THE DIMENSIONS OF DRIVER PERFORMANCE DURING SECONDARY MANUAL TASKS

Similar documents
Driver Distraction: Towards A Working Definition

Naturalistic Driving Performance During Secondary Tasks

Distracted Driving among Teens. What We Know about It and How to Prevent It May 31 st, 2017

Hazard Perception and Distraction in Novice Drivers: Effects of 12 Months Driving Experience

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

Mitigating cognitive distraction and its effects with interface design and collision avoidance systems

EFFECTS OF DISTRACTION TYPE, DRIVER AGE, AND ROADWAY ENVIRONMENT ON REACTION TIMES AN ANALYSIS USING SHRP-2 NDS DATA

Crash Risk Analysis of Distracted Driving Behavior: Influence of Secondary Task Engagement and Driver Characteristics

Estimation of Driver Inattention to Forward Objects Using Facial Direction with Application to Forward Collision Avoidance Systems

Evaluating the Safety of Verbal Interface Use while Driving

Changing Driver Behavior Through Unconscious Stereotype Activation

Driven to Distraction? A Review of Speech Technologies in the Automobile

OPTIC FLOW IN DRIVING SIMULATORS

Detecting and Reading Text on HUDs: Effects of Driving Workload and Message Location

Driver Distraction. Bryan Reimer. Auto UI Tutorials October 17, 2012

Distracted Driving. Stephanie Bonne, MD

Driver Alertness Detection Research Using Capacitive Sensor Array

The Effects of Age and Distraction on Reaction Time in a Driving Simulator

DISTRACTION AN ACADEMIC PERSPECTIVE. Steve Reed Loughborough Design School

1.0 Executive Summary Introduction Figure Monroe and North Street Analysis of Traffic Data... 4

A FIELD STUDY ASSESSING DRIVING PERFORMANCE, VISUAL ATTENTION, HEART RATE AND SUBJECTIVE RATINGS IN RESPONSE TO TWO TYPES OF COGNITIVE WORKLOAD

Research in Progress on Distracted Driving

Assessment of police subjective workload and preference for using a voice-based interface during simulated driving

December 7, Ian J. Reagan David G. Kidd. Jonathan Dobres Bruce Mehler Bryan Reimer MIT AgeLab, New England University Transportation Center

Distracted Driving Effects on CMV Operators

In-Vehicle Communication and Driving: An Attempt to Overcome their Interference

Annals of Advances in Automotive Medicine

Task Report: On-Road Study of Willingness to Engage in Distracting Tasks

PREVENTING DISTRACTED DRIVING. Maintaining Focus Behind the Wheel of a School Bus

Examining Distracted Drivers Underestimation of Time and Overestimation of Speed

Sensation Seeking and Drivers Glance Behavior while Engaging in a Secondary Task

The Misjudgment of Risk due to Inattention

Driving with ADD/ADHD. Presenter: Evan Levene, AAA Club Alliance Driving Instructor

Application of ecological interface design to driver support systems

Measuring IVIS Impact to Driver by On-road Test and Simulator Experiment Chenjiang Xie a,b, * Tong Zhu a,b Chunlin Guo a,b Yimin Zhang a a

The Danger of Incorrect Expectations In Driving: The Failure to Respond

Cell-Phone Induced Driver Distraction David L. Strayer and Frank A. Drews

optimising the value of sound quality evaluations by observing assessors driving strategies

8/26/2013 DMV UCSD CHP. Presenters. Law Enforcement s Identification and Referral of Medically Impaired Older Drivers. NHTSA Priority.

Effects of Driver and Secondary Task Characteristics on Lane Change Test Performance

The Effect of Glare in Shnulated Night Driving

Positioning Eye Fixation and Vehicle Movement: Visual-motor Coordination Assessment in Naturalistic Driving

Multimodal Driver Displays: Potential and Limitations. Ioannis Politis

Effects of Visual and Cognitive Distraction on Lane Change Test Performance


Distraction Related Accidents: Eyes on Road, Hands on Wheel, AND Mind on Task Sarah Wrenn, ThinkReliability

Policy Research CENTER

The safety and security of OnStar is available to TTY (Text Telephone) users. Welcome to OnStar.

