Recreating mental image with the aid of cognitive maps and its role in environmental perception

Similar documents
Mental Imagery. What is Imagery? What We Can Imagine 3/3/10. What is nature of the images? What is the nature of imagery for the other senses?

Navigation skill test could diagnose Alzheimer's long before memory fails 20 April 2016, by Gerry Everding

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

SECTION 1. CHAPTER 1 Environmental and Behavioral Issues. CHAPTER 2 Acoustical Principles. CHAPTER 3 Accessible Design Basics

UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society

Measuring Familiarity for Natural Environments Through Visual Images 1

Navigating Indoor with Maps: Representations and Processes

Time perception, cognitive correlates, age and emotions

Layout of reference points during navigation: Effects of a central reference point as anchor

68.1. Ability and intelligibility: Wayfinding and environmental cognition in the designed environment

Losing one's way in a digital world: VR studies on navigation

Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning

Accounting for cognitive and perceptual biases in computational models of human movement

Spatial Cognition: Learning what has been learnt from cognitive maps. Dr Tim Jones Controversies in Cognition 2006

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 156 ( 2014 ) 83 87

Development of methods for the analysis of movement and orientation behaviour in wayfinding tasks based on the case study mirror maze

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Visual Design. Simplicity, Gestalt Principles, Organization/Structure

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

APPENDIX L: ANALYTICS

Effects of ICT on the youth: A study about the relationship between internet usage and social isolation among Iranian students

How do we read YAH maps? An Eye-Tracking Study

Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal

Chapter 7: Descriptive Statistics

Object Recognition & Categorization. Object Perception and Object Categorization

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 205 ( 2015 ) th World conference on Psychology Counseling and Guidance, May 2015

TRAVEL TIME AS THE BASIS OF COGNITIVE DISTANCE*

P O D C A S T Transcript. Dr. Gary Small. Author of 2 Weeks to a Younger Brain

(SAT). d) inhibiting automatized responses.

The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby

Mapping fear of crime dynamically on everyday transport: SUMMARY (1 of 5) Author: Reka Solymosi, UCL Department of Security & Crime Science

Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making

Action, verbal response and spatial reasoning

MSc Psychological Research Methods/ MPsych Advanced Psychology Module Catalogue / 2018

Oscillatory Neural Network for Image Segmentation with Biased Competition for Attention

The Relationship Between Way-Finding Strategies, Spatial Anxiety, and Prior Experiences

Object Lessons. The Questions

draft Big Five 03/13/ HFM

How Far Away Is That? It Depends on You: Perception Accounts for the Abilities of Others

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 143 ( 2014 ) CYICER 2014

Arnold Trehub and Related Researchers 3D/4D Theatre in the Parietal Lobe (excerpt from Culture of Quaternions Presentation: Work in Progress)

Types of questions. You need to know. Short question. Short question. Measurement Scale: Ordinal Scale

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Deaf-blindness

CIF champion, XC All American, Two College Conference Championships, Nike Sponsorship, Boston Marathon Qualifier Conference Presenter 20 different

Views of autistic adults on assessment in the early years

An Artificial Neural Network Architecture Based on Context Transformations in Cortical Minicolumns

Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions.

VISUAL PERCEPTION & COGNITIVE PROCESSES

The effect of little bird program in decreasing problem behaviors of autistic children

Observation and Assessment. Narratives

An Alternative Explanation for Premack & Premack

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby

Architectural Visualization

CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

Assessing Environmental Perceptions

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

TEN early signs of a Dementia

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge

Computers in Human Behavior

Concepts About the Causes of Development: Travel, Visual Experience, and the Development of Dynamic Spatial Orientation

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) for Physical Health Conditions

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Visual Design: Perception Principles. ID 405: Human-Computer Interaction

Chapter 02 Ethical Decision-Making: Personal and Professional Contexts

Podcast Script. Battle of the Sexes: Do women really have better social skills than men? Psyco 403: Topics in Evolutionary Psychology.


