Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant

Similar documents
Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold

COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO

Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present. A Thesis Presented

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A.

AN EVALUATION OF CHOICE ON INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICACY AND INDIVIDUAL PREFERENCES AMONG CHILDREN WITH AUTISM KAREN A. TOUSSAINT TIFFANY KODAK

Differential Reinforcement of Prompted and Independent Responses: An Alternative. Procedure to Decrease Prompt Dependency. A Thesis Presented

The Effects of Video Modeling on New Staff Training. of Discrete Trial Instruction. A thesis presented. Tamarra Forbes

Running head: EVALUATING INHIBITORY STIMULUS CONTROL. Increasing the Reinforcing Efficacy of Low-Preference Teachers and Caretakers

A Comparison of Methods for Marking Correct Responses. in Matching to Sample. A Thesis Presented. Kara Schmidt

EFFECTIVENESS OF THE IPAD WITH THE PROLOQUO2GO APPLICATION FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM. Presented by, Melissa L. King, M.S.

A Comparison of Edible, Social, and No Contrived Reinforcement on the. Acquisition of a Behavior Chain. A Thesis Presented.

topic : MOBILE TECHNOLOGY AND COM- MUNICATION JUNE 2012 ISSUE 12

Conditioned Reinforcement and the Value of Praise in Children with Autism

Determining Preference for Social Interaction. A Thesis Presented. Hillary S. Balog. In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

Increasing Independent Task Engagement in an Individual. with Autism. A thesis presented. Jessica Paredes

Evaluation of a presentation and measurement method for assessing activity preference

Reinforcing Effects of Social Stimuli on Responding After. Implementation of A Pairing Procedure. A Thesis Presented. Amanda J.

Running head: UTILITY OF TWO DEMAND ASSESSMENTS 1. An Evaluation of the Relative Utility of Two Demand Assessments

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

The Effects of Varying Response Requirements on. Reinforcer Potency. A Thesis Presented. Deborah L. Edelman

INCREASING INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICIENCY BY PRESENTING ADDITIONAL STIMULI IN LEARNING TRIALS FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS JASON C.

Evaluation of Salient Stimulus Cues During. Brief Functional Analyses in a Classroom Setting. Kelly L. McConnell. B.S., University of Florida

NEW ENGLAND CENTER FOR CHILDREN NORTHEASTERN UNIVERSITY

Running Head: AUDITORY PREFERENCE ASSESSMENT THE EFFECTS OF HIGHLY PREFERRED AUDITORY STIMULI IDENTIFIED

Running Head: Improving Parent Implementation of Discrete Trial Teaching

Reinforcer Strength in Transfer of Stimulus Control From Tacts to Mands

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT WITH AND WITHOUT BLOCKING AS TREATMENT FOR ELOPEMENT NATHAN A. CALL

The Effects of Question-Present Versus Item Present Conditions on Acquisition of Mands

Running Head: BEHAVIORAL ECONOMIC ANALYSIS OF CHOICE. A Behavioral Economic Analysis of Choice. A Thesis Presented. Jamie Leigh Lebowitz

Identifying & Enhancing the Effectiveness of Positive Reinforcement

A Thesis Presented. Delna Bharucha. The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology. In partial fulfillment of the requirements

Assessing the Effects of Matched and Unmatched Stimuli on the Persistence of Stereotypy. A Thesis Presented. Sarah Scamihorn

ASSESSMENT OF THE FUNCTIONS OF VOCAL BEHAVIOR IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES: A REPLICATION MICHAEL E. KELLEY

International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy ISSN: Universidad de Almería España

THE EFFECTS OF THERAPIST-MEDIATED SIGNALS AND PARTICIPANT-MEDIATED RESPONSES ON THE DELAY TOLERANCE OF TYPICALLY DEVELOPING PRESCHOOL CHILDREN

Teaching Two Children Diagnosed with Autism to Tolerate Physical Contact

Behavioral Development Bulletin. Mary E. McDonald 1, Sharon A. Reeve 2, and Erin J. Sparacio 3. Volume 19 Number 1 March 2014

AN EVALUATION OF PREFERENCE FOR VIDEO AND IN VIVO MODELING KANEEN B. GEIGER AND LINDA A. LEBLANC

BRIEF (TEST-CONTROL) FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS AND TREATMENT EVALUATION OF AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR EVOKED BY DIVIDED ATTENTION

EFFECTS OF ALTERNATIVE RESPONSES ON BEHAVIOR EXPOSED TO NONCONTINGENT REINFORCEMENT JAVIER VIRUES-ORTEGA BRIAN A. IWATA TARA A. FAHMIE JILL M.