1. Find the appropriate value for constructing a confidence interval in each of the following settings:

How Common In-Car Distractions Affect Driving Performance in Simple and Complex Road Environments

American Society of Highway. Beardsley Connector Project. Jacob Dean

Does order matter? Investigating the effect of. of sequence on glance duration during on-road. driving. Abstract. Introduction

Driver Behaviour Issues relevant to Temporary Traffic Management solutions

Capturing Driver Response to In-Vehicle Human- Machine Interface Technologies Using Facial Thermography

THE TACTILE DETECTION RESPONSE TASK: PRELIMINARY VALIDATION FOR MEASURING THE ATTENTIONAL EFFECTS OF COGNITIVE LOAD

Verbal Collision Avoidance Messages of Varying Perceived Urgency Reduce Crashes in High Risk Scenarios

Undiagnosed ADHD Among Unionized Drivers in Ghana: Public Health and Policy Implications

In-Car Information Systems: Matching and Mismatching Personality of Driver with Personality of Car Voice

Agents and Environments

95% of all injuries are behavioral 05% of all injuries are mechanical

Young Adults with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): The impact of secondary tasks on driving performance

PROCEEDINGS of the Eighth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design

Southern Legislative Conference: Why Focus on Distraction in Transportation? Chairman Deborah A. P. Hersman

THE EFFECT OF VOICE INTERACTIONS ON DRIVERS GUIDANCE OF ATTENTION

ABC OF BRIDGE NO. 465 I-195 Ramp (Dr-2) Over Warren Avenue

Characterizing Visual Attention during Driving and Non-driving Hazard Perception Tasks in a Simulated Environment

Exploring the Factors that Impact Injury Severity using Hierarchical Linear Modeling (HLM)

Does Workload Modulate the Effects of In-Vehicle Display Location on Concurrent Driving and Side Task Performance?

Driving Under the Influence of Alcohol

Error Detection based on neural signals

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING

August 15, Prepared for: LOUISIANA HIGHWAY SAFETY COMMISSION Lisa Freeman, Executive Director Post Office Box Baton Rouge, LA 70896

Efficient Measurement of Large Light Source Near-field Color and Luminance Distributions for Optical Design and Simulation

Adjusted Crash Odds Ratio Estimates of Driver Behavior Errors: A Re-Analysis of the SHRP2 Naturalistic Driving Study Data

Sight Distance AMRC 2012 MODULE 7 CONTENTS

Trucker sleep patterns influence driving performance

Classifying Cognitive Load and Driving Situation with Machine Learning

Koji Sakai. Kyoto Koka Women s University, Ukyo-ku Kyoto, Japan

Prevalence of Drowsy-Driving Crashes: Estimates from a Large-Scale Naturalistic Driving Study

The John D. Rockefeller IV Career Center Adult and Community Programs. Daytime and Evening Courses. Fall 2013

Distracted Driving: Review of Current Needs, Efforts and Recommended Strategies OVERVIEW

Birsen Donmez, Linda Ng. Boyle, John D. Lee and Daniel V. McGehee 1

Protecting Workers with Smart E-Vests

I just didn t see it It s all about hazard perception. Dr. Robert B. Isler Associate Professor School of Psychology University of Waikato Hamilton

Don't Just Look for Your Lost Keys Under the Street Light : Engaging Workplaces to Improve Health and Safety

Sanako Lab 100 STS USER GUIDE

Intelligent Agents. Chapter 2 ICS 171, Fall 2009

How Safe Are Our Roads? 2016 Checkpoint Strikeforce campaign poster celebrating real area cab drivers as being Beautiful designated sober drivers.

22c:145 Artificial Intelligence

Usage of a Driving Simulator in the Design Process of new HMI concepts for eco-driving Applications (ecomove project)

04/12/2014. Research Methods in Psychology. Chapter 6: Independent Groups Designs. What is your ideas? Testing

Technical Report UMTRI June Frequency of Distracting Tasks People Do while Driving: An Analysis of the ACAS FOT Data

Case Study on a Worksite Sleep Disorder Program for Commercial Motor Vehicle Drivers

Detecting distraction and degraded driver performance with visual behavior metrics

Empirical Research Methods for Human-Computer Interaction. I. Scott MacKenzie Steven J. Castellucci

Cell Phones and Driving 1. Why Do Cell Phone Conversations Interfere With Driving?

