Do People s First Names Match Their Faces?

Similar documents
Child attention to pain and pain tolerance are dependent upon anxiety and attention

Author's personal copy

Objectives. 6.3, 6.4 Quantifying the quality of hypothesis tests. Type I and II errors. Power of a test. Cautions about significance tests

Reinforcing Visual Grouping Cues to Communicate Complex Informational Structure

Money and Mimicry: When Being Mimicked Makes People Feel Threatened. Jia (Elke) Liu. University of Groningen. Kathleen D. Vohs

Sampling methods Simple random samples (used to avoid a bias in the sample)

Annie Quick and Saamah Abdallah, New Economics Foundation

Randomized controlled trials: who fails run-in?

State-Trace Analysis of the Face Inversion Effect

Patterns of Inheritance

Origins of Hereditary Science

Transitive Relations Cause Indirect Association Formation between Concepts. Yoav Bar-Anan and Brian A. Nosek. University of Virginia

Introducing Two-Way and Three-Way Interactions into the Cox Proportional Hazards Model Using SAS

Cognitive Load and Analogy-making in Children: Explaining an Unexpected Interaction

carinzz prophylactic regimens

+ + =?? Blending of Traits. Mendel. History of Genetics. Mendel, a guy WAY ahead of his time. History of Genetics

Mendel Laid the Groundwork for Genetics Traits Genetics Gregor Mendel Mendel s Data Revealed Patterns of Inheritance Experimental Design purebred

Research. Dental Hygienist Attitudes toward Providing Care for the Underserved Population. Introduction. Abstract. Lynn A.

Differential Prosocial Behaviour Without Altered Physical Responses in Mirror Sensory Synesthesia

Automatic System for Retinal Disease Screening

Family Dysfunction Differentially Affects Alcohol and Methamphetamine Dependence: A View from the Addiction Severity Index in Japan

Anchor Selection Strategies for DIF Analysis: Review, Assessment, and New Approaches

Does Job Strain Increase the Risk for Coronary Heart Disease or Death in Men and Women?

Title: Correlates of quality of life of overweight and obese patients: a pharmacy-based cross-sectional survey

Decision Analysis Rates, Proportions, and Odds Decision Table Statistics Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) Analysis

Cocktail party listening in a dynamic multitalker environment

A Note on False Positives and Power in G 3 E Modelling of Twin Data

Severe Psychiatric Disorders in Mid-Life and Risk of Dementia in Late- Life (Age Years): A Population Based Case-Control Study

The Application of a Cognitive Diagnosis Model via an. Analysis of a Large-Scale Assessment and a. Computerized Adaptive Testing Administration

Gender Differences and Predictors of Work Hours in a Sample of Ontario Dentists. Cite this as: J Can Dent Assoc 2016;82:g26

Course: Animals Production. Unit Title: Genetics TEKS: 130.3(C)(6)(B) Instructor: Ms. Hutchinson. Objectives:

Differences in the local and national prevalences of chronic kidney disease based on annual health check program data

Treating Patients with HIV and Hepatitis B and C Infections: Croatian Dental Students Knowledge, Attitudes, and Risk Perceptions

Dental X-rays and Risk of Meningioma: Anatomy of a Case-Control Study

Min Kyung Hyun. 1. Introduction. 2. Methods

Regret theory and risk attitudes

Medical Center, Van der Boechorststraat 7, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands

State Mindfulness and the Red Bull Effect

Comparative judgments of animal intelligence and pleasantness

Chapter 12 Patterns of Inheritance. What is Inheritance? Who is the Father of Modern Genetics? Answer: The passage of genes from parent to offspring

Relating mean blood glucose and glucose variability to the risk of multiple episodes of hypoglycaemia in type 1 diabetes

Fear of crime among university students: A research in Namik Kemal University

Presymptomatic Risk Assessment for Chronic Non- Communicable Diseases

The duration of the attentional blink in natural scenes depends on stimulus category

King s Research Portal

Estimating shared copy number aberrations for array CGH data: the linear-median method

Internet-based relapse prevention for anorexia nervosa: nine- month follow-up

Remaining Useful Life Prediction of Rolling Element Bearings Based On Health State Assessment

Assessment of Health Professionals Views and Beliefs about Mental Illnesses: A Survey from Turkey

Syncope in Children and Adolescents

The effect of presentation time and working memory load on emotion recognition.

