The effect of education and occupational complexity on rate of cognitive decline in Alzheimer s patients

Similar documents
Validity of Family History for the Diagnosis of Dementia Among Siblings of Patients With Late-onset Alzheimer s Disease

Estimating the Validity of the Korean Version of Expanded Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) Scale

The Reliability and Validity of the Korean Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (K-IADL)

Cognitive Reserve and the Relationship Between Depressive Symptoms and Awareness of Deficits in Dementia

The Pleasant Events Schedule-AD: Psychometric Properties and Relationship to Depression and Cognition in Alzheimer's Disease Patients 1

Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument, Chinese Version 2.0 (CASI C-2.0): Administration and Clinical Application

Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised*

UDS Progress Report. -Standardization and Training Meeting 11/18/05, Chicago. -Data Managers Meeting 1/20/06, Chicago

Baseline Characteristics of Patients Attending the Memory Clinic Serving the South Shore of Boston

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Metab Brain Dis. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 October 24.

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Change in Cognitive Function in Older Persons From a Community Population

MODIFYING daily activities may be among the simplest

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Comparison of the Short Test of Mental Status and the Mini-Mental State Examination in Mild Cognitive Impairment

Clinical Study Depressive Symptom Clusters and Neuropsychological Performance in Mild Alzheimer s and Cognitively Normal Elderly

Evaluation of Preventive Care Program for Cognitive Function Decline among Community-dwelling Frail Elderly People A Pilot Study

Hallucinations, delusions, and cognitive decline in Alzheimer s disease

Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised: Norms for Elderly African Americans

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Longitudinal Assessment of Patient Dependence in Alzheimer Disease

CHAPTER 2 CRITERION VALIDITY OF AN ATTENTION- DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER (ADHD) SCREENING LIST FOR SCREENING ADHD IN OLDER ADULTS AGED YEARS

Chapter 7. Depression and cognitive impairment in old age: what comes first?

Diversity and Dementia

A semantic verbal fluency test for English- and Spanish-speaking older Mexican-Americans

Outline. Minority Issues in Aging Research. The Role of Research in the Clinical Setting. Why Participate in Research

Treatment of AD with Stabilized Oral NADH: Preliminary Findings

Ann Acad Med Singapore 2013;42:315-9 Key words: Cognitive impairment, Dementia, SPMSQ, Validation

Online Cognitive Training Improves Cognitive Performance.

Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia

THERE has been considerable interest in the ability to

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Staging Dementia Using Clinical Dementia Rating Scale Sum of Boxes Scores

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen

NEUROPSYCHOMETRIC TESTS

IMPACT OF CULTURE & EDUCATION ON COGNITIVE AGING

Anosognosia, or loss of insight into one s cognitive

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION

Key words Machine Learning, Clinical Dementia Rating Scale, dementia severity, dementia staging, Alzheimer s disease.

DEMENTIA DUE TO ALZHEImer

Ψ Psychology Press Taylor & Francis Group

K. Kahle-Wrobleski 1, J.S. Andrews 1, M. Belger 2, S. Gauthier 3, Y. Stern 4, D.M. Rentz 5, D. Galasko 6

Modelling Mini Mental State Examination changes in Alzheimer s disease

The combination of cognitive testing and an informant questionnaire in screening for dementia

Four Tissue Segmentation in ADNI II

ORIGINAL ARTICLE Neuroscience INTRODUCTION MATERIALS AND METHODS

Awareness of Deficit in Alzheimer's Disease: Relation to Caregiver Burden 1

Partners in Care: A Model of Social Work in Primary Care

Comparison of clock drawing with Mini Mental State Examination as a screening test in elderly acute hospital admissions

Test Assessment Description Ref. Global Deterioration Rating Scale Dementia severity Rating scale of dementia stages (2) (4) delayed recognition

Behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia characteristic of mild Alzheimer patients