WHITE PAPER. Efficient Measurement of Large Light Source Near-Field Color and Luminance Distributions for Optical Design and Simulation

Chapter 7 Guided Notes. Alcohol, Other Drugs and Driving. It is categorized as a because of the effects it has on the.

APPENDIX FOR "DRIVING SIMULATOR FOR PERSONS WITH IMPAIRED COGNITION", PI WILLIAM K DURFEE

Transcription:

THE DIMENSIONS OF DRIVER PERFORMANCE DURING SECONDARY MANUAL TASKS Richard A. Young, Linda S. Angell General Motors Engineering Warren, MI USA 48090-9055 1 INTRODUCTION Drivers manage multiple tasks: Primary driving tasks Steer, accelerate, brake, choose and maintain speed and lane, navigate, monitor hazards Secondary tasks Today - converse with other passengers, eat, drink, groom, listen to audio entertainment, use portable devices, place phone call, adjust mirrors, set cruise control, access OnStar, etc. New communicate, entertain, navigate, Internet tasks, address book tasks, etc. 2

ISSUES How determine which secondary tasks may interfere with primary driving tasks? Without an answer, how is it possible to: Minimize driver distraction? Develop design and driver performance standards for secondary tasks? What key dimensions of driver performance provide a complete and valid assessment of secondary task effects on primary driving performance? Numerous measures of driver performance have been explored, and intercorrelations between measures have been described. By examining the structure within those correlations, it is possible to identify the underlying driver performance dimensions. 3 OBJECTIVE AND SCOPE The main objective was to determine the underlying dimensions of driver performance during the conduct of secondary manual tasks inside a moving vehicle. The scope of the study was restricted to: Tasks that could be performed using the eyes and hands (visualmanual) Visual-manual is most common in vehicles today, and so was deemed of importance to study first, compared to voice, haptic or other methods. Only one secondary task at a time It was felt necessary to understand the effect of single secondary tasks first, before studying them in combination. 4

METHODS Subjects Equal numbers of licensed drivers assigned to two age groups: 18-34 and 45-65 years. Equal numbers of males and females assigned to each experimental condition (vehicle system), within each age group. A total of 81 paid subjects participated in the on-road study. Vehicles and Vehicle Systems Five vehicles: four sedans, one sport-utility vehicle. One manufacturer made four of the vehicles, and another the fifth. Different in-vehicle information systems had been installed in four of the vehicles. One vehicle did not have an information system but had a prototype radio interface. Three of the systems were experimental prototypes for demonstration purposes, not scheduled for production then or now. The system without the information function is now in production, and the fifth was a commercial system. 5 METHODS: SECONDARY TASKS The tasks were selected to represent typical tasks that drivers would do in a vehicle with the functions available to them for a given system. 79 tasks chosen were selected from the categories of: Communication (e.g., phone dialing) Entertainment (radio, CD, information) Navigation (e.g., route destination entry) Internet tasks (e.g., get weather, sports stories) Address book (e.g. data entry) Conventional tasks that are commonly done in the vehicle today 6

METHODS: EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN The main independent variable manipulated between subjects was Vehicle System (5 total). Twelve to 18 drivers were stratified by age (younger, older) and gender (male, female) within each of the five vehicle systems. The main independent variable manipulated within subjects was Tasks (13 to 18 per vehicle system). 7 METHODS: DEPENDENT VARIABLES # Variable Name 1 Task Completion Time tasktime 2 Eyes-Off-Road Time eort 3 Number of Glances to the In-Vehicle System glances 4 Number of Lane Deviations lanedev 5 Subjective Workload workload 6 Subjective Situation Unawareness sit_unaw 7 Number of Speed Deviations speeddev 8 Percent Unsuccessful Task Completion per_unsu 9 Percent of Total Visual Events Missed allmiss 10 Percent of Forward Visual Events Missed hoodmiss 11 Percent of Side Visual Events Missed sidemiss 12 Mean Single Glance Time to System glncedur 13 Time to Respond to Total Visual Events evnttime 14 Time to Respond to Side Visual Events sidetime 15 Time to Respond to Forward Visual Events hoodtime 8