Risk and Rationality: Uncovering Heterogeneity in Probability Distortion

STAT 200. Guided Exercise 7

1* 1 2. Kirsten McEwan, James Elander and Paul Gilbert. Centre for Psychological Research, University of Derby, UK 2

Developmental enamel defects and their impact on child oral health-related quality of life

Khalida Ismail, 1 Andy Sloggett, 2 and Bianca De Stavola 3

The majority of ad exposure occurs under incidental conditions where

CRIKEY - A Temporal Planner Looking at the Integration of Scheduling and Planning

The Model and Analysis of Conformity in Opinion Formation

Online publication date: 01 October 2010

Amino acids in retinitis pigmentosa

University of Groningen. Depression vulnerability: is it really what you think? van Rijsbergen, Gerard

AAST 2012 PLENARY PAPER. Exception from informed consent for emergency research: Consulting the trauma community

Acute Myocardial Infarction Mortality in Comparison with Lung and Bladder Cancer Mortality in Arsenic-exposed Region II of Chile from 1950 to 2000

The vignette, task, requirement, and option (VITRO) analyses approach to operational concept development

Interactions between Symptoms and Motor and Visceral Sensory Responses of Irritable Bowel Syndrome Patients to Spasmolytics (Antispasmodics)

Attitudes and beliefs about mental illness among relatives of patients with schizophrenia

Lipoprotein Cholesterol in the Russian Lipid Research Clinics Prevalence Follow-up Study

RESEARCH ARTICLE. Systematic Review of Research into the Psychological Aspects of Prostate Cancer in Asia: What do we Know?

Brain regions supporting intentional and incidental memory: a PET study

SOME ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN BLOOD GROUPS AND DISEASE

Article 4 Comparison of Amplitude of Accommodation in Different Vertical Viewing Angles

Computerized semen analysis: objective measurement of semen characteristics is biased by subjective parameter setting

in adolescent wrestlers.

PHYSICAL ACTIVITY ASSOCIATED TO LOWER PREVALENCE OF DEMENTIA IN ELDERLY SUBJECTS

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Research Article ABSTRACT. Amanda Myhren-Bennett College of Nursing, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC 29208, USA

Chapter 2. Horm Behav Jul;60(2):

adolescents; children; CONSORT; pain; randomized controlled trial.

Chapter Forty-Six 46 Concordance Marjorie C. Weiss STUDY POINTS * What is meant and understood by the term concordance * How concordance differs from

Relative Role of Factors Associated With Cerebral Infarction and Cerebral Hemorrhage

REI MONDEN, 1 STIJN DE VOS, 1 RICHARD MOREY, 2 ERIC-JAN WAGENMAKERS, 3 PETER DE JONGE 1 & ANNELIEKE M. ROEST 1

Hot Flashes and Related Outcomes in Breast Cancer Survivors and Matched Comparison Women

Evaluation of the Coping Strategies Used by Knee Osteoarthritis Patients for Pain and Their Effect on the Disease-Specific Quality of Life

Theory of mind in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome

Linear Theory, Dimensional Theory, and the Face-Inversion Effect

Acne Itch: Do Acne Patients Suffer From Itching?

A modular neural-network model of the basal ganglia s role in learning and selecting motor behaviours

Acute Myocardial Infarction Mortality in Comparison with Lung and Bladder Cancer Mortality in Arsenic-exposed Region II of Chile from 1950 to 2000

AN age-related reduction of muscle mass and strength is

Salutogenesis as a framework for improving health resources of adolescent boys

Outcomes following first-episode psychosis Why we should intervene early in all ages, not only in youth

Functioning and depression in patients under cognitivebehavioral

Prefrontal cortex fmri signal changes are correlated with working memory load

Merging of Experimental and Simulated Data Sets with a Bayesian Technique in the Context of POD Curves Determination