Recognition of Alzheimer s Disease: the 7 Minute Screen

MEASURING EVERYDAY COGNITIVE FUNCTIONING

CONSIDERATIONS WHEN ENGAGING THE COMMUNITY IN BRAIN HEALTH: Lessons Learned from Memory Screening and Memory Training

A COMPARISON OF IMPUTATION METHODS FOR MISSING DATA IN A MULTI-CENTER RANDOMIZED CLINICAL TRIAL: THE IMPACT STUDY

ONLINE DATA SUPPLEMENT - ASTHMA INTERVENTION PROGRAM PREVENTS READMISSIONS IN HIGH HEALTHCARE UTILIZERS

CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE

The course of neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia. Part II: relationships among behavioural sub-syndromes and the influence of clinical variables

Longitudinal Study of Quality of Life in People with Advanced Alzheimer s Disease

Application of a Growth Curve Approach to Modeling the Progression of Alzheimer's Disease

Title. CitationAustralasian Journal on Ageing, 31(3): Issue Date Doc URL. Rights. Type. File Information

Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the American Statistical Association, August 5-9, 2001

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Detecting Dementia With the Mini-Mental State Examination in Highly Educated Individuals

Supplementary Online Content

Why would caregivers not want to treat their relative's Alzheimer's disease?

Department of Neurology, Soonchunhyang University Bucheon Hospital, Bucheon, Korea

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Diagnostic Validity of the Dementia Questionnaire for Alzheimer Disease

SUPPLEMENTARY APPENDIX

Normative data for a brief neuropsychological battery administered to English- and Spanish-speaking community-dwelling elders

2018 Limited English Proficiency Plan for the City of West Palm Beach

Gerardo Machnicki 1, Ricardo F. Allegri 1,2 *, Carol Dillon 1, Cecilia M. Serrano 1,2 and Fernando E Taragano 2 SUMMARY INTRODUCTION

The Cross-Cultural Neuropsychological Test Battery (CCNB): Effects of Age, Education, Ethnicity, and Cognitive Status on Performance

THE ROLE OF ACTIVITIES OF DAILY LIVING IN THE MCI SYNDROME

PREVALENCE AND CORRELATES OF ANXIETY IN ALZHEIMER S DISEASE

The Zarit Burden Interview: A New Short Version and Screening Version

Avoidant Coping Moderates the Association between Anxiety and Physical Functioning in Patients with Chronic Heart Failure

The Development and Validation of Korean Dementia Screening Questionnaire (KDSQ)

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

Pain Assessment in Elderly Patients with Severe Dementia

Anxiety, Depression, and Dementia/Alzheimer Disease: What are the Links?

Neuropsychological detection and characterization of preclinical Alzheimer s disease

Risk Factors for Vascular Dementia: A Hospital-Based Study in Taiwan

Department of Psychology, Sungkyunkwan University, Seoul, Korea

Recognizing Dementia can be Tricky

Alzheimer disease is one of the leading causes of death. Article

Functional assessment scales in detecting dementia

DATA CORE MEETING. Observational studies in dementia and the MELODEM initiative. V. Shane Pankratz, Ph.D.

Cognitive Decline in Patients with Alzheimer's Disease, Vascular Dementia and Senile Dementia of Lewy Body Type

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Conscientiousness and the Incidence of Alzheimer Disease and Mild Cognitive Impairment

Department of Neuropsychiatry, Kangwon National University Hospital, Chunchon, Kangwon-do, Korea. 2

Neuropsychological Correlates of Performance Based Functional Status in Elder Adult Protective Services Referrals for Capacity Assessments

Exploration of a weighed cognitive composite score for measuring decline in amnestic MCI

NEXT-Link DEMENTIA. A network of Danish memory clinics YOUR CLINICAL RESEARCH PARTNER WITHIN ALZHEIMER S DISEASE AND OTHER DEMENTIA DISEASES.