Mechanisms of Spatial Cognition: Behavioral Experiments in Virtual Environments

The Concept of Simulation in Control-Theoretic Accounts of Motor Control and Action Perception

SESSION 2. 4MAT Online Instructional Design Certification. The Brain & Balance

Neural Systems for Visual Scene Recognition

The effects of multimedia learning on children with different special education needs

The validity of inferring real-world cognitive mapping ability based on performance in a virtual environment

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning

Hebbian Plasticity for Improving Perceptual Decisions

A Direct Object of Perception

To What Extent Can the Recognition of Unfamiliar Faces be Accounted for by the Direct Output of Simple Cells?

Supporting Children with an Autism Spectrum Disorder. An Introduction for Health and Social Care Practitioners

Differences of Face and Object Recognition in Utilizing Early Visual Information

T1: RESOURCES TO ADDRESS THE NEEDS OF PERSONS WITH DEMENTIA AND THEIR CAREGIVERS 2014 GOVERNOR S CONFERENCE ON AGING AND DISABILITY

11 DO BEES SEE SHAPES?1

RISK COMMUNICATION FLASH CARDS. Quiz your knowledge and learn the basics.

Biology Notes Chapter 1 The Study of Life

The Impact of Schemas on the Placement of Eyes While Drawing.

Psychology Perception

An overview of the verbatim argument in Husserl s Fifth Cartesian Meditation

Spatial Orientation Using Map Displays: A Model of the Influence of Target Location

NST II Psychology NST II Neuroscience (Module 5)

The Science of Changing Your Mind Part 3 of an interview with Joe Dispenza, DC

The architect s perspective on the tour and map perspective

Importance of Deficits

Study of Imagery. Banned by behaviorists

Dynamic Route Decision Model-based Multi-agent Evacuation Simulation - Case Study of Nagata Ward, Kobe

Chapter 6: Attribution Processes

PROFICIENCY BENCHMARKS

Lumus360 Psychometric Profile Pat Sample

Thinking the environment aurally An enactive approach to auditory-architectural research and design

Material Anchors for Conceptual Blends

Transcription:

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 32 (2012) 53 62 4 th International Conference of Cognitive Science (ICCS 2011) Recreating mental image with the aid of cognitive maps and its role in environmental perception Fatemeh Imani a, Marzieh Tabaeian b,* a MS candidate, Islamic Azad University of Isfahan, Khorasgan Branch, Architecture department. b Department of Art and Architecture, Islamic Azad University, Tehran, Iran Abstract People have some kind of internal model of a geographical space. This research is concerned with aspects of environmental cognition; that is our ability to imagine about our spatial world. In particular, this study makes an attempt to describe cognitive maps and the individual differences between people s map. Data was gathered from a sample of fifty persons. All of them walked through a historical region in Isfahan and were asked to draw a sketch map; the map was then assessed. The major finding was that our spatial knowledge is map-like rather than, say, a sequence of instructions for various routes. 2011 Published by by Elsevier Ltd. Ltd. Selection and/or peer-review under responsibility of the 4th International Conference of Cognitive Science Open access under CC BY-NC-ND license. Keywords: Environmental perception; mental image; cognitive map; sketch map; environmental psychology; way-finding ability; spatial knowledge; historical region; internal representation; pictorial information 1. Introduction How do people utilize information about their environment to find their way? This has been a research on different aspects of environmental cognition; that is their ability to imagine and think about their spatial world. This study in particular makes an attempt to describe cognitive maps, evaluates their function and individual differences between people s map. 1.1. Cognitive maps A familiar experience can occur when persons have a party, but not all of the guests know how to get to their house. They may draw a map even without the experience of having seen a map of the area. It is as though they have some kind of internal model of a geographical space. This was described by Tolman (1948), who was the first to use the term cognitive map, which he defined as an internal representation that animals develop about the spatial relationships within their environment. A cognitive map needs only to provide a path with appropriate nodes in order to be effective in enabling them to engage in successful way-finding along a single predetermined journey. However, if they wish to take alternative * Corresponding author. Tel.: +0098-9133198057; fax:+0-000-000-0000 E- mail address: marzieh.tabaeian1@gmail.com 1877-0428 2011 Published by Elsevier Ltd. Selection and/or peer-review under responsibility of the 4th International Conference of Cognitive Science Open access under CC BY-NC-ND license. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.01.010