CARBONE CLINIC VIVIAN ATTANASIO VERBAL BEHAVIOR INSTITUTE AND TAMARA KASPER CENTER FOR AUTISM TREATMENT

Using Pivotal Response Training with Peers in Special Education to Facilitate Play in Two Children with Autism

The Use of a Visual Aid to Facilitate Transitional Periods for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Effects of Differential Reinforcement of Short Latencies on Response Latency, Task Completion, and Accuracy of an Adolescent with Autism

A Comparison of Two Interventions to Treat Food Selectivity. A Thesis Presented. Shannon C. Garvey

OBSERVATIONAL EFFECTS ON PREFERENCE SELECTION FOR FOUR CHILDREN ON THE AUTISM SPECTRUM: A REPLICATION

Research. News and Notes About Scientific Research on ASD and Other Developmental and Behavioral Disorders.

A Comparison of Methods for Teaching Object Imitation: In-Vivo versus Video Modeling. A Thesis Presented. Amy N. Wick

Teaching a Scanning Response to a Child with Autism

METHOD Participants and Setting The participants in this study were 11 children (10 boys, 1 girl) with an independent

Recent work in applied behavior analysis has begun to examine the variables that influence

The Effects of Varying Levels of Procedural Integrity during Prompting. on Conditional Discrimination Performance. A Thesis Presented.

A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER GREGORY P. HANLEY KYLIE M.

A Comparison of Functional Analysis Results When Conducted in Contrived and. Home Settings. A Thesis Presented. Dana DeIngenis

A comparison of response-contingent and noncontingent pairing in the conditioning of a reinforcer

An Evaluation of the Utility of Two Demand Assessments in Identifying Negative. Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Casey E. Bethay

The Effect of Manual Sign Mand Training and Prompt Delay on the Acquisition. of Vocal-Verbal Behavior by a Two-Year-Old with PDD-NOS

Comparison of Two Approaches for Identifying Reinforcers in Teaching Figure Coloring to Students with Down Syndrome

Implementing Discrete Trial Teaching

ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT OF SOCIAL AVOIDANCE JILL M. HARPER

It is important to recognize that DTT in itself is not a good or bad method, and like any teaching method, it may be implemented on a continuum from

USING VIDEO MODELING AND REINFORCEMENT TO TEACH PERSPECTIVE-TAKING SKILLS TO CHILDREN WITH AUTISM LINDA A. LEBLANC AND ANDREA M.

Assessment of Delayed Matching in Preschoolers with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Hannorah O. Thurman

Functional Communication Training with Demand Fading. To Reduce Self-Injurious Behavior. Introduction

Research. News and Notes About Scientific Research on ASD and Other Developmental and Behavioral Disorders. Fall 2017

Abstract. Authors. Shauna Whiteford, Ksusha Blacklock, Adrienne Perry. Correspondence. Keywords

The Effects of a CABAS Conditioning Procedure To Increase Appropriate Toy Play and Decrease Stereotypy

THE EFFECTS OF INTERSPERESED TRIALS AND DENSITY OF REINFORCEMENT ON ACCURACY, LOOKING AWAY, AND SELF- INJURIOUS BEHAVIOR OF A CHILD WITH AUTISM

TEST-SPECIFIC CONTROL CONDITIONS FOR FUNCTIONAL ANALYSES TARA A. FAHMIE BRIAN A. IWATA ANGIE C. QUERIM JILL M. HARPER

ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT OF EXCESSIVE STRAIGHTENING AND DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR IN AN ADOLESCENT DIAGNOSED WITH AUTISM DAVID E. KUHN