I n the past three decades various cognitive screening

.. Mini-Mental State Examination MMSE TPQ

MODIFIED INFORMANT QUESTIONNAIRE ON COGNITIVE DECLINE IN THE ELDERLY (IQCODE) AS A SCREENING TEST FOR DEMENTIA FOR THAI ELDERLY

Prevalence and Impact of Medical Comorbidity in Alzheimer s Disease

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Telephone-Based Identification of Mild Cognitive Impairment and Dementia in a Multicultural Cohort

Preliminary examination of cognitive reserve theory in closed head injury

Neuropsychological test performance in African-American* and white patients with Alzheimer s disease

TITLE: A Data-Driven Approach to Patient Risk Stratification for Acute Respiratory Distress Syndrome (ARDS)

Evidence-Based Interventions to Improve Caregiver and Patient Outcomes in Dementia

A Short Test of Mental Status: Description and Preliminary Results

Transcription:

Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society (2006), 12, 147 152. Copyright 2006 INS. Published by Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. DOI: 10.10170S1355617706060206 BRIEF COMMUNICATION The effect of education and occupational complexity on rate of cognitive decline in Alzheimer s patients ROSS ANDEL, 1 CHERYL VIGEN, 2 WENDY J. MACK, 2 LINDA J. CLARK, 3 and MARGARET GATZ, 4 1 School of Aging Studies, University of South Florida, Tampa, Florida 2 Department of Preventive Medicine, University of Southern California Keck School of Medicine, Los Angeles, CA 3 Department of Neurology, University of Southern California Keck School of Medicine, Los Angeles, CA 4 Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA (Received April 15, 2004; Final Revision September 12, 2005; Accepted September 19, 2005) Abstract We explored the effect of education and occupational complexity on the rate of cognitive decline (as measured by the Mini-Mental State Examination) in 171 patients with a confirmed Alzheimer s disease (AD) diagnosis. was measured as substantive complexity of work and complexity of work with data, people, and things. Average lifetime occupational complexity was calculated based on years at each occupation. Participants were followed for an average of 2.5 years and 3.7 visits. In multivariate mixed-effects models, high education, high substantive complexity, and high complexity of work with data and people predicted faster rates of cognitive decline, controlling for age, gender, native language, dementia severity, and entry into the analyses at initial versus follow-up testing. These results provide support for the concept of cognitive reserve according to which greater reserve may postpone clinical onset of AD but also accelerate cognitive decline after the onset. (JINS, 2006, 12, 147 152.) Keywords: Alzheimer s disease, of work, Lifetime occupation, Mini-Mental State Examination, Cognitive reserve, Mixed-effects models INTRODUCTION The concept of cognitive reserve has been proposed as an explanation for why Alzheimer s disease (AD) diagnosis may be postponed in older adults with more education or higher-status occupations (Stern et al., 1994). Cognitive reserve has been defined as the amount of brain damage an individual can tolerate before reaching a clinical threshold for impairment (Katzman, 1993; Stern, 2002). Thus, individuals with greater reserve are hypothesized to sustain more AD-related neuronal damage before onset of symptoms and clinical diagnosis. Lending support to this hypothesis, two brain imaging studies found that AD patients with more demanding lifetime occupations (Stern et al., 1995) and higher education (Stern et al., 1992) had greater extent of Reprint requests to: Ross Andel, Ph.D., School of Aging Studies, University of South Florida, 4202 E. Fowler Ave., Tampa, FL 33620. Telephone: (813) 974-9743. Fax: (813) 974-9754. E-mail: randel@ cas.usf.edu brain pathology when dementia severity was controlled. Bennett and colleagues (2003) found a similar effect for education using brain autopsies. Although onset may initially be postponed in older adults with greater reserve, compensation for deficits may be more difficult after AD diagnosis due to greater brain pathology, leading to a faster rate of cognitive decline. Higher education (Teri et al., 1995; Wilson et al., 2004) and high occupational status (Stern et al., 1999) have been associated with more rapid cognitive decline in AD. However, others did not find the same effect for education (Le Carret et al., 2005) and occupational status (Fritsch et al., 2002). We explored the effect of education and lifetime levels of occupational complexity measured by substantive complexity of work and complexity of work with data, people, and things on rate of decline on the Mini-Mental State Examination (Folstein et al., 1975) in patients with AD. Measures of occupational complexity have not previously been evaluated in relation to cognitive decline in AD patients. 147