54 Fatemeh Imani and Marzieh Tabaeian / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 32 (2012) 53 62 routes or find their way to new locations, they would need a more sophisticated cognitive map with landmarks and networks of paths, or they could seek further navigational aids. 1.2. The nature of way-finding Way-finding is an issue in which the environment and behavior are indisputably intertwined. Way-finding is a behavior. Successful way-finding involves knowing where you are, knowing your destination, knowing and following the best route to your destination, being able to recognize your destination upon arrival, and reversing the process to find your way back out (Carpman & Grant, 1997). 1.3. What is disorientation? Disorientation is a widespread problem in many environments. They may indeed lose their selves, but this tends to be short-lived. By keeping moving until they find somewhere familiar, or by returning to a known location, they can re-establish effective way-finding. 1.4. Action plans According to Garling, Book, and Lindberg (1986), way-finding is a decision-making process that operates through a four-step action plan: - Determine the location - Localize the destination - Select a route - Decide how to travel. 2. Method The various techniques for investigating cognitive maps each have their own advantages and disadvantages, but the simplest, using mazes and drawing sketch maps, are still often used. In this research, we have used the technique of drawing sketch maps. This is a map drawn from observation rather than from exact measurements and representing the main features of an area. 2.1. Drawing sketch maps This technique, pioneered by Lynch (1960), enables us to gather much detailed information about people s cognitive maps and has been used extensively. It aims to reproduce the individual s internal image of the environment and is relatively easy in essence. However, there are some major drawbacks with this method. One of the biggest difficulties is that the sketch maps of different individuals will vary not only because their cognitive maps differ but also as a consequence of variation in drawing ability, the perspective from which the map has been drawn, the scale and the type of map (spatial or sequential). Sequential map is a map that relates to journeys between places and is focused on paths and nodes, and spatial map is a map that more closely resembles a bird s eye view, emphasizing spatial organization. 2.2. Case study Participants were a sample of fifty persons, who were for investigating the effects of gender and familiarity with the environment, were chosen from two gender groups: males and females. Also to investigate the effect of familiarity with the environment, 25 of them went to the place for the first time and the other half were resident in the area for longer. They were then taken on a tour visiting historical region in Isfahan. Haj Mohammad Jafar region is a historical area in the heart of Isfahan (figure 1). The intention of historical region is a part of the city that has historical, social, spatial and place values and it is chosen because in this region, buildings, spaces and valuable elements are as a contiguous set with an integrated structure. All of the participants walked through the historical region and were asked to draw a sketch map. It was assessed in three ways: - The frequency of landmarks, paths and nodes (table 1, table 5). This technique enables us to gather much detailed information about people s sketch map and it aims to reproduce the individual s internal image of the environment.

Fatemeh Imani and Marzieh Tabaeian / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 32 (2012) 53 62 55 - The complexity of the map (spatial maps showing appropriate interconnections being better than sequential maps) - Accurate placement of the 8 target buildings seen on the tour (figure 1). The quality of the sketch map was the best indicator of way-finding ability. Also after the end of the visiting, participants were shown illustrations depicting different landmarks, and asked whether they recognized or could identify them when they were interspersed with illustrations of unknown locations (e.g., figure 2, 3). Figure 1. Haj Mohammad Jafar region in Isfahan: 1- HABIBEH KHATOON SARA, 2- HAJ MOHAMMAD- JAFAR MOSQUE, 3- BAZAR, 4- FESHARAKI HOUSE, 5- GHODSIEH SCHOOL, 6-TADAYYON HISTORICAL HOUSE, 7- VAZIR HAMMAM 8- DARBE-EMAM HOLY IMAMZADEH Figure 2. Path to the Vazir Bazar