Evaluating Picture Exchange and the ipad as a Speech Generating Device to Teach Communication to Young Children with Autism

Functionality. A Case For Teaching Functional Skills 4/8/17. Teaching skills that make sense

Pairing & Manding. Vincent J. Carbone Ed.D., BCBA-D NYS Licensed Behavior Analyst

STRATEGIES FOR SUCCESSFUL INSTRUCTION

Evaluating & Teaching Yes/No Responses Based on an Analysis of Functions. Jennifer Albis, M.S., CCC-SLP

RESPONSE PATTERNS IN FUNCTIONAL ANALYSES: A PRELIMINARY ANALYSIS. Christine M. Gibson, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE

The Impact of Formatting Skill Acquisition Procedures and Treatment Integrity. A Thesis Presented. Kara E. Lamb

EFFECTS OF MATCHED REINFORCEMENT ON CORRECT RESPONDING AND DISRUPTIVE BEHAVIOR DURING ACADEMIC INSTRUCTION. Elizabeth Lynn Zook

TEACHING SIMPLE AUDITORY DISCRIMINATIONS TO STUDENTS WITH AUTISM. Kristine L. Marino, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE

MARCUS INSTITUTE AND EMORY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE

MANDS FOR INFORMATION USING WHO? AND WHICH? IN THE PRESENCE OF ESTABLISHING AND ABOLISHING OPERATIONS M. ALICE SHILLINGSBURG

Functional Analysis and Reduction of Inappropriate Spitting

An Analysis of Shifting Preferences for Tasks Involved In Contingency Schedules

Jason Garner, M.A. ABA Clinical Director

How to Conduct Direct Preference Assessments for Persons with. Developmental Disabilities Using a Multiple-Stimulus Without Replacement

Treatment of Pica Using a Pica Exchange Procedure with Increasing Response Effort

REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE THINNING FOLLOWING TREATMENT WITH FUNCTIONAL COMMUNICATION TRAINING GREGORY P. HANLEY, BRIAN A. IWATA, AND RACHEL H.

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form

6/11/2017. Addressing Stereotypy The Importance of a Balanced Approach to this Core Symptom of Autism. stereotypy. Gregory P. Hanley Ph.D.

MAKING IT WORK AS AN ABA THERAPIST

Overview. Environmental Causes of Problem Behaviours Exhibited by Children and Youth with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Assessment

1/20/2015. Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction. Importance of Effective Teaching

The Effect of Choice-Making Opportunities during Activity Schedules on Task Engagement of Adults with Autism

Training and generalization of complex auditory-visual conditional discriminations in individuals with autism: New procedures using dynamic stimuli.

Establishing Conditioned Reinforcers in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Derived Textual Control in Recreational Activity Schedules with Children with Autism

COMPARISON OF SENSORY AND EDIBLE REINFORCERS

Chapter 10 Prompting and Transfer of Stimulus Control

TREATMENT OF COVERT SELF-INJURIOUS BEHAVIOR IN INDIVIDUALS WITH PRADER-WILLI SYNDROME

USING PERCENTILE SCHEDULES TO INCREASE EYE CONTACT IN CHILDREN WITH FRAGILE X SYNDROME SCOTT S. HALL, NATALEE P. MAYNES, AND ALLAN L.

Transcription:

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition A Thesis Presented By Robert Mark Grant In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science In the field of Applied Behavior Analysis Northeastern University Boston, MA July, 2011

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 2 Table of Contents I. Title A. Abstract 3 B. Introduction.. 4 C. Method 1. Participants and setting. 8 2. Procedure.. 9 3. Dependent variables.. 11 4. Measurement and interobserver agreement.. 11 D. Results.. 12 E. Discussion. 14 F. References. 17 G. Acknowledgement... 18 H. Figures.. 19

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 3 Abstract In order to effectively teach new skills, it is important to identify ways in which to reinforce the behavior. One important aspect of reinforcement is the way in which the reinforcer is delivered upon the completion of the task. Direct and indirect reinforcement are examples of two different contingencies of reinforcement, each associated with different stimulus arrangements. Direct reinforcement refers to arrangements in which the reinforcer is obtained through the completion of the task. Indirect reinforcement refers to arrangements in which the reinforcer is delivered through social mediation upon the completion of the task. Previous research conducted by Thompson and Iwata (2000), has found direct reinforcement to be more effective than indirect reinforcement in skill acquisition. The purpose of this study was to attempt to replicate the research of Thompson and Iwata (2000), and determine whether similar results would be found in a population of individuals with autism who have had a history with socially mediated reinforcement.