148 R. Andel et al. METHOD Participants Participants were enrolled in a longitudinal study of aging and dementia at the University of Southern California Alzheimer Disease Research Center. Enrolling entailed commitment to semiannual retesting. On enrollment, each participant received a neurological assessment, medical evaluation, and neuropsychological testing. Informed consent was obtained from all participants or their caregivers using procedures approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Southern California. AD was diagnosed according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders, 3rd edition revised (DSM-IIIR) criteria for dementia, and National Institute of Neurologic and Communicative Disorders and Stroke0AD and Related Disorders Association (NINCDS0ADRDA) criteria for AD (McKhann et al., 1984). Dementia severity was graded using the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (CDR; Burke et al., 1988), a 5-point scale used to discriminate individuals without dementia (0), and with questionable (0.5), mild (1), moderate (2), and severe (3) dementia. Criteria for inclusion in the analyses were CDR of 1 or greater at baseline or at retesting, clinical diagnosis of AD, at least one follow-up testing after AD diagnosis, baseline MMSE scores greater than 5 to avoid floor effects on retesting, and information about education and occupation obtained either from a knowledgeable informant or from the participant before AD diagnosis. In all, 171 AD patients met these criteria; 144 were diagnosed with AD at their baseline assessment, while 27 were diagnosed with AD at retesting. For these 27, this retest occasion served as the baseline for the present analyses. These 171 patients were identified from a total of 974 individuals who were tested as part of the longitudinal study. Of these 974, 509 were diagnosed with AD at initial or follow-up testing. Among those with AD, 10 had only selfreported information collected when or after AD diagnosis was established, 160 had baseline scores of 5 or lower on the MMSE, and 14 died before the first follow-up testing could be scheduled. Another 88 had missing education and0or occupational data. Of the remaining 237 AD patients, 171 (72%) had at least one follow-up testing. During the first testing with an AD diagnosis, average MMSE score was 17.7 (SD 5 5.5), 123 patients received a CDR score of 1, 43 received a score of 2, and 5 received a score of 3. The design called for retesting subjects every 6 months if demented. In this study, the average number of follow-ups after AD diagnosis was 3.7 (SD 5 3.5) over an average of 2.5 years (SD 5 1.9 years). Therefore, the actual average follow-up interval was 8.1 months. Delays in followups were mostly due to scheduling issues and illness. Follow-up for cognitive testing was ended due to death in 15 participants. In 146 participants, cognitive test follow-up was ended due to another medical reason or inability to complete a cognitive test battery. Ten participants were still active in the longitudinal cognitive testing. The sample was 61% Caucasian, 27% Hispanic, 11% African American, and 1% Asian0Pacific Islander. Information about ethnicity was missing for one participant. Women composed 57% of the sample; 78% were English-speaking and 22% were Spanish-speaking. Previously, the Spanish versions of the neuropsychological battery (Taussig et al., 1992) and the MMSE (Taussig et al., 1996) showed no measurable cultural bias and reliably discriminated AD cases from normal controls. Measures Rate of cognitive decline was measured with the Mini- Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975), a 30-point test of global cognition that includes orientation to time and place, language, memory, and visuomotor performance. The main independent variables were education and complexity of occupation. Information about education, lifetime occupational history, and demographics was obtained from participants when they were cognitively intact (9%), or otherwise from a spouse (38%), son0daughter (22%), brother0sister (4%), or another, unspecified person (27%). For participants who gave self-reports, average interval between time of report and AD diagnosis was 6.0 years. Occupations were coded by using categories from the 1970 U. S. Census. To measure complexity, we used scores for substantive complexity of work, and for complexity of work with data, people, and things calculated for the 1970 U. S. Census occupational categories by Roos and Treiman (1980) based on ratings in the Dictionary of Occupational Titles (see Miller et al., 1980). Once occupation was coded with its 1970 U. S. Census category, occupational complexity ratings are available in a directory. For the substantive complexity factor, scores range from 0 to 10. Interpretation of scales for complexity of work with data, people, and things is presented in the Appendix. These measures are intended to reflect the levels of complexity at which a worker in a particular occupation functions in relation to data, people, and things (Miller et al., 1980, p. 19). The scores used in our analyses reflect the average complexity of occupations held by each participant throughout adulthood. We multiplied the complexity score for each reported occupation by years spent in that occupation, summed the values across all occupations, and divided the total sum by the total number of years spent in all occupations. Years spent as a homemaker were not included in these calculations because complexity ratings for this category were not available. Substantive complexity was correlated with complexity of work with data (r 5.94, p,.01) and people (r 5.71, p,.01), and complexity of work with data was correlated with complexity of work with people (r 5.66, p,.01). of work with people was negatively correlated with complexity of work with things (r 52.30, p,.01). Substantive complexity, complexity of work with data, and