An Intuitive Approach to Understanding the Attributable Fraction of Disease Due to a Risk Factor: The Case of Smoking

Ovarian Cancer Survival McGuire et al. Survival in Epithelial Ovarian Cancer Patients with Prior Breast Cancer

IN a recent article (Iwasa and Pomiankowski 1999),

Performance of the Vocal Source Related Features from the Linear Prediction Residual Signal in Speech Emotion Recognition

Transcription:

First names and faces 1 Journal of Articles in Suort of the Null Hyothesis Vol. 12, No. 1 Coyright 2015 by Reysen Grou. 1539-8714 www.jasnh.com Do Peole s First Names Match Their Faces? Robin S. S. Kramer University of York, UK Alex L. Jones Gettysburg College, PA, USA We often feel that eole s first names suit their faces in some way. Evidence has already shown that we share common stereotyes about how eole with articular names should look. Here, we investigate whether there is any accuracy to these beliefs. Simly, can we match eole s names to their faces? Across two exeriments, we tested whether American (Exeriment 1) and British articiants (Exeriment 2) were able to match the first names of strangers with hotograhs of their faces. Although Exeriment 1 rovided some initial suort for accuracy in female articiants, we were unable to relicate this result in Exeriment 2. Therefore, we find no overall evidence to suggest that articular names and faces are associated with each other. Key Words: first names, facial aearance, facial stereotyes, face ercetion Corresonding author: Dr. Robin Kramer, Deartment of Psychology, University of York, York YO10 5DD, UK Email: remarknibor@gmail.com

2 Journal of Articles in Suort of the Null Hyothesis. JASNH, 2015, Vol. 11, No. 1 What s in a name? Although Shakeseare might argue how unimortant or arbitrary names are, there is a growing body of research investigating the link between our names and who we are. Here, we focus on first names rather than surnames since the former demonstrate much more freedom and variety when it comes to selection while the latter tend to be determined more by lineage. Although evidence now suggests that eole s names may influence their lives (e.g., Pelham, Carvallo, & Jones, 2005), few studies have exlored the otential link between names and faces. The aim of the resent study is to investigate whether there is any truth behind the common anecdote that eole actually look like their names. Researchers have found that names affect eole s lives. For examle, women with masculine names find more success in the legal rofession (Coffey & McLaughlin, 2009). Boys with female-sounding names misbehave more at school (Figlio, 2007). Women with attractive first names may be seen as more hysically attractive (Erwin, 1993; Garwood, Cox, Kalan, Wasserman, & Sulzer, 1980). A name s ethnicity (e.g., Greg versus Jamal) may also have detrimental effects when alying for jobs (Bertrand & Mullainathan, 2004; cf. Fryer & Levitt, 2004). Our names may even lay a role in where we choose to live or which jobs we ursue (Pelham, Mirenberg, & Jones, 2002). If our first names can influence our lives, through how we erceive ourselves or how others erceive us, then it is imortant to consider how our names are chosen. Rather than arbitrarily, there are clear atterns regarding names and their owners. Names are generally associated with a secific sex (although a relatively small number are gender-neutral, for examle, Jamie). In addition, arents race and education influence name choices (Lieberson & Bell, 1992). Names also convey age information, as trends in naming fluctuate through the decades. For instance, the name Mildred suggests an older White woman in today s society. Gallagher and Chen (2008) rovided a comuter model with information from the US Social Security baby name database, which contains the 1000 most oular male and female baby names during the eriod 1880-2006. Simly by combining this information with the erceived age and gender of Americans in hotograhs, the model was able to redict first names with an overall accuracy of 82%. Our names may also reresent our heritage, with first names being assed down through generations of families (Finch, 2008). While names rovide information regarding general category membershis (e.g., young, female, Black), do they also suggest more secific information about our facial aearance? We know that surnames were often derived from occuations (e.g., McCord & Shater, 1964), but recently, researchers have shown that we also share facial stereotyes of occuation grous (Oldmeadow, Sutherland, & Young, 2012). Simly, eole seem to agree on how the faces of those with certain occuations should look. More imortant for the current work, there is also evidence that we share facial stereotyes for first names (Lea, Thomas, Lamkin, & Bell, 2007). Particiants were asked to construct faces (using comuter software) for male first names. Those faces judged as good examles (by a second grou) were averaged together to create a rototye for each name. These rototyes were subsequently matched with the correct names by a third grou at above-chance levels. As such, we aear to have a shared idea of what eole should look like based on their first names. Comutational aroaches have demonstrated the utility of first names as facial attributes. By downloading hotograhs from Flickr, tagged with their owners first names, comuter models were able to use commonalities among images of eole with the same

