Enhancing the Effectiveness of Alcohol Warning Labels With a Self-Affirming Implementation Intention

Similar documents
Responsible alcohol consumption

How useful are the stages of change for targeting interventions? randomized test of a brief intervention to reduce smoking

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

An experimental test of an alcohol expectancy challenge in mixed gender groups of young heavy drinkers

AlcoholEdu for College

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

Measuring and Assessing Study Quality

Development of the Web Users Self Efficacy scale (WUSE)

REDUCING FRIDAY ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION AMONG MODERATE, WOMEN DRINKERS: EVALUATION OF A BRIEF EVIDENCE-BASED INTERVENTION

Running Head: STEREOTYPE THREAT AND THE RACIAL ACHIEVEMENT GAP 1

At the Israel Electric Company: Israel Railways

CURVE is the Institutional Repository for Coventry University

Measuring Self-Esteem of Adolescents Based on Academic Performance. Grambling State University

Predicting and explaining transtheoretical model stage transitions in relation to condom-carrying behaviour

The Effect of Background Color and Level of Threat on Evoked Fear and Smoking Cessation Intentions

Alcohol Problems in Intimate Relationships

Predictors of Cigarette Smoking Behavior Among Military University Students in Taiwan. Wang, Kwua-Yun; Yang, Chia-Chen

Thank you very much for your guidance on this revision. We look forward to hearing from you.

Integrating Emotion and the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Consumers Activism in the Internet Web site

Risky Choice Decisions from a Tri-Reference Point Perspective

UBC Social Ecological Economic Development Studies (SEEDS) Student Report

Survey Research. We can learn a lot simply by asking people what we want to know... THE PREVALENCE OF SURVEYS IN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH

Motivational enhancement therapy for high-risk adolescent smokers

Invitation and Family Questionnaire

Running head: SEXUAL VICTIMIZATION IN THE TRANSITION TO COLLEGE 1. Sexual Victimization During the First Two Months at SUNY Geneseo:

Thriving in College: The Role of Spirituality. Laurie A. Schreiner, Ph.D. Azusa Pacific University

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies

Applying Behavioral Theories of Choice to Substance Use in a Sample of Psychiatric Outpatients

Evaluating Smokers Reactions to Advertising for New Lower Nicotine Quest Cigarettes

Web-based brief personalized feedback intervention in a Non- Treatment Seeking Population of Heavy Drinkers a Randomized Controlled Trial

A theory-based online health behaviour intervention for new university students results from a repeat randomized controlled trial

School orientation and mobility specialists School psychologists School social workers Speech language pathologists

VIRGINIA LAW REVIEW IN BRIEF

Examining the efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) to understand pre-service teachers intention to use technology*

GOLDSMITHS Research Online Article (refereed)

Beth J. Massie. The Effect of Training on User Buy-In. A Master s Paper for the M.S. in I.S degree. April, pages. Advisor: Dr. Robert Losee.

Running head: RANK-FRAMING IN SOCIAL NORMS INTERVENTIONS 1

Asking and answering research questions. What s it about?

Measuring the Impact of One Assignment on Reported Sustainability-Related Behaviors

Extension of the Children's Perceptions of Interparental Conflict Scale for Use With Late Adolescents

EXPLORING CASINO GAMBLING IMPACT PERCEPTIONS: A GENDERED SOCIAL EXCHANGE THEORY APPROACH

Nova Scotia Board of Examiners in Psychology. Custody and Access Evaluation Guidelines

Professional Development: proposals for assuring the continuing fitness to practise of osteopaths. draft Peer Discussion Review Guidelines

Armitage, Harris and Arden (2011) Alcohol Caffeine

Improving clinicians' attitudes toward providing feedback on routine outcome assessments

Donate to research not involving embryonic stem cells

Alcohol, drug and related health and wellbeing issues among young people completing an online screen.

David O Malley, Ph.D., LISW Case Western Reserve University Cleveland, Ohio

!!!!!!! !!!!!!!!!!!!