56 Fatemeh Imani and Marzieh Tabaeian / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 32 (2012) 53 62 Figure 3. Imamzadeh courtyard 3. Results The major finding was that people s spatial knowledge is map-like rather than, say, a sequence of instructions for various routes. In addition, sketch maps provide a way to externalize the cognitive maps. They are internal representations; thus, cognitive maps are not accurate replicas of the real world and contain mistakes. 3.1. Errors in cognitive map The nature of the errors in cognitive maps is itself interesting. - Participants cognitive maps are incomplete: they tend to omit minor and sometimes even major details. For example, in sketch maps many small paths and roads are missing and we also find that they have left out a whole district or key location! - Participants cognitive maps may be distorted; for example, the distances between locations may be too small or too great, and the relationships between road junctions may be simplified. Roads that meet at a junction tend to be represented as closer to right angles than they really are. - Participants maps are disproportionate: they tend to exaggerate the size of places that they know well or prefer. They overestimated the size of their own neighbourhoods compared to other districts of the city. Pinheiro (1998) found that the appearance and size of countries on students sketch maps of the world were related to factors such as military and economic power as well as their actual size. Seibert and Anooshian (1993) reported that participants omitted from their sketch maps areas or landmarks that they did not like. - Participants may augment their maps; that is, add non-existent features to them. This enables us to recognize another potential problem with the use of sketch maps as a measure of cognitive maps; people may lack actual geographical knowledge but make correct inferences, leading to augmentations that are accurate. This means that individuals are inappropriately credited for aspects of their maps that are the result of augmentation rather than experience with the environment. 3.2. Reasons of individual differences between cognitive maps Individual differences between cognitive maps arise as a function of familiarity and gender. In other words individual differences arise due to differences in how well people know the locality. Sketch maps of persons that they are familiar with this area thus tend to be more detailed and accurate. Another factor that affects familiarity

Fatemeh Imani and Marzieh Tabaeian / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 32 (2012) 53 62 57 with the local environment is socio-economic status. This may arise because wealthier people have a greater opportunity to experience the environment, and perhaps because they are more likely to be driving than walking or using public transport. Males preferred orientation strategies that use global references, such as compass points or the position of the sun. Females reported using route strategies, relying on local references such as sequences of left and right turns and landmarks. 3.3. Familiarity with the environment One of the most significant factors affecting the accuracy of participant's cognitive map is their familiarity with the environment and so individual differences arise due to differences in how well people know the locality. Sketch maps of persons that they are familiar with the area thus tend to be more detailed and accurate, so participants who have been resident in an area for longer, produce more detailed sketch maps. These also tend to be of a spatial nature compared to the more sequential maps produced by newcomers to the area. Evans, Marrero, and Butler (1981) found that, as time progresses, our maps develop from a focus on nodes and paths to inclusion of landmarks as an area becomes more familiar. The stability of spatial relationships between landmarks is important if they are to assist navigation. Of course, persons familiarity with their environment does not have to be visual: other cues may also assist their way-finding. Sommer (1998) describes the importance of auditory cues to navigation for a blind student. It was found that familiarity with the type of environment may also affect way-finding ability; participants from a grid-patterned, repetitive layout made more errors on tasks measuring abstract imagery (for example, drawing a route map) than others from non-repetitive environments.(abu-obeid, 1998) Therefore, the way-finding problems may arise as a result of insufficient visually memorable cues to aid the building of an effective environmental image or cognitive map. Table1. Frequency of landmarks, paths and nodes familiar unfamiliar landmark 95% 65% node 98% 60% path 95% 70% Table2. Frequency of 8 target building familiar unfamiliar Habibeh Khatoon Sara 95% 60% Haj Mohammad-Jafar mosque 100% 85% Bazar 100% 95% Fesharaki historical house 90% 55% Ghodsieh school 90% 30% Tadayyon historical house 90% 35% Vazir Hammam 100% 50% Darbe-Emam Holy Imamzadeh 100% 95% Action plans may be spatially asymmetrical; that is, peoples may plan journeys such that they leave by one route and return via another. People prefer routes that are initially long and straight, even if they cover a much greater total distance. This region (Haj Mohammad Jafar) had two alternatives for participants to navigate: path A and path B that shows in figure 4.