First names and faces 3 name to redict the names of new images at a level four times greater than chance (Chen, Gallagher, & Girod, 2014). Although this level of accuracy clearly benefited from naming conventions regarding sex, age, and ethnicity, it may well have also incororated more secific commonalities arising from face name associations. In the resent study, we investigate whether there is an association between faces and names above and beyond sex, age, and ethnicity. While often noted in real-world exeriences, there is no exerimental evidence attemting to answer this question to our knowledge. Exeriment 1 We utilised a set of Americans as models, and recruited a samle of American students as articiants. It was imortant to make sure models and articiants were of the same nationality, and also a similar age range, in order that they would be most exerienced with any ossible name-face relationshis within their culture and age grou if they existed. Particiants Method Fifty-seven undergraduate students at Gettysburg College volunteered to articiate in exchange for either $10 or course credits. Of these, 52 self-reorted as White Americans (age M = 19.92 years, SD = 1.22 years; 22 men), and their resonses were included in subsequent analyses. Stimuli Two hundred and sixteen images were downloaded from an online database (www. facity.com) that contains high quality hotograhs of faces from cities around the world, along with their owners first names and years of birth. All hotograhs in the database are osed front-on and with a neutral exression. Minimal or no cosmetics are worn. Images were croed just below the chin and to the middle of the forehead. No clothing was visible. Of these 216 images (all those in the database who lived in US cities), 86 individuals (43 men) were chosen as models because they were White and their birth years fell between 1980 and 1997. The website does not reort when each hotograh was taken, although we know this ranges from January 2010 (when the database extended beyond Germany) to Aril 2015 (when the images were downloaded). Therefore, the minimum and maximum ages of the models deicted in our hotograhs were 12 and 35 resectively. Due to this uncertainty regarding models recise ages, our attemts to remove age as a otential cue to a erson s name were only artially successful, given the maximum age range of 23 years. Procedure Particiants were shown airs of images on a lato comuter using custom PsychoPy software (Peirce, 2007). Models were aired at random for each articiant with

