a target (AA, DD, DA, AD, AB, BA, AC, CA, DB, BD, DC, or CD), but another response ("No") if none of the letters presented is a target (i.e., both stimuli are distractors, BB, CC, BC, and CB). The impact of redundancy is expressed by the redundant target effect: Affirmative target responses are faster to AA, DD, AD, or DA (redundant- or double-targets displays) than to AB, BA, AC, CA, DB, BD, DC, or CD (single-target displays). Note that the redundant targets displays can contain physical replicates of the same letters (AA, DD) or different letters (AD, DA). Surprisingly, the redundant target effect is larger when the redundant targets comprise different letters than when they comprise the same letters (performance and redundancy gains are better with AD or DA than with AA or DD). In the typical SDJ experiment, the same letters and category assignment (i.e., criteria for sameness and difference) are used. Again, a pair of letters is presented on a trial and the participant's task is to make one response if the two stimuli are the same (e.g., AA, DD, AD or DA) but another response if they are different (e.g., AB, BA, AC, CA, DB, BD, DC or CD). In the SDJ task, performance (latency and errors for "same" responses) is better with replicates of the same letters (AA, DD) than with different letters that share the

category by virtue of the experimenter's assignment (AD, DA). Therefore, to a first approximation, sameness affects behavior differently in the two tasks. There exists a different targets advantage in the RTD task but a same target advantage in the SDJ task. In this article, we elucidate this paradox. Levels of Processing This brief introduction already makes clear the need to deconstruct the notion of sameness. It is not the simple self-evident concept that it may appear on a first look. Stimuli can be the same (or different) in at least three aspects. They can be the same physically, when a stimulus reappears after a temporal interval or when stimuli are replicas of one another. Stimuli can be the same nominally when they share the same name. And, stimuli can be the same semantically when they mean the same thing or category or invite the same action. Typically, reproductions of a physical stimulus go by an invariant (i.e., the same) name, and share a semantic meaning. However, physically different stimuli can carry the same name as in A and a. And the nominally identical stimuli A and a are semantically different stimuli when sameness is defined by case. Another interesting exception is the physical stimulus "O," sometimes called "zero," and sometimes called the "o" as in "O my goodness!" Therefore, physically same stimuli can go by different names and mean different things in different contexts. In many an instance, stimuli that are the same semantically do not share physical features and do not go by the same name. In the aforementioned RTD experiment, A and D were the same semantically (targets triggering the same response), hence redundant stimuli, by virtue of the experimenter's decision to assign them a common response. On the road, a policeman signaling to stop, red traffic lights, or a little girl crossing, all cause you, the driver, to reach for the brakes. They are redundant stimuli because any one of these stimuli alone suffices for eliciting your response. It is this semantic sameness that enables you to reap a redundancy gain when more than one of the stimuli are present (you reach for the brakes faster with policeman signaling and the little girl crossing than you do with either stimulus appearing alone). As we recounted in the outset, the criterion selected for sameness and difference (by nature or by an experimenter) affects the behavioral outcome in a systematic fashion. Surprisingly, the effect appears to be different in the RTD experiment and the SDJ experiment. This difference might tap different cognitive heuristics. Using variants of the SDJ task, Posner (1978, see also, Posner and Snyder, 1975) proposed a general hierarchy of processing based on the three levels of sameness or difference that we discussed (physical, nominal, or semantic). In Posner's view, various tasks and criteria tap onto one (or more) of these levels of processing. Perceptual tasks are fastest, semantic tasks are slowest, and nominal ones are intermediate. In substantive terms, the visual system first analyses the physical features of the presented stimulus, constructing a visual code for comparison. This visual code then is linked to a name. This name in turn serves in semantic tasks of categorization. This theory implies a temporal hierarchy from physical to nominal to semantic sameness (or difference). We next examine the pertinent results in the two tasks associated with sameness and redundancy.