Internal Consistency and Reliability of the Networked Minds Measure of Social Presence

Citation for published version (APA): Weert, E. V. (2007). Cancer rehabilitation: effects and mechanisms s.n.

An Empirical Study of the Roles of Affective Variables in User Adoption of Search Engines

Chapter 3-Attitude Change - Objectives. Chapter 3 Outline -Attitude Change

The Effects of Societal Versus Professor Stereotype Threats on Female Math Performance

Community Trials Intervention to Reduce High-Risk Drinking

GUIDELINES FOR SCHOOL PEER GUIDE CO-ORDINATORS

Principles of publishing

Models of Information Retrieval

Investigating Motivation for Physical Activity among Minority College Females Using the BREQ-2

SELF-EFFICACY AND THE MODERATE (OR LESS) DRINKER: A SURVIVOR S GUIDE FOR COPING WITH HEAVY DRINKING ON (OR NEAR) COLLEGE CAMPUSES

Patient Reference Group Action Plan

ProScript User Guide. Pharmacy Access Medicines Manager

Visual Information Priming in Internet of Things: Focusing on the interface of smart refrigerator

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION. smoking causes cancer, heart related diseases, impotent, and serious damage to unborn

WARNING, DISTRACTION, AND RESISTANCE TO INFLUENCE 1

MERLE MULLINS COUNSELING REGISTRATION FORM (Please Print) CLIENT INFORMATION

Motivation: Internalized Motivation in the Classroom 155

Information and education

Asia Illicit Tobacco Indicator 2016: Singapore. Prepared by Oxford Economics October 2017

Title: The Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) and Texting While Driving Behavior in College Students MS # Manuscript ID GCPI

MC IRB Protocol No.:

A Study of a Diet Improvement Method for Controlling High Sodium Intake Based on Protective Motivation Theory

University of Bristol - Explore Bristol Research

Management Science Letters

BRIEF REPORT OPTIMISTIC BIAS IN ADOLESCENT AND ADULT SMOKERS AND NONSMOKERS

The Effects of #fitspiration Instagram Photos on Adolescent. Girls' Self Perceived Body Image. Name. V00xxxx. University of Victoria

Group Assignment #1: Concept Explication. For each concept, ask and answer the questions before your literature search.

Journey to Truth Counseling

THE souhce OF A PERSUASIVE MESSAGE has at her/his disposal numerous

This paper contains analysis of the results of these processes and sets out the programme of future development.

SURVEY TOPIC INVOLVEMENT AND NONRESPONSE BIAS 1

POLICY. Institutional Research Projects/Data Requests #7220

Computer tailored persuasion: effectiveness and side-effects. Arie Dijkstra University of Groningen The Netherlands

Social Support as a Mediator of the Relationship Between Self-esteem and Positive Health Practices: Implications for Practice

Title: Intention-to-treat and transparency of related practices in randomized, controlled trials of anti-infectives

Consumption of different types of alcohol and mortality

Title: Use of food labels by adolescents to make healthier choices on snacks: a cross sectional study from Sri Lanka

Measuring mathematics anxiety: Paper 2 - Constructing and validating the measure. Rob Cavanagh Len Sparrow Curtin University

This is a repository copy of Combining Self-Affirmation and Implementation Intentions to Reduce Heavy Episodic Drinking in University Students.

Randomized controlled trial of physical activity counseling as an aid to smoking cessation: 12 month follow-up

Open Access at ETH Zurich: Experiences and Challenges Impact of Open Science on research careers, swissuniversities,

White Rose Research Online URL for this paper:

Research-Based Insights on Motivation. Laurel McNall, Ph.D. Associate Professor of Psychology

KAP Keys. For Clinicians. Based on TIP 24 A Guide to Substance Abuse Services for Primary Care Clinicians. CSAT s Knowledge Application Program

Deakin Research Online Deakin University s institutional research repository DDeakin Research Online Research Online This is the published version:

Evaluation of the effectiveness of social marketing approach in smoking cessation and promoting health in Australian university students

43. Can subliminal messages affect behavior? o Subliminal messages have NO effect on behavior - but people perceive that their behavior changed.