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 4 Comparison of direct and indirect reinforcement contingencies on task acquisition When teaching a new skill, it is important to consider which stimuli will be delivered to reinforce the target response. In order to identify items that may be preferred by the individual, a variety of preference assessments may be conducted. One preference assessment to consider is the forced choice assessment proposed by Fisher, Piazza, Bowman, Hagopian, Owens, and Slevin (1992). In this study, a stimulus preference assessment and a forced choice assessment was conducted with 16 different stimuli with 4 participants with mental retardation. In stimulus preference sessions, the 16 stimuli were presented individually 10 times over 8 sessions. In each session, 4 stimuli were presented 5 times, and approach to a stimulus resulted in 5s of access to the item. In the forced choice assessment, each of the 16 items was paired once with each of the other stimuli. These stimuli were paired in a randomized order, for a total of 120 paired stimulus presentations. If the participant approached one of the items within 5s, they were granted access to the item for 5s and the other item was removed. The stimuli used in these assessments were then grouped based on the results as: high-high (approached at least 80% of the time on both assessments), SP high (approached at least 80% of the time in the stimulus preference assessment and less than 60% of the time in the forced choice assessment) or FC high (approached at least 80% of the time in the forced choice assessment and less than 60% of the time in the stimulus preference assessment). When multiple stimuli met criteria for a high-high stimulus, the 2 stimuli with the greatest agreement across both assessments were chosen. Conversely, when multiple stimuli met criteria for a SP-high stimulus, the 2 with the greatest disagreement between both assessments were chosen. These selected reinforcers were then placed in one of two squares or chairs, and the individual could then gain access to the reinforcer for 10s by completing independent in seat work. Results indicated that the stimuli identified as

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 5 high-high stimuli were associated with significantly higher durations of in-seat behavior. These results suggest that the forced choice assessment could be more effective in identifying potential reinforcers than just conducting a stimulus preference assessment alone. Another method of identifying preferred items is the multiple-stimulus preference assessment presented by DeLeon and Iwata (1996). In this study, a multiple stimulus without replacement (MSWO), a paired stimulus (PS) and a multiple stimulus with replacement (MS) were conducted with 7 participants. In the MSWO assessment all of the items were laid out on the table, and after an item was chosen it was immediately removed from the area. The MS procedures were identical to those in the MSWO except for the fact that when an item was selected, it was replaced prior to the following session. Results indicated that there were stimuli that were identified as preferred in the MSWO and PS procedures that did not appear to be in the MS procedure. The effectiveness of these stimuli was assessed in a second experiment in order to determine if they supported levels of responding above baseline. The stimuli selected from the PS and MSWO procedures were implemented to reinforce a target response on an FR1 schedules. Results indicated that the stimuli that were never selected during the MS procedure but selected during the MSWO and PS procedures increased responding. These results indicate that the MSWO and PS procedures identified more potential reinforcers than the MS procedure. The studies conducted by Fisher et al. (1992) and DeLeon and Iwata (1996) showed that direct assessment of preference is the most effective way to identify potential reinforcers. Stimuli that are found to be preferred through these assessments can then be delivered contingent upon desired behaviors in order to increase the frequency at which they occur. Once preferred stimuli are identified, it is also important to determine the contingencies under which