4 Journal of Articles in Suort of the Null Hyothesis. JASNH, 2015, Vol. 11, No. 1 a restriction that for each airing, both models were of the same sex. In addition, the two models names could not share the first three letters. This revented airs of models aearing together where both had the same name or the same root to their names. On each of the 42 trials, two faces aeared onscreen along with one of the models first names (chosen at random from the two models dislayed). Particiants were instructed to select which erson s name it was ( Which of these individuals is named X? ). Resonses were made using the mouse, and no time limit was given. The exeriment took less than five minutes to comlete. Results and Discussion Accuracy was calculated for each articiant as the roortion of correct resonses, searately for male trials (where airs of men were shown) and female trials. These values were entered into a 2 (Particiant Sex: Male, Female) x 2 (Model Sex: Male, Female) ANOVA. Particiant Sex varied between subjects while Model Sex varied within subjects. We found a significant main effect of Particiant Sex, F(1, 50) = 6.96, =.011, η 2 = 0.12, with women (M = 0.537, 95% CI [0.509, 0.565]) achieving higher accuracies than men (M = 0.481 [0.448, 0.513]). We found no main effect of Model Sex, F(1, 50) = 1.00, =.323, η 2 = 0.02, and no interaction between these two factors, F(1, 50) = 0.61, =.439, η 2 = 0.01. Therefore, accuracies were aggregated across Model Sex to roduce a combined roortion correct for all trials. One-samle t-tests were then carried out in order to determine whether women and men (searately) erformed differently from chance (roortion correct of 0.5). We found that men erformed at chance, t(21) = 1.19, =.246, d = 0.25. However, women were significantly more accurate than what would be exected by chance, t(29) = 2.66, =.013, d = 0.49. Even after alying Bonferroni correction for multile tests (using α =.025), this remained significant. It is interesting that women aear better able to accurately erceive cues to names from faces, given revious research demonstrating that women tend to outerform men when identifying facial exressions (Hall, 1978; Nowicki & Hartigan, 1988; Thayer & Johnsen, 2000). Although the above result with female articiants may be considered to have a medium effect size, we remain cautious in our interretation. Given that revious research has shown that eole share beliefs regarding how those with certain names should look (Lea et al., 2007), might it be ossible that this could exlain the significant accuracy found here? For examle, if our female articiants had a shared belief about which face goes with the name Bob, and in our samle of images, we haened to have a Bob that fit the descrition, then resonses on this trial would tend to be correct. By chance, we might get such a name face airing in our set of stimuli that coincides with a common stereotye. As such, women would have a baseline score of one trial correct, and so erforming at chance for the remaining trials would give an average roortion correct of 21.5 out of 42, or 0.512. Simly, a single model may drive their above chance accuracy. If we suose that chance erformance were indeed 21.5/42, then we find that women do not differ from what we would exect by chance, t(29) = 1.81, =.080, d = 0.33. However, if we focus on the model as the unit of analysis, we fail to find suort for this exlanation. We calculated the roortion of correct trials for each model (where that model and their name aeared onscreen, aired with a second model), across all female articiants resonses. Tests for normality, examining standardised skewness and

First names and faces 5 the Shairo-Wilk test, indicated the data were statistically normal. In addition, we found no evidence of statistical outliers associated with high resonse accuracies. Therefore, there is no evidence to suggest that one or a few models reresented excetions, associated with articularly high roortions correct, and caused the above result. Indeed, similar analyses lead to the same conclusions regarding the male articiants resonses, suggesting that their chance-level erformance was not the result of one or a few low-scoring outliers bringing down an otherwise medium level of accuracy. Another exlanation for the resent result may be that female articiants were able to use age information in order to imrove resonse accuracy. Given the ossibility of an age difference of u to 23 years between the air of models aearing onscreen, it may be that the name was (accurately) erceived to suit one model more because of their aarent age. We therefore analysed all female articiants trials, recording the age difference (which may not reflect the exact difference in ages at the times the hotograhs were taken see above) between the two models on each trial and whether or not the resonse was correct. We carried out an indeendent samles t-test comaring correct and incorrect trials. However, we found no significant difference in the models age gas between correct (M = 4.73, 95% CI [4.47, 4.98]) and incorrect trials (M = 4.59, [4.31, 4.87]), t(1258) = 0.70, =.484, d = 0.04. Therefore, although this does not rule out the ossibility that age cues were useful, we find no suorting evidence for this exlanation. Similar analyses also suggest that male articiants made no use of age cues, t(922) = 0.39, =.698, d = 0.02. In order to increase our confidence that this finding with female articiants is not simly a false ositive, we decided to relicate the exeriment (rather than relying solely on null hyothesis testing; Cohen, 1994) using a new set of models and articiants. If the women again demonstrated above-chance accuracy, we could be more confident that observers are able to read cues to names in eole s faces. Exeriment 2 We sought to relicate the results of our first exeriment using a new set of British models, and a grou of British articiants as observers. Again, models and articiants were of the same nationality and were of similar ages. We utilised a more restricted age range for our models in this second exeriment in order to better control for any otential age-related cues to names. If we find accuracy in resonses for a second time, this would both increase our confidence in the result and also demonstrate its generalisability across cultures. Particiants Method Forty-one students at the University of York volunteered to articiate in exchange for chocolate biscuits. Of these, 40 self-reorted as White British (age M = 22.63 years, SD = 3.94 years; 20 men), and their resonses were included in subsequent analyses. Stimuli Ninety-six facial hotograhs were taken of undergraduate students at Bangor

