Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing

Similar documents
Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Dual-Task Interference with Equal Task Emphasis: Graded Capacity-Sharing or Central Postponement? and Eliot Hazeltine. NASA - Ames Research Center

Kimron Shapiro (Ed.), pp Oxford University Press

Dual-Task Interference in Simple Tasks: Data and Theory

Parallel response selection in dual-task situations via automatic category-to-response translation

Does Mental Rotation Require Central Mechanisms?

Procedural Learning: 1. Locus of Practice Effects in Speeded Choice Tasks

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task?

Is the psychological refractory period effect for ideomotor compatible tasks eliminated by speed-stress instructions?

Repetition blindness is immune to the central bottleneck

Editorial. From the Editors: Perspectives on Turnaround Time

University of Alberta. The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm. Shawn Tan

Tasks at the Same Time

Multiple Spans in Transcription Typing

Does the Central Bottleneck Encompass Voluntary Selection of Hedonically-Based Choices?

Repetition Blindness has a perceptual locus: Evidence from online processing of targets in RSVP streams

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Alternative Explanations for Changes in Similarity Judgments and MDS Structure

Separating Limits on Preparation Versus Online Processing in Multitasking Paradigms: Evidence for Resource Models

Modality pairing effects and the response selection bottleneck

Audio: In this lecture we are going to address psychology as a science. Slide #2

CS 544 Human Abilities

Procedural Learning: 2. Intertrial Repetition Effects in Speeded-Choice Tasks

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

Blindness to response-compatible stimuli in the psychological refractory period paradigm

A model of parallel time estimation

Why Practice Reduces Dual-Task Interference

Does the Central Bottleneck Encompass Voluntary Selection of Hedonically Based Choices?

To appear in Memory & Cognition

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning

The specificity of learned parallelism in dual-memory retrieval

Translating basic attentional paradigms to schizophrenia research: Reconsidering the nature of the deficits

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Attention capacity and task difficulty in visual search

Parallel Response Selection after Callosotomy

Thompson, Valerie A, Ackerman, Rakefet, Sidi, Yael, Ball, Linden, Pennycook, Gordon and Prowse Turner, Jamie A

Reinforcement Learning : Theory and Practice - Programming Assignment 1

Dorsal and Ventral Processing Under Dual-Task Conditions Wilfried Kunde, 1 Franziska Landgraf, 1 Marko Paelecke, 1 and Andrea Kiesel 2

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

Page # Perception and Action. Lecture 3: Perception & Action. ACT-R Cognitive Architecture. Another (older) view

The generation of two isochronous sequences in parallel

Task Prioritization in Multitasking During Driving: Opportunity to Abort a Concurrent Task Does Not Insulate Braking Responses from Dual-Task Slowing

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Salient Feature Coding in Response Selection. Circularity?

The scope of perceptual content, II: properties

Sleeping Beauty is told the following:

Cognition. Mid-term 1. Top topics for Mid Term 1. Heads up! Mid-term exam next week

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences

Running head: INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 1. Why to treat subjects as fixed effects. James S. Adelman. University of Warwick.

Does Working Memory Load Lead to Greater Impulsivity? Commentary on Hinson, Jameson, and Whitney (2003)

Central Interference in Driving

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

OVERVIEW TUTORIAL BEHAVIORAL METHODS CLAIM: EMLAR VII EYE TRACKING: READING. Lecture (50 min) Short break (10 min) Computer Assignments (30 min)

Properties represented in experience

Is Cognitive Science Special? In what way is it special? Cognitive science is a delicate mixture of the obvious and the incredible

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen

Perceptual, Cognitive, and Motoric Aspects of Transcription Typing

Stroop-Type Interference: Congruity Effects in Color Naming With Typewritten Responses

The influence of one memory retrieval on a subsequent memory retrieval*

Automaticity of Number Perception

Lecture 2.1 What is Perception?

Is it possible to gain new knowledge by deduction?

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

How Does Analysis of Competing Hypotheses (ACH) Improve Intelligence Analysis?

Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept. of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information:

Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli

Eliminative materialism

Parallel scanning ofauditory and visual information

Optical Illusions 4/5. Optical Illusions 2/5. Optical Illusions 5/5 Optical Illusions 1/5. Reading. Reading. Fang Chen Spring 2004

CONTEXT OF BACKWARD COMPATIBILITY EFFECTS

Modeling a reaction time variant of the Perruchet effect in humans

Confirmation, Falsification, and Fallibility

Working memory involvement in dual-task performance: Evidence from the backward compatibility effect

Color Science and Spectrum Inversion: A Reply to Nida-Rümelin

How To Optimize Your Training For i3 Mindware v.4 And Why 2G N-Back Brain Training Works

Our previous accounts of perceptual experience accepted the phenomenal principle:

Task Switching. Higher-Level Cognition Oct 7, 2008

One slide on research question Literature review: structured; holes you will fill in Your research design

Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks

Attention and Scene Perception

Running head: SIMULTANEOUS SUPERIORITY, NOT A SELECTION BIAS 1

Dynamics of the Central Bottleneck: Dual-Task and Task Uncertainty

Design Methodology. 4th year 1 nd Semester. M.S.C. Madyan Rashan. Room No Academic Year

Phenomenal content. PHIL April 15, 2012

Short-Term Memory Demands of Reaction-Time Tasks That Differ in Complexity

PLANNING THE RESEARCH PROJECT

Appearance properties

Capacity Limits in Mechanical Reasoning

ANTICIPATING DYNAMIC LOADS IN HANDLING OBJECTS.