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 6 the reinforcer will be delivered. Koegel and Williams (1980) compared direct and indirect response reinforcer relationships in teaching new behaviors. The purpose of this study was to examine differences between responding when the response was directly related to reinforcement, and when the response was not directly related to reinforcement and required social mediation to receive reinforcement. Direct response-reinforcer relationship was defined as one in which the target behavior is a direct part of a response chain required to produce a reinforcer (e.g., opening a container and retrieving a piece of food from inside the container). An indirect response-reinforcer relationship was defined as one in which the target behavior is an indirect part of the chain (e.g., a therapist delivers a piece of food contingent upon opening a container). Tasks were arbitrarily selected and included; saying wh, clapping, touching a book, discriminating white from green, discriminating between a square and a circle, and handing the therapist their own printed name. Results indicated that acquisition was met at a faster rate across participants and tasks when the target response was directly related to the response chain. The indirect condition was associated with low to high variable levels of correct responding, while the direct condition was associated with fast acquisition and high levels of correct responding in fewer trials. Williams, Koegel, and Egel (1981), also compared the effects of direct and indirect reinforcement contingencies on acquisition, however, in this study the authors referred to direct as functional and indirect as arbitrary. Functional reinforcement was defined as reinforcement that was directly related to the response, while arbitrary reinforcement was defined as reinforcement that was not directly related and required social mediation. The purpose of this study was to determine if arbitrary stimuli can improve learning by being arranged so that they become functionally related to the target response. Furthermore, the authors were interested in

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 7 observing whether or not the performance achieved under functional reinforcement would be maintained if arbitrary reinforcers were introduced. Simple tasks were taught for each participant, for example; bending over, touching one s head, and reaching out one s hand. These tasks were observed under both the arbitrary and functional reinforcement conditions. In the arbitrary condition, touching one s head was reinforced by placing a reinforcer in the child s hand upon completion of the task, while in the functional condition the reinforcer was placed on the child s head, resulting in immediate reinforcement for head touching. Results showed low levels of responding in the initial arbitrary condition. Subsequent functional reinforcement sessions resulted in an immediate increase in acquisition for all of the participants. Furthermore, all of the children rapidly achieved high levels of correct responding between 5 and 25 trials. Maintenance was observed when the arbitrary condition was reinstated. The effectiveness of these contingencies was also shown in a study conducted by Thompson and Iwata (2000) in which acquisition was observed under both direct (functional) and indirect (arbitrary) contingencies of reinforcement. The purpose of this study was to compare the effects of direct and indirect contingencies on individual performance, and to take data on reaching behavior under the indirect condition to observe whether the behavior may interfere with the target response. The effects of the direct and indirect contingencies were assessed on acquisition of opening clear plastic containers. Data were taken on correct independent responding, prompted correct responding, and reaching (observed only in the indirect condition). Reaching was defined as the participant trying to take the reinforcer from the therapist instead of engaging in the target response. Effects of these conditions were assessed under a multi-element design in which baseline, indirect, and direct contingencies were alternated. For all the other participants, results indicated that the direct condition was correlated with more rapid acquisition

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 8 than in the indirect condition. Response acquisition was observed in both conditions for 4 of the participants: however, 3 of these participants had a higher percentage of correct responding in the direct reinforcement condition. The indirect reinforcement condition was also found to be ineffective for 2 of the participants in that the response was not acquired in this condition. These results supported the hypothesis made by the authors who stated that the direct condition would be more effective in teaching acquisition, and that reaching may have inhibited responding in the indirect condition. The purpose of the current study was to replicate the procedures of the research conducted by Thompson and Iwata (2000), in order to determine whether the results obtained are similar to those produced by Thompson and Iwata (2000), which indicated that direct contingencies were more effective than indirect contingencies in teaching new skills. Participants and Setting Methods Six participants with autism spectrum disorder were selected to participate in this study because they were recommended by their teachers as not already having these skills in their repertoire. Alex, an 11 yr-old boy diagnosed with autism who has adequate fine motor skills and was able to print, eat, and dress himself with his dominant hand. He also had the ability to attend to verbal and model prompts. Sessions were conducted at his residential home in a room in which other teachers and students weren t present, typically the kitchen or living room. These sessions were conducted once or twice per week with 2 sessions conducted per work day. Ricky, a 12 yr-old boy diagnosed with autism who has limited fine motor skills, as he has low muscle tone in his hands. At times, his ability to complete fine motor tasks was also limited due to lack