Cognitive decline in Alzheimer s patients 149 complexity of work with people were correlated with education (r 5.61,.57,.57, respectively, p,.01). Analysis Education and measures of occupational complexity were dichotomized into high (1) and low (0) categories. Education was classified as 12 years or less versus more than 12 years. Substantive complexity was dichotomized at the median, as scores greater than or equal to 4.9 versus less than 4.9. of work with data, people, and things were dichotomized along complexity levels (see the Appendix) as follows: of work with data was classified as Analyzing or more complex work versus Compiling or less complex work, complexity of work with people as Speaking0Signaling or more complex work versus Serving or Taking Instructions, and complexity of work with things as Manipulating or more complex work versus Tending or less complex work. We used mixed-effects models in the SAS procedure MIXED (SAS Institute, 2003) to test whether average rate of decline on MMSE was predicted by levels of education or occupational complexity. Education and each complexity measure were entered into separate models. A mixedeffects model yields estimates of overall fixed effects, or average effects for the group, as well as random effects that reflect individual deviation from the fixed effect. Random effects were calculated using an unstructured covariance matrix and included baseline MMSE (intercept) and rate of decline (slope). The procedure accounts for intraindividual correlation among MMSE scores over time. All models controlled for age (centered at the mean), gender, native language (English0Spanish), AD diagnosis at initial testing vs. at retesting, and dementia severity at baseline (CDR score). Models with occupational complexity also controlled for years of education. RESULTS Out of 237 AD patients eligible to participate in this study, 66 did not have follow-up testing. Logistic regression analyses indicated that the two groups were comparable in age, gender, baseline CDR scores, substantive complexity, and complexity of work with data and people ( p..05). However, AD patients without follow-up were more likely to be Spanish-speaking, were less educated, had lower baseline MMSE scores, and had held occupations with higher complexity of work with things ( p,.05). Descriptive information for the sample is presented in Table 1. There were more men than women with high education and with high complexity of work with data ( p,.05). Chi-square tests also indicated that patients who were diagnosed with AD at retesting were more likely than those diagnosed with AD at initial testing to be in the high groups for education, substantive complexity, and complexity of work with people ( p,.05). Those who were English- Table 1. Descriptive statistics: Total sample by education and occupational complexity categories with things with people with data Substantive complexity Total sample High Low High Low High Low High Low High Low Education Total n 171 89 82 87 84 77 94 112 59 77 94 Women, n 97 39 58 42 55 46 66 62 35 45 52 English speaking, n 134 80 54 82 52 74 60 101 33 58 76 Age, mean (SD) 76.5 (8.7) 73.7 (8.6) 76.4 (8.7) 77.1 (8.7) 76.0 (8.8) 76.4 (8.1) 76.6 (9.2) 77.6 (8.5) 74.6 (8.8) 76.6 (8.8) 76.5 (8.7) Years of education, mean (SD) 12.1 (4.7) 15.6 (1.5) 8.2 (3.9) 14.7 (2.5) 9.3 (4.9) 14.9 (2.4) 9.7 (4.9) 13.8 (3.8) 8.8 (4.6) 11.3 (4.8) 12.7 (4.5) Baseline MMSE, mean (SD) 17.7 (5.5) 18.5 (5.7) 16.9 (5.3) 18.2 (5.9) 17.2 (5.1) 18.3 (5.6) 17.2 (5.4) 18.2 (5.6) 16.9 (5.4) 18.3 (5.3) 17.2 (5.7) Baseline Clinical Dementia Rating, n Mild (1) 123 61 62 62 62 53 70 78 45 61 62 Moderate (2) 43 26 17 23 20 23 20 31 12 13 30 Severe (3) 6 2 3 3 2 1 4 3 2 3 2 No. of follow-ups after AD diagnosis, mean (SD) 3.7 (3.5) 4.0 (3.3) 3.4 (3.6) 3.7 (2.9) 3.7 (4.0) 3.7 (2.9) 3.7 (3.9) 3.6 (2.9) 3.9 (4.4) 3.8 (3.4) 3.8 (3.4) Years of follow-up after AD diagnosis, mean (SD) 2.5 (1.9) 2.5 (1.8) 2.5 (2.0) 2.4 (1.6) 2.7 (2.1) 2.4 (1.6) 2.6 (2.1) 2.4 (1.6) 2.8 (2.3) 2.5 (1.9) 2.5 (1.8)