Marketing a healthier choice: Exploring young people s perception of e-cigarettes

Centerstone Research Institute

Transcription:

Enhancing the Effectiveness of Alcohol Warning Labels With a Self-Affirming Implementation Intention ARMITAGE, Christopher J and ARDEN, Madelynne A <http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6199-717x> Available from Sheffield Hallam University Research Archive (SHURA) at: http://shura.shu.ac.uk/12127/ This document is the author deposited version. You are advised to consult the publisher's version if you wish to cite from it. Published version ARMITAGE, Christopher J and ARDEN, Madelynne A (2016). Enhancing the Effectiveness of Alcohol Warning Labels With a Self-Affirming Implementation Intention. Health psychology, 35 (10), 1159-1163. Repository use policy Copyright and Moral Rights for the papers on this site are retained by the individual authors and/or other copyright owners. Users may download and/or print one copy of any article(s) in SHURA to facilitate their private study or for noncommercial research. You may not engage in further distribution of the material or use it for any profit-making activities or any commercial gain. Sheffield Hallam University Research Archive http://shura.shu.ac.uk

Self-Affirming Implementation Intention 1 Enhancing the Effectiveness of Alcohol Warning Labels With a Self-Affirming Implementation Intention Christopher J. Armitage University of Manchester Madelynne A. Arden Sheffield Hallam University Correspondence to: Christopher J. Armitage Manchester Centre for Health Psychology, School of Psychological Sciences Manchester Academic Health Science Centre, University of Manchester Coupland Street, Oxford Road Manchester, M13 9PL, UK E-mail: chris.armitage@manchester.ac.uk Tel: +44 (0) 161 275 2556 Fax: +44 (0) 161 275 2623 Author note. The authors declare no conflict of interest. We would like to thank the Alcohol Research UK for funding Study 1 and Claire Wolstenholme for her help with collecting and entering the data for Study 1 and to Rebecca Smith for her help with collecting and entering the data for Study 2.

Self-Affirming Implementation Intention 2 Brief Report: Enhancing the Effectiveness of Alcohol Warning Labels With a Self- Affirming Implementation Intention Objective. Excess alcohol consumption extorts significant social and economic costs that are increasing despite the presence of mandatory warning labels on packaged alcoholic beverages. We used a novel approach by adding a brief statement based on self-affirmation theory (Steele, 1988) to alcohol warning labels. Method. In two studies (N = 85; N = 58), we randomized regular wine drinkers recruited from University campuses to complete a wine pouring task with bottles that had standard labeling, or bottles that added a self-affirming implementation intention to the standard labeling. Alcohol consumption, behavioral intention and self-efficacy were measured premanipulation; message acceptance was measured post-manipulation; and alcohol consumption, behavioral intention and self-efficacy were measured again at follow-up. Results. In both studies, the self-affirming implementation intention significantly reduced subsequent alcohol consumption (ds = 0.70 and 0.91, respectively). However, message acceptance, behavioral intention, and self-efficacy did not significantly mediate the observed effects. Conclusions. Self-affirming implementation intentions augmented the effect of alcohol warning labels to reduce subsequent alcohol consumption, but consistent with the broader self-affirmation literature it was not clear what mediated the effects. Further research is required to examine whether self-affirming implementation intentions could augment the effects of other kinds of public health-related labeling. KEY WORDS: brief intervention; self-affirmation; health behavior change; implementation intentions; alcohol; labeling.