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 9 of attending. He also engages in frequent problem behavior, such as climbing and grabbing. Sessions were conducted in his residential group home twice a week, with 3 sessions conducted per work day. These sessions were conducted in a quiet room with only relevant task materials, reinforcers, and his primary teacher present. Ethan, an 8 yr-old boy diagnosed with autism who has very limited fine motor skills and may require assistance completing simple tasks, such as opening a box of cereal. Sessions were conducted 3 days a week, with 3 sessions conducted per work day at his residential group home in a quiet room with only relevant materials present. Matt, a 9 yr-old boy diagnosed with autism who also has very limited fine motor skills and may require assistance completing simple tasks. This individual also has poor in-session behavior, meaning he has trouble attending, is easily distracted, may attempt to bolt, or will flop on the ground for extended periods of time. His sessions were conducted in a cubby area designated for him at his school. This cubby consisted of a desk with 2 chairs, and a curtain in order to block the view of others walking by. Sessions were conducted 2 days per week and 2 4 sessions were conducted per work day. Procedure To determine which containers would be selected, pre-session assessments were conducted to identify appropriate materials for each student. If the participant could independently open the container more than a couple of times, that session was discontinued and a different session began with new materials. Once an appropriate container was selected, a paired stimulus preference assessment was conducted with each participant to identify a preferred edible to use for the reinforcement conditions.

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 10 In baseline and reinforcement sessions, the participant was given the verbal prompt open, and was then given 5 seconds to open the container. If the participant did not open the container within the initial 5 seconds, the experimenter either modeled the correct response (Alex and Ryan) or manually guided the participant to complete it (Ethan and Matt), and repeated the verbal prompt open. The participant was then given an additional 30 seconds to open the container. If the participant opened the container during the additional 30 seconds, it was considered a correct prompted response. If the participant was unable to open the container during the additional 30-s, it was considered to be an incorrect response. Baseline, direct, and indirect conditions were alternated in a multi-element design for one participant and a multiple baseline design for all others until mastery criteria was met in one of the conditions (9/10 correct independent responding for 3 consecutive sessions). In baseline sessions, there was no consequence for correct or incorrect responding. If the participant emitted the target response, there was a 15-s delay before the initiation of the next trial. In the direct condition, procedures were similar to those described in baseline; however, preferred edible reinforcers were placed inside the clear container. In this condition, correct responding resulted in direct reinforcement as the participant was able to retrieve the edible without social mediation. The indirect condition procedures were also similar to baseline, however, in the indirect condition the experimenter held out a preferred edible approximate 12 inches away from the participant so that it was in their view. Correct responding resulted in the delivery of the edible by the experimenter. Instances of reaching in indirect sessions were ignored and the trail continued, however, the experimenter attempted to prevent the participant from obtaining the edible.

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 11 Dependent Variables The dependent variables in this study are correct responding, reaching and problem behavior. Correct responding was defined as opening the container enough so that the participant could reach inside. If the participant opened the container with-in the additional 30-s, it was considered to be a correct prompted response. Reaching was defined as instances in which the participant reached out for an edible held with-in eye sight, which interfered with the completion of the target response. Problem behavior was defined as any undesirable behavior, such as; aggression, flopping, climbing, or self-injury. Measurement and Interobserver Agreement During pre-sessions and training sessions, data were taken with pen and paper by the instructor conducting the sessions. Data were collected in 10 trial sessions, in which data was taken after each trial. For each trial, the participant could have emitted a correct response, a correct prompted response, or an incorrect response. Data were also taken on interfering behavior that occurred in the indirect condition only which was referred to as reaching. Occurrences of reaching behavior were tallied after each trial to determine if it may have been related to the participant s performance in the indirect condition. Other forms of problem behaviors (e.g. aggression, flopping, climbing, or self-injurious behavior) were also recorded. Interobserver agreement (IOA) was conducted by training a graduate level student to score data from previously videotaped sessions. Percentage of agreement was calculated by dividing the number of sessions with agreement by the number of sessions with agreement and disagreement.