Levels of Sameness in the RTD Task What does sameness mean in an RTD environment? Do stimuli need to be physical replicates of one another in order to be redundant? Does nominal or semantic sameness suffice to produce the redundant targets effect? How does the magnitude of the redundant targets effect depend on the composition of the targets? In general, to what extent or to what level do stimuli have to be the same in order to be redundant? This critical issue has not been pursued in the RTD literature with a single notable exception. Grice and Reed (1992) defined as targets the letters A and a. Obviously, these targets share names but differ physically. Grice and Reed compared performance on double-target trials comprising letters Aa with that on double target trials comprising AA or aa in which the targets were physical reproductions of one another. They found redundancy gains for both types of doubletargets. Notably, the gains were considerably larger for Aa than for either AA or aa. In another experiment, Grice and Reed (1992) used targets that shared neither physical nor nominal features. The capital letters A and D were defined as targets. Redundancy gains were obtained both for AD or DA and for AA or DD as redundant targets. Again, the redundant target effect was larger for the different targets displays (AD, DA) than for the same target displays (AA, DD). Subsequently, Mordkoff and Miller (1993) argued that if participants respond much faster to one target than to the other, then Grice and Reed's (1992) results obtain. Mordkoff and Miller (1993) replicated the second experiment by Grice and Reed, controlling for target preferences and inter-stimulus contingencies. Notably, Mordkoff and Miller still found performance on different-targets trials to be at least as good as performance on identical targets trials. Therefore, in the RTD experiment physically and nominally different targets tend to yield better performance than physically same targets. Levels of Sameness in the SDJ Task The issue of levels of sameness has received considerable attention in the SDJ literature. Early results reported by Posner and Mitchel (1967) proved to be general. They presented pairs of letters in an SDJ task under three different criteria for sameness: physical, nominal, and semantic. Participants were faster to classify pairs of letters as "same" on the basis of physical identity (AA) than on the basis of nominal identity (Aa). It took participants even longer to classify letters on the basis of semantic category membership (AE are the same because both are vowels). A summary of Posner and Mitchell's data is presented in Figure 1. Observe the effect of response criterion: RTs for physical sameness were 74 milliseconds faster than for nominal sameness, which in turn were 76 milliseconds faster than those for semantic sameness. That is, reaction times (RTs) for a criterion at a given level of processing were faster by 75 milliseconds on average than those for a criterion at the next level of processing. In the SDJ task, therefore, physically identical stimuli tend to yield better performance than nominally or semantically same stimuli.

Rules for Sameness Average "Same" Responses Physical 549 AA 623 Aa 699 Ae (vowels) Nominal Semantic Average "Different" Responses 801 An Figure 1: Illustration of the effect of the definition of sameness on performance. Mean RTs (milliseconds) for letter pairs in a SDJ experiment. [Adapted from Posner and Mitchel, 1967]. The Paradox Sameness and redundancy are like Tweedledum and Tweedledee. Each implies the other to the extent that they probably refer to a single concept. Sameness implies redundancy reflected in the common caveat against repeating the same stimulus twice or more. And, redundancy, in turn, implies sameness. In the dictionary (The American Heritage Dictionary, 1985, p. 1038), redundancy is defined as "unnecessary repetition." Obviously, you can repeat only the same action or stimulus. In Information Theory, too, redundancy is conceived as "repeated information" (Miller, 1953, p. 9), implying sameness. By definition, different information cannot be redundant. This much granted, stimuli need not be physical replicates of each other in order to reap redundancy gains in behavior. Given the close association between sameness and redundancy, it is puzzling that two popular tests of the effect of each stimuli on behavior yield seemingly inconsistent results. The effect of stimulus sameness can be generally assessed through two separate paths. RTD exemplifies the functional or behavioral approach: All stimuli producing a given response are considered "same" by virtue of this fact. In other words, response evocation defines sameness or redundancy. The SDJ is predicated on another, psychophysical approach. Stimuli are the same if they share the same properties and are perceived correctly as replicates. Sameness is thus defined directly by the judgments of observers. Why does physical sameness enhance performance in the SDJ task, but does not enhance performance in the RTD task? Conversely, why do physically (and nominally) different stimuli lead to large gains in the RTD task, but are less well perceived and processed in the SDJ task? When judging stimuli directly on sameness, physical features seem decisive. However, when the stimuli are presented for action, common meaning, and not common physical appearance is decisive. Resolving the various effects of stimulus sameness and redundancy on behavior gives sustenance.

References Grice G. R., & Reed, J. M. (1992). What makes targets redundant? Perception & Psychophysics, 51 (5), 437-442. Miller, G. A. (1953). What is information measurement? American Psychologist, 8, 3-11. Mordkoff, J. T., & Miller, J. (1993). Redundancy gains and co-activations with two different targets: The problem of target preferences and the effects of display frequency. Perception and Psychophysics, 53 (5), 527 535. Posner, M. I. (1978) Chronometric explorations of mind The third Paul M. Fitts lectures delivered at the university of Michigan (Chapter 2, pp.27-56). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum associates publishers. Posner, M. I., & Mitchell R. F. (1967). Chronometric analysis of classification. Psychological Review, 74, 392-409. Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C.R.R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 55-85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum The American Heritage Dictionary (1985, Second Edition). Boston: Houghton Mifflin company. Townsend, J. T., & Nozawa, G. (1995). Spatio-temporal properties of elementary perception: An investigation of parallel, serial, and coactive theories. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 39, 321-359.