Self-Affirming Implementation Intention 3 Brief Report: Enhancing the Effectiveness of Alcohol Warning Labels With a Self- Affirming Implementation Intention Excess alcohol consumption extorts significant social and economic costs that are increasing despite the presence of mandatory warning labels on packaged alcoholic beverages. Research shows that people react defensively to the information on the labels meaning that their alcohol consumption remains unaffected (Andrews, 1995). According to self-affirmation theory (Steele, 1988), defensiveness arises because people are motivated to defend their global sense of self-worth, which is threatened by alcohol warning labels. However, accumulated empirical evidence demonstrates that affirming the self leads consistently to improvements in the way in which threatening health messages are processed and to increases in people s motivation to act in accordance with the message (Epton, Harris, Kane, van Koningsgruggen, & Sheeran, 2015). A self-affirming implementation intention has been developed that significantly reduces alcohol consumption (Armitage, Harris, & Arden, 2011; Armitage, Rowe, Arden, & Harris, 2014) and could be adapted for use on alcohol warning labels. The self-affirming implementation intention works on the principles that: (a) specifying the critical situation feeling threatened or anxious increases the salience of that critical situation when it is aroused by an alcohol warning label, and (b) linking feeling threatened or anxious to an appropriate affirming response (e.g., thinking about the things that are important to me ) ensures that the affirming response is triggered automatically (e.g., Gollwitzer, 1993). Two studies to date show that when adults (Armitage et al., 2011) and adolescents (Armitage et al., 2014) are asked to form self-affirming implementation intentions, subsequent alcohol consumption is significantly reduced. Of particular relevance in the present context is that significant reductions in alcohol consumption occurred even when the self-affirming implementation intention was not copied out in full (as per the instructions),

Self-Affirming Implementation Intention 4 and was instead ticked or circled (Armitage et al., 2011; Armitage et al., 2014). The implication is that even minimal processing of self-affirming implementation intentions, such as might occur when reading the label on a wine bottle, might reduce alcohol consumption. The principal aim of the present research is to see whether standard warning information can be augmented with a self-affirming implementation intention to bring about reduced alcohol consumption. A second aim was to address limitations in Armitage et al. s (2011, 2014) operationalization of message acceptance and motivation as potential mediators of the effects of self-affirming on alcohol consumption. Two studies were designed to test the hypotheses that: (a) alcohol warning labels augmented with a self-affirming implementation intention would significantly decrease subsequent alcohol consumption, and (b) any effect of self-affirmation on alcohol consumption would be mediated by greater message acceptance and increased motivation. Method Participants Regular wine drinkers were invited to take part in a study on alcohol consumption. Participants were recruited from University campuses and via advertisements placed on student PC screensavers and in staff e-newsletters, and made appointments to attend the laboratory via e-mail. Eighty-five agreed to participate in Study 1 (Table 1) and fifty-eight people agreed to participate in Study 2 (Table 2). Participants were paid 5 (circa US$7.50) in high street vouchers (Study 1) or received course credit (Study 2). The study received full ethical approval from the appropriate University research ethics committee. Assuming.80 power and alpha =.05 we required at least 58 participants in total at follow-up to test repeated measures differences between intervention and control groups based on the average effect size of implementation intentions, namely, d = 0.65 (Gollwitzer & Sheeran, 2006). Participants were randomly allocated using online randomization software to one of the two

Self-Affirming Implementation Intention 5 conditions. Participants were blind with respect to condition. Design and Procedure On arrival at the laboratory, participants were provided with information about the study, reminded of their right to withdraw, and asked to sign a consent form. Participants were then asked to complete a pre-manipulation questionnaire. On completion of the premanipulation questionnaire participants were led to an adjacent room in which there was a set of written instructions, a wine bottle and four empty wine glasses. Participants were presented with a standard 750ml wine bottle that appeared to contain white wine and instructed to read the labels on the bottle. In fact, the bottle was filled with water colored with three drops of yellow food coloring to resemble a light colored white wine (e.g., Pinot Grigio). The labels on the back of the wine bottles included standard UK government information about alcohol intake; the experimental label additionally included a selfaffirming implementation intention: If I feel threatened or anxious, then I will think about the things that are important to me (supplemental material; Armitage et al., 2011, 2014). Participants were instructed to pour what they thought would be a safe amount to drink in a single session into one (or more) of four empty wine glasses. The wine glasses and bottles were weighed before and after the experiment. Given that 1g of water equals 1ml of water and that there are 9 units of alcohol in a 750ml bottle of wine (12% alcohol by volume), then: Units poured = weight of water (g) x (9/750). The correlation between the amount poured into the glasses and the amount remaining in the bottle was, r = 0.98, p <.001, and so the number of units poured into the glass(es) was used in subsequent analyses. After the task participants completed the post-manipulation questionnaire. Following completion of the post-manipulation questionnaire, participants were invited to provide contact details. All participants chose to be contacted by e-mail and were sent an online questionnaire one month post-manipulation. Contact details were kept separate from