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 12 IOA was collected for 26% of all sessions for Alex, with a mean agreement of 98% (range 90%-100%), and a mean agreement of 100% for reaching. IOA was collected for 38% of all sessions for Ricky, with a mean agreement of 98% (range 90% - 100%) for correct responding, a mean agreement of 100% for reaching, and a mean agreement of 82% (range 80% -90%) for problem behavior. IOA was collected for 29% of all sessions for Matt, with a mean agreement of 100% for correct responding, and a mean agreement of 97% (range of 90% - 100%) for reaching. IOA was collected for 15% of sessions for Ethan with a mean agreement of 97% (range 90% - 100%) for correct responding, and a mean agreement of 100% for reaching. Results Results from this study are shown in figures 1-4. Results for Alex, shown in figure 2, indicate that both the direct and indirect contingencies were equally effective in teaching the target response. Initial baseline sessions resulted in no correct responding, prompted or independent, for 2 consecutive sessions. However, once the reinforcement contingencies were implemented, correct responding was observed in 100% of opportunities in the initial indirect session and each of the subsequent indirect or direct sessions. While levels of correct responding were high in the indirect conditions, little to no instances of reaching were observed. An increasing trend of correct responding was also observed across baseline. Results for Ricky indicate that both reinforcement contingencies were effective in teaching the target response. Initial baseline sessions produced no correct responding, prompted or independent. An immediate increase in correct responding was observed once reinforcement conditions were implemented. While performance appeared to be slightly better in the initial direct sessions, both conditions produced high stable levels of correct responding and were

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 13 effective in teaching the task. Levels of reaching in indirect conditions were low and stable, as only 1 instance occurred in the 4 th session. Problem behavior was also measured for Ryan as it was observed in each of the conditions. Levels of problem behavior were the highest in the baseline sessions, ranging from low to high levels (3 7 instances per session). Problem behavior was observed at low undifferentiated levels in the direct and indirect conditions. Results for Matt indicate that both reinforcement contingencies were effective in teaching the target response. Initial baseline sessions produced no correct responding. In both the direct and indirect condition, the first sessions produced no correct responding. Subsequent sessions were then conducted in a cubby at his school. After the relocation, an immediate increase was observed in both the indirect and direct conditions and remained stable for each remaining session. Reaching occurred at very low levels and was only observed once during the 7 th session. Results for Ethan indicate that both direct and indirect reinforcement were equally effective in teaching the target skill. Initial baseline sessions produced no correct responding, prompted or independent. The initial direct and indirect conditions also produced no correct responses, as a result the prompting procedure was changed as it was determined that a model prompt was not sufficient. As a result a physical prompt was utilized as it was believed to be more appropriate for this individual as he had no history with model prompts. The first session with the physical prompt was an indirect session which produced no correct responses. However, on the subsequent direct session, a high level of correct responses was observed. Each session after that produced 100% correct responding in both the direct and indirect conditions. Reaching occurred at moderate levels in the first indirect session, however, as Ethan acquired the task levels of reaching decreased and remained stable.

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 14 Discussion Results from the current study did not replicate the results of previous research that found direct contingencies to be more effective in teaching than indirect contingencies. While the results that were observed were unexpected, there are several reasons why this may have occurred. First, the majority of the participants in this study have a long history with socially mediated contingencies of reinforcement. As a result, the difference between direct and indirect reinforcement may not have been as significant for these participants. The availability of reinforcement in this case seemed to function as enough of a reinforcer that the arrangement did not have a significant impact on its effectiveness. Another possible explanation for the results is that work itself may have been reinforcing. Since these participants have had such a long history with reinforcement for following instructions, the presentation of a verbal prompt may function as a discriminative stimulus, and completion of the task is automatically reinforced. This could explain the increasing levels of correct responding observed with Alex during baseline. Finally, the simplicity of the tasks taught could explain why similar results were observed. While the individuals who participated in this study couldn t initially complete the task, rapid acquisition was observed across participants under both the direct and indirect conditions. Since this was the case, the participants may have consistently emitted the correct response because of the simplicity of the task and reinforcement delivered or obtained upon completion. In the present study there are a number of potential limitations. First, each of the participants that took part in the study had some sort of history of reinforcement for correctly responding to