METHOD: FACILITIES Center portion of Virginia Tech Transportation Institute (VTTI) Smart Road: 1.0-mile, two-lane roadway Turn-arounds at each end used for practice sessions Curves, hills and bridges, but no intersections Roadside ravines and/or hillsides on both sides of the roadway, as well as bridges and bridge abutments, meant that the driving experience was more challenging than on a closed-course test track or straightaway on which lane wander might have little practical consequence. The road was closed to traffic other than the vehicles involved in the testing. 9 METHOD: APPARATUS Visual Events Detection Task The instrumentation for this task consisted of two red lights mounted on the vehicle (intended conceptually to emulate visual roadway events such as a vehicle braking), together with software to control their presentation. One or the other light randomly turned on three to six seconds after the brake pedal was pressed in response to a prior light event. Which light came on in that period (hood or side) was also randomly determined. A light remained on until the driver responded to it. Responses longer than 3.5 seconds were recorded as a miss. 10

METHOD: APPARATUS Peripheral Light Eye Camera Central Light 11 PROCEDURE All subjects were given a demo trial, then a training trial, for every task while in the training area. The training for each task was done immediately before the road test trial. This procedure, as per previous investigations with this method (not shown), typically produces a level of training that is likely at or near the maximum of the short-term learning curve for that particular task for that subject. Further details of the experimental procedure are given in: Angell, L. S., Young, R. A., Hankey, J. M. and Dingus, T. A. (2002) An Evaluation of Alternative Methods for Assessing Driver Workload in the Early Development of In-Vehicle Information Systems, SAE Proceedings, 2002-01-1981, Government/Industry Meeting, May 15, Washington, DC, USA 12

PROCEDURE: ROAD TEST TRIAL 13 DATA PROCEDURES Each variable for each task was averaged across all subjects tested for that task, yielding a 79 x 15 input data matrix. A 15 x 15 correlation matrix between the variables was then calculated across the 79 tasks. Principal Component Analysis (PCA) was then used to reduce the dimensionality of the standardized data set (i.e. each variable is normalized to a mean of zero and a standard deviation of one) to the major orthogonal dimensions. 14

RESULTS: CORRELATIONS 1 tasktime 2 eort 3 glances 4 lanedev 5 workload 6 sit_unaw 7 speeddev 1 tasktime 1.000.984.989.923.931.910.914.876.576.521.462.521.393.312.281 2 eort.984 1.000.989.920.917.895.875.882.564.508.464.583.369.289.273 3 glances.989.989 1.000.922.935.911.885.866.566.499.462.535.352.276.247 4 lanedev.923.920.922 1.000.882.846.820.797.578.517.477.504.400.336.269 5 workload.931.917.935.882 1.000.985.815.775.540.459.451.479.374.299.211 6 sit_unaw.910.895.911.846.985 1.000.799.742.509.439.417.470.367.294.200 7 speeddev.914.875.885.820.815.799 1.000.823.527.500.397.420.309.229.224 8 per_unsu.876.882.866.797.775.742.823 1.000.431.431.358.407.240.210.209 9 allmiss.576.564.566.578.540.509.527.431 1.000.775.849.544.673.534.541 10 hoodmiss.521.508.499.517.459.439.500.431.775 1.000.460.394.512.346.568 11 sidemiss.462.464.462.477.451.417.397.358.849.460 1.000.487.587.605.376 12 glncedur.521.583.535.504.479.470.420.407.544.394.487 1.000.412.307.316 13 evnttime.393.369.352.400.374.367.309.240.673.512.587.412 1.000.812.734 14 sidetime.312.289.276.336.299.294.229.210.534.346.605.307.812 1.000.383 15 hoodtime.281.273.247.269.211.200.224.209.541.568.376.316.734.383 1.000 8 per_unsu 9 allmiss 10 hoodmiss 11 sidemiss 12 glncedur 13 evnttime 14 sidetime 15 hoodtime Table 1. Correlation matrix between all 15 variables across all 79 tasks. Bolded r-values are statistically significant at p < 0.000001 (r > 0.522). 15 MAIN RESULT Using Principal Component Analysis, all the redundancy between variables in the correlation matrix was removed the PCs are orthogonal. The information was simplified and represented in the first three principal component dimensions, without loss of significant information. The three dimensions taken together can explain 83% of the total variation (in all the tasks and all the variables), which is deemed substantial. 16