Self-Affirming Implementation Intention 6 participants anonymized data and all participants were successfully contacted. Measures Pre-manipulation questionnaires measured age, gender, ethnicity, alcohol consumption, and motivation. Alcohol consumption was measured using an adapted version of the timeline follow-back technique (Sobell & Sobell, 1992). Behavioral intention was measured with three items, including: I intend to drink within [safe levels, Study 1]/[government recommended levels, Study 2] definitely do not (1)-definitely do (7). Internal reliability was high in both Study 1 ( =.94) and Study 2 ( =.81). Self-efficacy was measured with three items, including: How confident are you that you will be able to drink within [safe levels, Study 1]/[government recommended levels, Study 2]? not very confident (1)-very confident (7). Cronbach s indicated high internal reliability in both Study 1 ( =.88) and Study 2 ( =.86). Post-manipulation questionnaires assessed message acceptance. In Study 1, message derogation (Witte, 1994) was measured with four items, e.g., The information on the alcohol label was exaggerated strongly disagree (1)-strongly agree (7), and anger (Dillard & Peck, 2001) also consisted of four items, e.g., The information on the alcohol label made me feel angry not at all (1)-very much (7). Cronbach s indicated high internal reliability ( s =.88 and.95, respectively). In Study 2, message acceptance was operationalized in terms of perceived expertise (2 items e.g., expert not at all [1]-very [7], =.84) and perceived credibility (3 items e.g., reliable not at all [1]-very [7], =.74; Wu & Shaffer, 1987); and message utility (4 items e.g., useful not at all [1]-very [7], =.71) and message satisfaction (6 items e.g., interesting not at all [1]-very [7], =.54, Moon & Nass, 1996). Follow-up questionnaires were administered online one month post-intervention and included repeat measures of alcohol consumption, behavioral intention ( Study 1 =.89; Study 2 =.87) and self-efficacy ( Study 1 =.85; Study 2 =.88).

Self-Affirming Implementation Intention 7 Results MANOVA and chi-square were used to test whether the intervention and control groups were equivalent at baseline. All the multivariate and univariate tests were nonsignificant (Tables 1 and 2), showing equivalence between groups at baseline in terms of age, gender, ethnicity, behavioral intention, self-efficacy, and alcohol consumption. MANOVA was used to test whether intervention and control groups differed postmanipulation in terms of the amount of wine poured and message acceptance. In both studies, there were no significant main effects of condition, ps >.28; no significant main effects of gender, ps >.28; and no significant condition x gender interactions, ps >.28. MANCOVA was used to test whether intervention and control groups differed in behavioral intention, self-efficacy and the amount of alcohol consumed at follow-up controlling for baseline measures of each. There was a significant multivariate main effect of condition in both Study 1, F(3, 71) = 3.69, p =.02, 2 p =.13, and Study 2, F(3, 49) = 3.74, p =.02, 2 p =.19. However, there were no significant main effects of gender, F Study 1 (3, 71) = 1.07, p =.37, 2 p =.04; F Study 2 (3, 49) = 1.15, p =.34, 2 p =.07, and no condition x gender interactions, F Study 1 (3, 71) = 1.64, p =.19, 2 p =.06; F Study 2 (3, 49) = 1.02, p =.39, 2 p =.06. Scrutiny of the univariate tests revealed no significant differences in behavioral intention, F Study 1 (1, 73) = 0.78, p =.38, 2 p =.01; F Study 2 (1, 51) = 1.76, p =.19, 2 p =.30; and self-efficacy, F Study 1 (1, 73) = 0.29, p =.59, 2 2 p =.004; F Study 2 (1, 51) = 1.51, p =.22, p =.03, at follow-up in either study. However, participants exposed to the self-affirming label were consuming significantly fewer units of alcohol at follow-up than those exposed to the standard label in both Study 1, F(1, 73) = 8.86, p <.01, 2 p =.11, d = 0.70 and in Study 2, F(1, 51) = 10.59, p <.01, 2 p =.17, d = 0.91. Discussion We adopted a novel approach by adding a brief statement based on self-affirmation