6 Journal of Articles in Suort of the Null Hyothesis. JASNH, 2015, Vol. 11, No. 1 University. Of these, 89 White British individuals (27 men; age range 19-24 years; age unavailable for 7 individuals) were used as stimuli. Before hotograhs were taken, individuals were asked to remove any cosmetics they were wearing, and were instructed to ose with a neutral exression, facing front-on to the camera. Images were croed just below the chin, but the whole head, including hairstyle, was left visible. Minimal clothing around the shoulders also remained. First names were recorded. Students were awarded course credits for taking art. Procedure The same rocedure was used as in Exeriment 1, excet that there were 44 trials here. For each articiant, images were randomly aired, leaving one male hoto unused (as there was an odd number of men in the stimulus set). As above, resonses were made using the mouse, and no time limit was given. The exeriment took less than five minutes to comlete. Results and Discussion Accuracy was calculated for each articiant as the roortion of correct resonses, searately for male trials (where airs of men were shown) and female trials. These values were entered into a 2 (Particiant Sex: Male, Female) 2 (Model Sex: Male, Female) ANOVA. Particiant Sex varied between subjects while Model Sex varied within subjects. We found no main effect of Particiant Sex, F(1, 38) = 0.46, =.502, η 2 = 0.01, no main effect of Model Sex, F(1, 38) = 2.80, =.102, η 2 = 0.07, and no interaction between these two factors, F(1, 38) = 2.05, =.160, η 2 = 0.05. Therefore, accuracies were aggregated across Model Sex and Particiant Sex to roduce a combined roortion correct for all trials. A one-samle t-test was then carried out in order to determine whether articiants (combined) erformed differently from chance (roortion correct of 0.5). We found that our articiants (M = 0.500, 95% CI [0.474, 0.526]) erformed at chance, t(39) = 0.00. As such, we failed to relicate our first exeriment, and found no evidence that articiants were able to read cues to names from models faces. General Discussion We know that first names rovide information regarding numerous social categories, including sex, age, and ethnicity. Here, we investigated the relationshi between first names and facial aearance. While revious research has shown that eole share stereotyes regarding how the faces of those with articular names should look (Lea et al., 2007), we failed to extend this idea to show that those beliefs about name face relationshis are accurate. In Exeriment 1, we found evidence that American women may be able to identify American faces from first names at above-chance levels of accuracy. However, Exeriment 2 failed to relicate this result with a new set of British faces and articiants. While one could argue that such a result may therefore be culture-secific, we see no reason to draw such a conclusion. Instead, we believe the more likely exlanation is simly that our initial significant effect was a false ositive, and this is why we were unable to relicate. Acknowledging the numerous anecdotes on this toic, why might we think there

First names and faces 7 could be an actual relationshi between facial aearance and first names? First, it could be that facial features are statistically associated with different names. Even controlling for sex and ethnicity, names are assed down within families (i.e., Robert I, Robert II, etc.) and this could lead to secific names becoming associated with certain facial features (see Lea et al., 2007). Second, the characteristics of our names may lead others to erceive or treat us in certain ways ( Lilith is utight ), and this may result in us behaving accordingly, including the ways in which we dress, groom, or exress ourselves. Third, the reverse may be true and our faces may influence our names. If we look articularly childlike, for instance, we (or others) may choose to abbreviate our names to increase congruency (e.g., Timmy rather than Timothy). While one or more of these hyotheses may hold some truth, their effects are not large enough to roduce accuracy in the current exeriments. In contrast, we can hyothesise many reasons why no (detectable) relationshi exists between first names and facial aearance. For instance, there are many thousands of ossible first names available for even a secific gender, age grou, and ethnicity, and there are virtually unlimited facial morhologies. Even if these two categories were somehow related, it seems unlikely that we could develo any workable knowledge of the countless ossible relationshis, given the limited number we each could conceivably exerience. Alternatively, there may simly be no relationshi between faces and first names because arents select names based on ersonal reference, regardless of the aearance of the baby (which, of itself, may rovide little insight into its adult aearance). The lack of a relationshi between first names and faces demonstrated here is articularly interesting given revious evidence that eole hold shared stereotyes regarding how individuals with secific names should look (Lea et al., 2007). Our results suggest these stereotyes arise for reasons other than a genuine link between names and aearance. For examle, cross-modal interaction between a name s sound and a erson s facial features may lead us to icture a man called Bob (acoustically round-sounding) as round-faced (see Melara & Marks, 1990). The current findings may also have more ractical relevance. Increasingly, sohisticated algorithms are emloyed to identify hotograhs osted to social networking sites, and their develoers will utilise any available statistical atterns in order to imrove such algorithms. Beyond simle category membershi, we suggest first names will rovide little use in these tyes of domains. In conclusion, we find no overall evidence suggesting that articular first names and faces are associated, once sex, age, and ethnicity have been taken into account. So next time you meet someone who looks like a Bob, bear in mind that his name could just as easily be Steve.