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 15 verbal prompts. Therefore, the presentation of work may have been reinforcing itself, as it may have signaled that reinforcement would be delivered. This could possibly explain why both the direct and indirect contingencies were equally effective in teaching the tasks presented. Different results may have been observed if participants with no history of socially mediated reinforcement were included in this study. Second, a change was made in the prompting procedure used for Ethan during the reinforcement sessions. This was done because it was determined that a model prompt was not appropriate for this individual as he had no history with that type of prompt, and did not have the skills in his repertoire to attend to a model prompt. Therefore, a physical prompt was implemented as the researcher believed it would give Ethan the opportunity to acquire the task, and possibly still allow for some difference in the reinforcement contingencies to be shown. While this prompt proved to be sufficient in teaching the task, no difference was noted between the reinforcement contingencies. Therefore, it is difficult to determine whether the prompting procedure or the reinforcement contingencies were responsible for teaching the task, or to what extent each teaching method had on acquisition. Future research should examine the effectiveness of direct and indirect contingencies of reinforcement on individuals who have had no history with socially mediated reinforcement and those who have to determine whether a history of this type of reinforcement contingency has an effect on performance. Results could then be compared to see if it is the presentation of the task itself which is reinforcing or whether it is certain aspects of the direct and indirect contingencies. More complex skills could also possibly be taught under these contingencies in order to determine whether or not one is more effective than the other. More complex tasks may take

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 16 more time for the participant to acquire the skill and may allow for some differentiation to be observed between the direct and indirect contingencies. More complex tasks may also result in longer session length which might evoke problem behaviors such as reaching or other problem behaviors which could influence performance under each condition. Future research should also examine whether or not socially mediated reinforcement necessary to teach new skills. If reinforcement is necessary, to what extent do you need to utilize it in order to effectively teach? And if reinforcement is not necessary, to what extent can it be removed. Work itself may be reinforcing for some individual s with a history of reinforcement for following instructions, but if a particular response is put on extinction, how long would the individual continue to emit that response? Finally, future research should continue to compare these contingencies, and if differences in their effectiveness are observed, sources of stimulus control that may enhance or hinder these contingencies should be examined. In doing so, we could then apply the findings to further improve methods of reinforcement by limiting possible inhibiting properties of indirect contingencies and by utilizing the possible enhancing properties of direct contingencies.

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 17 References DeLeon, I.G. & Iwata, B.A. (1996). Evaluation of a multiple-stimulus presentation format for assessing reinforcer preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 519-533. Fisher, W., Piazza, C.C., Bowman, L.G., Hagopian, L.P., Owens, J.C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A comparison of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for persons with with severe and profound disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491-498. Koegel, R.L., & Williams, J.A. (1980). Direct versus indirect response-reinforcer relationships in teaching autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 8, 537-547. Thompson, R.H., & Iwata, B.A. (2000). Response acquisition under direct and indirect contingencies of reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 1-11. Williams, J.A., Koegel, R.L., & Egel, A.L. (1981). Response-reinforcer relationships and improved learning in autistic children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 53-60.

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 18 Acknowledgement The author would like to thank the New England Center for Children for providing the opportunity to conduct research on the present topic. The author would also like to thank Jessica Sassi, for her motivation, constant support, and encouragement throughout the study. The author would also like to thank Shawn Kenyon and Julie Weiss, for their constant support and input that they had on this study.

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 19 Figure 1. Results from the paired stimulus preference assessments conducted with all 4 participants before sessions began.

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 20 Figure 2. Results from the multi-elementotal number of correct responding, both prompted and unprompted, in all conditions. Bottom design conducted with Alex only. Top graph depicts the graph depicts instances of reaching behavior.

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 21 Figure 3. Results for Ricky, Matt, and Ethan, shown in a multiple baseline design, showing the total number of correct responses in baseline, and direct and indirect conditions.

Direct and Indirect Reinforcement 22 Figure 4. Data representing instances of reaching for Ricky, Matt and Ethan, and problem behavior.