Self-Affirming Implementation Intention 8 theory (Steele, 1988) to alcohol warning labels. In two studies, we showed that engaging in a wine pouring task and being exposed to a self-affirming implementation intention led to significant decreases in alcohol consumption at follow-up. Given the brevity of the intervention and the multiple domains in which it might be deployed (e.g., cigarette packaging), the present findings are encouraging and warrant further investigation. Consistent with the broader literature on self-affirmation theory (e.g., Epton et al., 2015) and the self-affirming implementation intention (Armitage et al., 2011; 2014), we were unable to identify significant mediators of the observed effects, which is potentially problematic for self-affirmation theory (Steele, 1988). One possible avenue for further research might be to consider whether implicit as opposed to explicit measures mediate the effects of self-affirming. Another possible avenue is to consider whether motivation provides an adequate explanation of the effects of self-affirming; one possibility is that selfaffirming might prompt self-regulatory mechanisms, as opposed to behavioral intention and self-efficacy. Consistent with this view, recent research suggests that the effects of implementation intentions on behavior change can at least partly be explained by changes in self-monitoring (Armitage, in press). Although the present research takes the literature on self-affirmation forward in some important respects, it is important to take note of some potential limitations. First, participants were invited to take part in a laboratory task that potentially lacked ecological validity when compared with a regular shopping experience and it would be valuable to test the effects of self-affirming implementation intentions in a more naturalistic setting. Nevertheless, it is notable that participants in both the intervention and control conditions poured out similar amounts of wine, which implies that the wine pouring task might not be necessary to exert the observed effects. Second, although the effects of the manipulation persisted beyond the experimental session, it would be valuable to see whether the effects are

Self-Affirming Implementation Intention 9 sustained over a period of time longer than a month. It is worth noting, however, that greater ecological validity and longer follow-up periods may end up being confounded with repeated exposures to the intervention. Third, alcohol consumption was measured using self-report and it would be valuable to obtain reliable and valid objective data about the main outcome measure. However, we have confidence in the reliability and validity of our dependent variable because, when used in similar situations to the present study, self-reports have been shown to agree 97.1% with biological measures (for a discussion, see Armitage et al., 2011; 2014). Fourth, we did not include a manipulation check and in future research it would be valuable to ascertain whether participants reported giving increased thought to personallysalient goals/outcomes as a result of forming a self-affirming implementation intention. Two studies showed that standard information augmented with a self-affirming implementation intention was capable of significantly reducing alcohol consumption. Although it is not yet clear which variables mediate the observed effects, the present research demonstrates potential for deploying a simple intervention with considerable public health reach both in reducing alcohol consumption specifically and health behaviors more generally.

Self-Affirming Implementation Intention 10 References Andrews, J. C. (1995). The effectiveness of alcohol warning labels - a review and extension. American Behavioral Scientist, 38, 622-632. doi: 10.1177/0002764295038004011 Armitage, C. J. (in press). Field experiment of a very brief worksite intervention to improve nutrition among health care workers. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. Armitage, C. J., Harris, P. R., & Arden, M. A. (2011). Evidence that self-affirmation reduces alcohol consumption. Health Psychology, 30, 633-641. Armitage, C. J., Rowe, R., Arden, M. A., & Harris, P. R. (2014). A brief psychological intervention that reduces adolescent alcohol consumption. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82, 546-550. doi: 10.1037/a0035802 Dillard, J. P., & Peck, E. (2001). Persuasion and the structure of affect: Dual systems and discrete emotions as models. Human Communication Research, 27, 38 68. Epton, T., Harris, P. R., Kane, R., van Koningsbruggen, G. M., & Sheeran, P. (2015). The impact of self-affirmation on health-behavior change: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology, 34, 187-196. doi: 10.1037/hea0000116 Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 69-119. doi: 10.1013/S0065-2601 Moon, Y. M., & Nass, C. (1996). How real are computer personalities? Communication Research, 23, 651-674. doi: 10.1177/009365096023006002 Sobell, L. C., & Sobell, M. B. (1992). Timeline follow-back. In R. Z. Litten & J. P. Allen (Eds.), Measuring alcohol consumption (pp. 41-72). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press. Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 261-302). New York, NY: AP. Wu, C. H., & Shaffer, D. R. (1987). Susceptibility to persuasive appeals as a function of source credibility and prior experience with the attitude object. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 677-688. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.52.4.677