150 R. Andel et al. Table 2. Model of longitudinal change in MMSE: Fixed and random effect estimates for the intercept, time (in years), and dichotomized predictor variables Education Substantive complexity with data with people with things Predictor variable Estimate SE Estimate SE Estimate SE Estimate SE Estimate SE Fixed effects Intercept 23.00*** 1.66 22.67*** 1.79 22.75*** 1.79 22.76*** 1.79 21.82*** 1.84 Time 22.41*** 0.31 22.39*** 0.30 22.37*** 0.28 22.56*** 0.37 23.10*** 0.30 Predictor 0.29 0.78 20.25 0.83 20.65 0.83 20.34 0.85 1.28 0.69 Time * predictor 21.46** 0.42 21.51** 0.41 21.75*** 0.41 20.98** 0.45 20.26 0.44 Random effects Variance of intercept 15.59* 2.17 15.75*** 2.19 15.65*** 2.18 15.79*** 2.20 15.46*** 2.16 Variance of time 4.37*** 0.90 3.99*** 0.88 4.01*** 0.86 4.55*** 0.94 4.79*** 0.97 Covariance of intercept, time 3.51*** 1.01 3.37*** 0.99 3.29*** 0.98 3.31*** 1.04 3.54** 1.05 Residual variance 6.74 6.82 6.76 6.76 6.75 22 log likelihood 4428.7 4430.1 4425.4 4438.1 4439.2 Note. ***p,.001; **p,.01; *p,.05. All models controlled for baseline Mini-Mental State Examination, age, gender, native language, mode of entry into the study (at initial testing vs. at retesting), and dementia severity (Clinical Dementia Rating score at baseline). Models with complexity measures also controlled for years of education. Residual variance shows residual error after accounting for fixed and random effects; 22 log likelihood shows model fit. speaking were more likely than those who were Spanishspeaking to be in the high groups for education, substantive complexity, and complexity of work with data and people ( p,.05). There were no differences in clinical dementia severity between participants with low and high education or complexity scores ( p..05), however, dementia severity was greater for those with lower initial MMSE score (r 52.55, p,.01). Independent sample t-tests yielded no significant differences in baseline MMSE between participants with low and high education or complexity scores, or between men and women ( p..05). Table 2 displays the results of mixed-effects analyses. The fixed effect for the intercept represents the average baseline MMSE score for those in the lower category on the predictor variable, adjusted for all variables in the model. In each model there was an average annual rate of decline in MMSE of approximately 2.5 points for every year elapsed since the initial MMSE test for participants in the lower education or occupational complexity category. The lack of statistical significance for the fixed effect for the predictor in all models indicated that baseline MMSE scores were not different between participants with high versus low scores on each predictor. The interaction of time by predictor constituted the test of the main hypothesis. The interaction yielded significant negative parameter estimates in all models except the model testing complexity of work with things. The negative parameter estimates indicated that participants with high scores on the predictor (i.e., education or occupation) declined significantly faster than their counterparts with low scores; the average difference in the rate of MMSE decline in participants in the high versus low complexity categories was between 0.98 MMSE points per year for complexity of work with people and 1.75 points per year for complexity of work with data. The significant random effects for the variance in the intercept across all models indicated that individual baseline MMSE scores varied. The significant variance for time showed significant individual differences in the rate of cognitive decline. Since individual differences in rate of decline existed in models with fixed effects for time and predictor included (controlled), it is likely that other factors besides the predictor might explain a portion of variation in individual rates of decline. Finally, the significant covariance between intercept and years of follow-up indicated that rate of decline was less steep among individuals with higher baseline MMSE. We also conducted post hoc analyses with continuous education and complexity variables. Since complexity of work with people was skewed, we log-transformed this variable. The pattern of results was similar to that obtained with dichotomous variables. Finally, complexity of work with data, people and things were entered into the same, covariate-adjusted model. Substantive complexity was not included in these models because of the high colinearity between this variable and complexity of work with data. AD patients with high complexity of work with data declined about 1.9 points per year faster on the MMSE than AD patients with low complexity of work with data ( p, 0.05), while complexity of work with people and things were not significantly related to rate of decline. DISCUSSION We examined the effects of education and complexity of occupation on rate of decline in the MMSE score among patients diagnosed with AD. We found that high education and high levels of occupational complexity were associated with faster rate of decline on MMSE. The results were adjusted for age, gender, native language, dementia sever-