The creation of lasting memories

Perceptual Load in Different Regions of the Visual Field and its Effect on Attentional Selectivity. Hadas Marciano Advisor: Yaffa Yeshurun

Comensana, McGrath Perceptual Reasons

Where No Interface Has Gone Before: What Can the Phaser Teach Us About Label Usage in HCI?

Further Properties of the Priority Rule

The Effect o f Alcohol on Human Information Processing Rate

Evidence for parallel semantic memory retrieval in dual tasks

SWINBURNE S NEW SOUL: A RESPONSE TO MIND, BRAIN AND FREE WILL

Transcription:

THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1994, 47A (1) 201-205 Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing Harold Pashler University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California, U.S.A. This paper deals with the claim made by McLeod and Hume (this issue) that, for a skilled typist, the rate of responding is controlled by a limit on the number of responses that can be executed simultaneously, not by a limit on response selection. I point out that McLeod and Hume's observations are consistent with various interpretations, including one in which response rates are limited by response selection. I also note the importance of trying to distinguish structural and strategic bottlenecks in speeded responding. Pashler (this issue) concluded that responses are selected one at a time in an unpractised serial choice reaction-time task, even when the subject was permitted to preview stimuli ahead of the ones to which they had responded. This was demonstrated by showing that the preview caused the duration of response selection to become rate-limiting, whereas the duration of perception and response execution ceased to be rate-limiting. McLeod and Hume (this issue) questioned whether such a claim could apply to the behaviour of skilled copy-typists, who produce a sequence of keypresses separated by extremely short interresponse intervals. They reported that a skilled typist made finger movements that anticipated the spatial position of keypresses well ahead of those currently being produced. For example, when the typist typed PINING, the finger on the left hand that would type the G often began moving into position around the time the first N was typed. If keypress responses were selected one at a time, McLeod and Hume reasoned, then anticipatory behaviour of this sort should not have occurred (see also Rumelhart & Norman, 1982). Details of the anticipations suggested to McLeod and Hume that response rate Requests for reprints should be sent to Harold Pashler, Department of Psychology C-0109, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093, U.S.A. The author is grateful to Clark Fagot and James C. Johnston for useful comments. 1994 The Experimental Psychology Society

202 PASHLER was limited by a bottleneck in execution of the keypresses, and they inferred that the proposed response selection bottleneck could not apply in typing. McLeod and Hume's observations are of great interest, but they are consistent with various interpretations other than those they mention. Logically speaking, what can be inferred from the fact that a hand moves into position to produce the response to Character N + 3 before the response to Character N has been produced? It would seem that the only thing one can infer with certainty is that the response to Character N + 3 was selected before the response to Character N was produced. This does not rule out the possibility that the response to Character N was selected before the response to Character N + 3 was selected. McLeod and Hume appear to assume that execution of a response follows its selection without delay. It is logically possible, however, that selection operates rapidly and sequentially, character by character, inserting response specifications into a buffer from which responses are sequentially produced (at a slightly slower rate) but after a significant further delay. This account appears to be consistent with all of McLeod and Hume's observations. It may also be problematic to assume that what amounts to response selection in typing if such a process exists in that task operates one character at a time. This assumption underlies McLeod and Hume's argument. There is another possibility, however: that the response selection process handles "chunks" larger than a single keypress. This is an old idea that has been discussed for a long time in the typing literature (see e.g., Salthouse 1984; Shaffer & Hardwick, 1970). On this view, the output of the response selection stage might be a relatively abstract (perhaps spatial) specification of a sequence of several keypress responses. This process might leave the details of particular finger movements to be determined by subsequent motor programming mechanisms. The fact that typists type random letter strings more slowly than they type words has been cited as evidence for this idea (Fendrick, 1937). The hypothesis that response selection at the level of these larger chunks proceeds sequentially would be quite consistent with anticipatory behaviour of the sort observed by McLeod and Hume. Several kinds of evidence might shed further light on these various possibilities. One interesting question is whether the anticipatory movements observed by McLeod and Hume still occur when a typist types letter strings that have extremely low transitional frequencies designed to discourage chunking at the word or even bigram level. Another experiment that might be informative would combine speeded typing of a word with another task, such as a vocal response to a tone presented at a short delay. Suppose there really is a response selection process in typing that takes input in word-sized chunks and constitutes a bottleneck, as suggested above. In that case the typing task ought to produce a PRP effect (delaying