RESULTS: DIMENSION 1 (61%) Loading 0.35 0.30 0.25 0.20 0.15 0.10 0.05 0.00 1 TaskTime 2 EORT 3 Glances 4 LaneDev 5 Workload 6 Sit_UnAw 7 SpeedDev 8 Per_unsu 9 AllMiss 10 HoodMiss Variable 11 SideMiss 12 GlnceDur 13 EvntTime 14 SideTime 15 HoodTime 17 RESULTS: DIMENSION 2 (17%) Loading 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0.0-0.1-0.2-0.3 1 TaskTime 2 EORT 3 Glances 4 LaneDev 5 Workload 6 Sit_UnAw 7 SpeedDev 8 Per_unsu 9 AllMiss 10 HoodMiss Variable 11 SideMiss 12 GlnceDur 13 EvntTime 14 SideTime 15 HoodTime 18

RESULTS: DIMENSION 3 (5%) 0.6 0.4 Loading 0.2 0.0-0.2-0.4-0.6 1 TaskTime 2 EORT 3 Glances 4 LaneDev 5 Workload 6 Sit_UnAw 7 SpeedDev 8 Per_unsu 9 AllMiss 10 HoodMiss Variable 11 SideMiss 12 GlnceDur 13 EvntTime 14 SideTime 15 HoodTime 19 RESULTS: DIM 2 x DIM 1 20

RESULTS: TASK SCORES The 79 tasks can now be scored in terms of the three orthogonal dimensions, instead of the 15 original variables. Control limits can now be established on the scores in each of the three primary dimensions, to determine if tasks are inside or outside control limits for acceptable driving performance. 21 DISCUSSION Dimension 1 is interpreted as overall driver demand. Dimension 2 is interpreted as low-workload-but-highinattentiveness. It specifies tasks that make drivers less attentive or more insensitive to outside events than expected, given their visual-manual workload, and contrasts them with tasks for which attentiveness is greater than expected for their given visual-manual workload. Likely associated with a larger class of driver attentional phenomena termed mind-off-road. Dimension 3 is interpreted as peripheral insensitivity. Opposing responsiveness for peripheral vs. central events. Possible narrowing of visual attention to the central visual field. 22

BENEFITS 1. Replaces a large set of variables by only three dimensions. The tasks can be represented as points in a 3-dimensional space formed by those dimensions. 2. May reduce errors in classifying tasks as inside or outside control limits for acceptable driving performance. May reduce false positives that claim a task is outside control limits when it really is inside control limits. By looking at fewer dimensions than variables there is less scope for false positive errors. Reduces customer frustration from false lockouts. May reduce false negatives that claim a task is inside control limits when it really is outside control limits. Multivariate methods may find different types of outliers (tasks outside of control limits) not seen by the original variables. Enables early detection of issues and improved design of products. 23 CONCLUSIONS 1. Overall driver demand as defined by Principal Component Analysis is a separate driver performance dimension from low-workload-but-highinattentiveness, and vice versa; peripheral insensitivity is an added dimension, separate from the first two. 2. These three dimensions by themselves can explain a substantial portion of the total variation in all the tasks and all the variables studied. 3. Scoring tasks on these dimensions instead of the original variables may reduce errors in classifying tasks as inside or outside control limits for acceptable driving performance. 4. No single variable by itself (e.g. task time, peripheral light % misses) can capture all the important variations in driver performance during secondary invehicle manual tasks. 5. A multivariate design and analysis is required. 24