Cognitive decline in Alzheimer s patients 151 ity, and entry into the study at initial (versus follow-up) testing, and for education in analyses with complexity measures. These findings provide additional support for the hypothesis that the individuals who may have greater reserve, measured by levels of education or complexity of occupation independent of education, are likely to show faster rate of cognitive decline after their reserve is depleted to the point of clinical expression of AD. Previously, faster rate of cognitive decline was found in AD patients with high education (Stern et al., 1999; Teri et al., 1995; Wilson et al., 2004) and with high occupational attainment defined as primary occupation being managerial, professional, or technical as opposed to trade, craft, or clerical (Stern et al., 1999). This was the first study to link occupational complexity associated with job titles to rate of cognitive decline in AD patients. This study has some limitations. Participants were volunteers from a university-based research center, limiting the ability to generalize the findings to the entire population. In addition, we used mostly prevalent AD cases and only one measure of cognitive ability to test rate of decline. We could not assure that the clinicians performing each neuropsychological evaluation were blinded to previous evaluations. Finally, we were unable to include complexity scores for time spent as a homemaker because such scores were not available. In conclusion, while neither education nor occupational complexity fully accounted for individual variance in the rate of decline, our findings provide further evidence that some type of reserve created through or associated with lifestyle factors, namely education and occupation, is related to an accelerated cognitive decline once clinical symptoms of AD are evident. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This study was supported by NIA grant nos. P03 AG17265 and P50 AG05142. REFERENCES Bennett, D.A., Wilson, R.S., Schneider, J.A., Evans, D.A., Mendes de Leon, C.F., Arnold, S.E., Barnes, L.L., & Bienias, J.L. (2003). Education modifies the relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive function in older persons. Neurology, 60, 1909 1915. Burke, W.J., Miller, P., Rubin, E.H., Morris, J.C., Coben, L.A., Ducheck, J., Wittels, I.G., & Berg L. (1988). Reliability of the Washington University Clinical Dementia Rating. Archives of Neurology, 45, 31 32. Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). Mini- Mental state : A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189 198. Fritsch, T., McClendon, M.J., Smyth, K.A., & Ogrocki, P.K., (2002). Effects of educational attainment and occupational status on cognitive and functional decline in persons with Alzheimertype dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14, 347 363. Katzman, R. (1993). Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer s disease. Neurology, 43, 13 20. Le Carret, N., Auriacombe, S., Letenneur, L., Bergua, V., Datrigues, J.-F., & Fabrigoule, C. (2005). Influence of education on the pattern of cognitive deterioration in AD patients: The cognitive reserve hypothesis. Brain and Cognition, 57, 120 126. McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E.M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer s disease. Neurology, 34, 939 944. Miller, A.R., Treiman, D.J., Cain, P.S., & Roos, P.A. (1980). Work, jobs, and occupations: A critical review of occupational titles. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Roos, P.A. & Treiman, D.J. (1980). DOT scales for the 1970 Census classification. In A.R. Miller, D.J. Treiman, P.S Cain, & P.A. Roos (Eds.), Work, jobs, and occupations: A critical review of occupational titles. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. SAS Institute (2003). SAS System for Microsoft Windows (Version 9) [Computer software]. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc. Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 448 460. Stern, Y., Albert, S., Tang, M.-X., & Tsai, W.-Y. (1999). Rate of memory decline in AD is related to education and occupation: Cognitive reserve? Neurology, 53, 1942 1947. Stern, Y., Alexander, G.E., Prohovnik, I., & Mayeux, R. (1992). Inverse relationship between education and parietotemporal perfusion deficit in Alzheimer s disease. Annals of Neurology, 32, 371 375. Stern, Y., Alexander, G.E., Prohovnik, I., Stricks, L., Link, B., Lennon, M.C., & Mayeux, R. (1995). Relationship between lifetime occupation and parietal flow: Implications for a reserve against Alzheimer s disease pathology. Neurology, 45, 55 60. Stern, Y., Gurland, B., Tatemichi, T.K., Tang, M.X., Wilder, D., & Mayeux, R. (1994). Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer s disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 271, 1004 1010. Taussig, I.M., Henderson, V.W., & Mack W. (1992). Spanish translation and validation of a neuropsychological battery: Performance of Spanish- and English-speaking Alzheimer s disease patients and normal comparison subjects. Clinical Gerontologist, 11, 95 108. Taussig, I.M., Mack, W.J., & Henderson, V.W. (1996). Concurrent validity of Spanish-language versions of the Mini-Mental State Examination, Mental Status Questionnaire, Information- Memory-Concentration Test, and Orientation-Memory- Concentration Test: Alzheimer s disease patients and nondemented elderly comparison subjects. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 286 298. Teri, L., McCurry, S.M., Edland, S.D., Kukull, W.A., & Larson, E.B. (1995). Cognitive decline in Alzheimer s disease: A longitudinal investigation of risk factors for accelerated decline. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences, 50A, M49 M55. Wilson, R.S., Li, Y., Aggarwal, N.T., Barnes, L.L., McCann, J.J., Gilley, D.W., & Evans, D.A. (2004). Education and the course of cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease. Neurology, 63, 1198 1202.

152 R. Andel et al. APPENDIX Description of occupational complexity levels as presented in Miller et al. (1980, p. 22) and the proportion of participants in each complexity level (N 5 171) Dimension with data % with people % with things % Function 0 Synthesizing 4 0 Mentoring 4 0 Setting up 1 1 Coordinating 21 1 Negotiating 1 1 Precision Working 10 2 Analyzing 20 2 Instructing 9 2 Operating 16 3 Compiling 21 3 Supervising 2 3 Driving0Operating 8 4 Computing 18 4 Diverting 6 4 Manipulating 10 5 Copying 10 5 Persuading 13 5 Tending 9 6 Comparing 6 6 Speaking0Signaling 30 6 Feeding0Offbearing 16 7 Serving 18 7 Handling 30 8 Taking instructions 17 Note. Lower levels represent relatively higher complexity