Self-Affirming Implementation Intention 11 Table 1 Effect of the Self-Affirming Implementation Intention (Study 1) Intervention, n = 42 Control, n = 43 M SD M SD p Pre-Manipulation Age (years) a 23.93 3.55 23.44 3.67.54 Alcohol Consumption (units/week) a 24.05 16.87 18.60 11.97.09 Behavioral Intention a 4.94 1.48 4.68 1.53.42 Self-Efficacy a 5.56 1.19 5.64 1.33.75 n % n % Gender b.72 Women 27 64.29 26 60.47 Men 15 35.71 17 39.53 Ethnicity b.17 Asian 2 4.76 3 6.98 Black 1 2.38 0 0.00 Mixed Race 0 0.00 2 4.65 White 39 92.86 38 88.37 M SD M SD Post-Manipulation Units Poured a 4.02 1.48 4.28 1.44.72 Message Derogation a 3.08 1.21 3.25 1.09.73 Anger a 1.48 1.11 1.62 0.99.57 Follow-Up Alcohol Consumption (units/week) c 13.71 10.29 21.85 16.42 <.01 Behavioral Intention c 4.38 1.29 4.56 1.25.38 Self-Efficacy c 5.39 0.99 5.49 1.19.59 Note. a p values are associated with the univariate F tests testing for differences in values between intervention and control conditions. b p values are associated with the chi-square tests for differences in between intervention and control conditions. c M values are raw and not adjusted for baseline; p values are associated with the univariate F tests testing for differences in values at follow-up between intervention and control conditions controlling for baseline values.

Self-Affirming Implementation Intention 12 Table 2 Effect of the Self-Affirming Implementation Intention (Study 2) Intervention, n = 29 Control, n = 29 M SD M SD p Pre-Manipulation Age (years) a 19.07 1.33 19.69 0.47.10 Alcohol Consumption (units/week) a 16.73 12.55 18.78 14.44.57 Behavioral Intention a 3.85 1.53 3.59 1.71.54 Self-Efficacy a 5.85 1.13 5.41 1.28.17 n % n % Gender b 1.00 Women 22 75.86 22 75.86 Men 7 24.14 7 24.14 Ethnicity b.52 Asian 7 24.14 5 17.24 White 22 75.86 24 82.76 M SD M SD Post-Manipulation Units Poured a 3.59 1.75 3.70 1.69.82 Source Expertise a 4.98 1.22 5.26 0.86.32 Source Credibility a 4.86 1.07 5.19 0.87.20 Message Utility a 4.64 1.05 4.71 0.91.79 Message Satisfaction a 3.81 0.66 3.71 0.78.59 Follow-Up Alcohol Consumption (units/week) c 13.87 9.61 20.76 17.93 <.01 Behavioral Intention c 4.54 1.33 3.87 1.68.19 Self-Efficacy c 5.74 1.21 5.09 1.38.22 Note. a p values are associated with the univariate F tests testing for differences in values between intervention and control conditions. b p values are associated with the chi-square tests for differences in between intervention and control conditions. c M values are raw and not adjusted for baseline; p values are associated with the univariate F tests testing for differences in values at follow-up between intervention and control conditions controlling for baseline values.