8 Journal of Articles in Suort of the Null Hyothesis. JASNH, 2015, Vol. 11, No. 1 References Bertrand, M., & Mullainathan, S. (2004). Are Emily and Greg more emloyable than Lakisha and Jamal? A field exeriment on labor market discrimination. The American Economic Review, 94, 991-1013. Chen, H., Gallagher, A. C., & Girod, B. (2014). The hidden sides of names: Face modeling with first name attributes. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 36, 1860-1873. Coffey, B., & McLaughlin, P. A. (2009). Do masculine names hel female lawyers become judges? Evidence from South Carolina. American Law and Economics Review, 11, 112-133. Cohen, J. (1994). The earth is round ( <.05). American Psychologist, 49, 997-1003. Erwin, P. G. (1993). First names and ercetions of hysical attractiveness. The Journal of Psychology: Interdiscilinary and Alied, 127, 625-631. Figlio, D. N. (2007). Boys named Sue: Disrutive children and their eers. Education Finance and Policy, 2, 376-394. Finch, J. (2008). Naming names: Kinshi, individuality and ersonal names. Sociology, 42, 709-725. Fryer, R. G., Jr., & Levitt, S. D. (2004). The causes and consequences of distinctively Black names. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 119, 767-805. Gallagher, A. C., & Chen, T. (2008). Estimating age, gender, and identity using first name riors. Proceedings of the IEEE Comuter Society: International Conference on Comuter Vision and Pattern Recognition. (Anchorage, AK, USA). Garwood, S. G., Cox, L., Kalan, V., Wasserman, N., & Sulzer, J. L. (1980). Beauty is only name dee: The effect of first-name on ratings of hysical attraction. Journal of Alied Social Psychology, 10, 431-435. Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 845-857. Lea, M. A., Thomas, R. D., Lamkin, N. A., & Bell, A. (2007). Who do you look like? Evidence of facial stereotyes for male names. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 901-907. Lieberson, S., & Bell, E. O. (1992). Children s first names: An emirical study of social taste. American Journal of Sociology, 98, 511-554. McCord, C. P., & Shater, C. (1964). Family names derived from occuations: The origin of 250 English family names from crafts. Archives of Environmental Health: An International Journal, 10, 947-949. Melara, R. D., & Marks, L. E. (1990). Processes underlying dimensional interactions: Corresondences between linguistic and nonlinguistic dimensions. Memory & Cognition, 18, 477-495. Nowicki, S., Jr., & Hartigan, M. (1988). Accuracy of facial affect recognition as a function of locus of control orientation and anticiated interersonal interaction. The Journal of Social Psychology, 128, 363-372. Oldmeadow, J. A., Sutherland, C. A. M., & Young, A. W. (2012). Facial stereotye visualization through image averaging. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 615-623. Pelham, B. W., Carvallo, M., & Jones, J. T. (2005). Imlicit egotism. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 106-110. Pelham, B. W., Mirenberg, M. C., & Jones, J. T. (2002). Why Susie sells seashells by the seashore: Imlicit egotism and major life decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 469-487. Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy Psychohysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162, 8-13. Thayer, J., & Johnsen, B. H. (2000). Sex differences in judgement of facial affect: A multivariate analysis of recognition errors. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 41, 243-246. Received: 5.31.2015 Revised: 6.14.2015 Acceted: 6.15.2015