CONTROL OF SERIAL ORDER IN TYPING 203 the vocal response), but that delay ought to depend very little on the length of the word being typed. Pashler and Christian (submitted) found roughly this pattern of RTs with a first task that required subjects to produce one of several arbitrary keypress sequences. The sequences consisted of a variable number of keypresses; variation in the number of keypresses required in the first task had little effect on how much the subsequent vocal response was delayed. One final experimental approach would be to seek factorial manipulations that would define translation and execution stages in typing and observe their effects on IRIs (with and without preview), as in the Pashler (this issue) study. In short, McLeod and Hume's observations are intriguing, but they are by no means inconsistent with the claim that there is a response selection process that proceeds sequentially in skilled copytyping. This process might select individual keypress responses very rapidly well before they are actually produced or, perhaps more plausibly, larger-sized response "chunks" at a more modest rate (Salthouse, 1984). Either of these possibilities would be consistent with response execution being rate-limiting, as McLeod and Hume argue, and further investigations of typing ought to be able to distinguish among these hypotheses. If it should turn out that selection of individual keypress responses proceeds in parallel as McLeod and Hume suppose this would raise the interesting question of why typing would differ from the serial choice-rt tasks investigated by Pashler (this issue). One obvious difference is the amount of practice the subjects have had with a particular S-R mapping low in the case of the serial RT task, and high for the typist. Response selection operations that have been consistently practised may simply operate in parallel with other activities. Although intuitively plausible, there is little objective evidence for this idea. The psychological refractory period (PRP) effect is not readily eliminated with practice (Gottsdanker & Stelmach, 1971), but as there is no specific evidence that response selection remains a bottleneck after extensive practice, that does not completely resolve the issue. McLeod and Hume offer a different sort of explanation for why response selection proceeds sequentially in the serial RT situation (Pashler, this issue), even though it might and, in their view, it does operate in parallel in typing. Their suggestion is that the variability of response selection duration in choice RT is so great that if responses were selected in parallel, responses would often come out in the wrong order. Therefore, they suggest, the subject adopts a strategy of selecting them one at a time in order to prevent this from occurring (whereas in typing, subjects do not need to do this). This is an interesting possibility. I am not aware of any serial RT experiments that would test it, but recent data collected using the PRP task does not support this idea. Carrier and Pashler (submitted) found evidence of a central bottleneck when the first task was quick (button

204 PASHLER press to the pitch of a tone), the second task was time-consuming (paired associate retrieval with a vocal response), and the instructions simply told the subject to perform each task as quickly as possible. The data showed the usual PRP effect, and association strength slowed the second response to the same degree, regardless of SOA. This evidence indicates queueing of central stages. It is difficult to see how this queueing could have been adopted as a strategy to ensure that responses came out in the right order, as (1) the responses would almost always have come out in the usual order, even if they had been selected independently, and (2) the instructions to the subjects did not require responding in any particular order in any case. Nonetheless, McLeod and Hume are surely correct that the issue of structural versus strategic bottlenecks is important and needs to be investigated further. In summary, McLeod and Hume's commentary achieves several useful purposes. It documents a very interesting effect in copytyping behaviour, an effect that might seem to make sequential selection of keypresses an unlikely possibility (although, it was argued above, that possibility actually remains viable); their commentary also raises the important issue of whether response selection bottlenecks are structural features of the human mind or strategies questions in need of further investigation. The present reply suggests that McLeod and Hume's copytyping data are compatible with the idea of a structural bottleneck in response choice operating even in the case of a skilled copytypist. Perhaps the most important conclusion to be drawn from the present exchange is this: some cognitive models may be difficult or impossible to test with psychological data, but hypotheses about the scheduling of relatively time-consuming mental operations (such as selecting responses and programming motor sequences) seem to be particularly suited to being empirically tested with the techniques of the experimental psychologist. These hypotheses also have fundamental implications about human cognitive architecture. REFERENCES Carrier, M., & Pashler, H. (submitted). Attentional demands of memory retrieval. Fendrick, P. (1937). Hierarchical skills in typewriting. Journal of Educational Psychology, 28, 609-620. Gottsdanker, R., & Stelmach, G.E. (1971). The persistence of psychological refractoriness. Journal of Motor Behavior, 3, 301-312. McLeod, P., & Hume, M. (1994, this issue). Overlapping mental operations in serial performance with preview: Typing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 193-199. Pashler, H.E. (1994, this issue). Overlapping mental operations in serial performance with preview. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 161-191. Pashler, H.E., & Christian, C. (submitted). Dual-task interference and the production of manual and vocal responses.

CONTROL OF SERIAL ORDER IN TYPING 205 Rumelhart, D.E., & Norman, D.A. (1982). Simulating a skilled typist: A study of skilled cognitive-motor performance. Cognitive Science, 6, 1-36. Salthouse, T.A. (1984). The skill of typing. Scientific American, 250, 128-135. Shaffer, L.H., & Hardwick, J. (1970). The basis of transcription skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 84, 424-440.