58 Fatemeh Imani and Marzieh Tabaeian / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 32 (2012) 53 62 Table3: frequency of use two alternatives Path A Path B Males 30% 18% Females 37% 15% Total 67% 33% Figure 4. Two alternatives for navigation from the beginning of Bazar to DARBE-EMAM HOLY IMAMZADEH Table 4. Type of action plans of participants symmetrical asymmetrical familiar 38% 62% unfamiliar 90% 10% 3.4. Gender Differences Differences between males and females in navigation are somewhat contentious. Appleyard research in Venezuela (1970) found that males cognitive maps were slightly more accurate and extensive, but concluded that this was a consequence of greater familiarity with the city. Other research has suggested that while there are few differences in accuracy, males and females maps seem to differ in style. Sketch maps produced by women seem to focus on landmarks and districts, whilst those of men are based on paths. Research has also shown that women may know more information about a district than they include on their sketch maps. Self-reports by men and women also reveal differences in their reliance on way-finding information. Men preferred orientation strategies that use global references, such as compass points or the position of the sun. Women reported using route strategies, relying on local references such as sequences of left and right turns and landmarks. As a consequence, the removal of local cues to position affects way-finding for women more than men. Harrel, Bowlby, and Hall-Hoffarth (2000) found that men provided more complete sketch maps but they were no more likely than women to include landmarks or labelled buildings. The men were also more confident and willing to lead visitors to their destinations. So differences in way-finding performance may be affected by emotions or confidence, as well as by navigational ability. Groen, Wunderlich, Spitzer, Tomczak, and Riepe (2000) investigated gender differences in brain activation during visuospatial navigation. In males, the left hippocampus showed distinct activation, while in females the right

Fatemeh Imani and Marzieh Tabaeian / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 32 (2012) 53 62 59 parietal and right prefrontal cortex were consistently activated. This has provided a neural basis for the wellestablished gender differences observed in spatio-cognitive performance. In general, men seem to be better at navigating through virtual environments than women, but this may be a consequence of their greater experience with computer games, rather than reflecting a real-world difference in wayfinding ability (Moffat, Hampson, & Hatzipantelis, 1998). Differences between men s and women s navigational ability may also depend on more general spatial skills. Typical findings show that men outperform women on tests of mental rotation. For example, they are better at judging whether an object that appears to have been turned in three-dimensional space is in fact the same or is a mirror image. Mental rotation ability correlated positively with way-finding performance and typical gender differences were found on the spatial ability task with men scoring higher than women on average although some women performed as well as men. The results suggest that while the spatial skills demonstrated by mental rotation may be important to way-finding in men they may not be so essential for effective way-finding in some women; they must be using strategies related to other skills (Malinowski, 2001). Table 5. Frequency of landmarks, paths and nodes males females landmark 55% 80% node 70% 75% path 80% 70% Table 6. Frequency of 8 target building males females Habibeh Khatoon Sara 60% 85% Haj Mohammad-Jafar mosque 90% 90% Bazar 90% 90% Fesharaki historical house 70% 60% Ghodsieh school 40% 35% Tadayyon historical house 32% 30% Vazir Hammam 90% 90% Darbe-Emam Holy Imamzadeh 95% 95% Table 7. Type of action plans of participants symmetrical asymmetrical Males 40% 60% Females 65% 35% 3.5. Assisting way-finding What else can be done to help the navigator to find his or her way? Garling, Book, and Lindberg (1984) identified three features that contribute to successful navigation. Differentiation is the extent to which different parts of the environment are similar or distinguishable. Evans, Smith, and Pezdek (1982) demonstrated that buildings with high differentiation, such as ones that stood alone or were of an unusual shape, were remembered better. So, if building differ in appearance persons can differentiate them, making way-finding more effective. If all the buildings look the same, they are more likely to get lost (e.g., figure 5, 6).

60 Fatemeh Imani and Marzieh Tabaeian / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 32 (2012) 53 62 Figure 5. Local landmark that contribute to successful navigation Figure 6. Differentiation in Imamzadeh courtyard entry affects in successful navigation Way-finding is also facilitated by a high degree of visual access; that is, being visible from different perspectives (figure 7).

Fatemeh Imani and Marzieh Tabaeian / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 32 (2012) 53 62 61 Figure 7. Path leading to the Bazar How difficult an area is to understand in terms of the amount of detail and its intricacy is referred to as the complexity of spatial layout. Way-finding is hampered in environments with a very complicated spatial layout. 4. Conclusions 4.1. Techniques for studying cognitive maps - Sketch maps provide a way to externalize the cognitive map in humans. - Sketch maps may not reliably indicate the detail of a person s cognitive map. Reflecting instead the individual s drawing ability. - Analyzing sketch maps may be difficult due to variations in the perspective from which they are drawn, the scale and the orientation of the paper chosen by the participant. - Recognition tasks cannot be used to measure internal representations of distances, nor do they provide as much detail as sketch maps. - Recognition tasks have good validity, as we rely on Recognising familiar locations to navigate in real-world environments. 4.2. Cognitive maps as an explanation for human navigation - The concept of cognitive maps can explain the ability to recall routes and select detours when a path is obstructed. - Cognitive maps seem to hold information on both routes and landmarks. - Individual differences in cognitive maps may in part explain gender differences in navigation and the effects of familiarity on accuracy of way-finding. - The possession of a cognitive map is not in itself sufficient to account for all of our spatial abilities. Way finding needs to be conceived as a macro issue involving the physical and operational environments in which it occurs, rather than being understood as something dealing only with individual perception, cognition and behavior. The findings suggest that the process of way-finding is assisted internally by our cognitive maps, action plans (we pre-plan our behavior in the environment) and magnetic compass orientation. External aids to way-finding include maps and virtual-reality environments and pictorial information, from variety in the shape, texture and

62 Fatemeh Imani and Marzieh Tabaeian / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 32 (2012) 53 62 contours, helps to differentiate landmarks. Maybe it occurs for all of us that we are lost in an environment with symmetrical design features. These problems in designing are very important and architects and urban designers have to pay attention to them when planning and designing components of a city, town or new district. References Abu-Obeid, N. (1998). Abstract and scenographic imagery: The effect of environmental from on wayfinding. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 18, 159-173. Appleyard, D. (1970). Styles and methods of structuring a city. Environment and Behavior, 2, 100-117. Carpman, J. R., & Grant, M. A. (1997). Wayfinding in healthcare facilities. In S. Marberry (Ed.), Healthcare design: An introduction. New York: Wiley. Evans, G. W., Marrero, D. G., & Butler, P. A. (1981). Environmental learning and cognitive mapping. Environment and Behavior, 13, 83-104. Evans, G. W., Smith, C., & Pezdek, K. (1982). Cognitive maps and urban form. Journal of the American Planning Association, 48, 232-244. Garling, T., Book, A., & Lindberg, E. (1984). Cognitive mapping of large-scale environments: The interrelationships of action plans, acquisition and orientation. Environment and Behavior, 16, 3-34. Garling, T., Book, A., & Lindberg, E. (1986) Spatial orientation and wayfinding in the designed environment: A conceptual analysis and some suggestions for postoccupancy evaluation. Journal of Architectural Planning Research, 3, 55-64. Harrel, W. A., Bowlby, J. W., & Hall-Hoffarth, D. (2000). Directing wayfinders with maps: The effects of gender, age, route complexity, and familiarity with the environment. Journal of Social Psychology, 140, 169-178. Gron, G., Wunderlich, A. P., Spitzer, M., Tomczak, R., & Riepe, M. W. (2000). Brain activation during human navigation: Gender-different neural networks as substrate of performance. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 404-408. Lynch, K. (1960). The image of the city. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press Malinowski, J. C. (2001). Mental rotation and real-world way finding. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 92, 19-30 Moffat, S. D., Hampson, E., & Hatzipantelis, M. (1998). Navigation in a virtual maze: Sex differences and correlation with psychometric measures of spatial ability in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 9, 73-87. Pinheiro, J. Q. (1998). Determinants of cognitive maps of the world as expressed in sketch maps. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 18, 321-339. Roberts, C., & Russell, J. (2002). Angles on environmental psychology. UK: Nelson Thornes Ltd. Seibert, P. S., & Anooshian, L. J. (1993). Indirect expression of preference in sketch maps. Environment and Behavior, 25, 607-624. Sommer, R. (1998). Shopping at the co-op. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 18, 45-53. Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189-208.