Effort Invested in Cognitive Tasks by Adults with Aphasia: A Pilot Study

Similar documents
How difficult is it? How well Adults with Aphasia Perceive Task Demands

INTRODUCTION METHODS

INTRODUCTION PURPOSE METHOD

Exploring the relationship between high level anomia, attention and cognitive processing deficits: a retrospective data analysis

The inner workings of working memory: Preliminary data from unimpaired populations

INTRODUCTION Use of images in multiple-choice tasks is common in linguistic comprehension

TITLE: Acquisition and generalization responses in aphasia treatment: Evidence from sentence-production treatment

Maximizing Generalization Effects of Semantic Feature Analysis

Background. Can APT-3 Improve Reading Impairment in Mild Aphasia?

Effects of a sensory intake task on heart rate and heart rate variability

Effects of Response Elaboration Training on Increased Length and Complexity of Utterances with Two Participants with Fluent Aphasia

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

The effect of word familiarity and treatment approach on word retrieval skills in aphasia

Consolidating working memory: Enhancing cognitive performance through effective encoding

Introduction Persons with aphasia who are trained to generate abstract words (e.g., justice) in a specific context-category (e.g.

Key Words: aphasia, working memory, memory measures

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis

Critical Review: Do mind-body therapies improve language outcomes in persons with aphasia?*

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction

3/23/2017 ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT NEEDS OF THE INDIVIDUAL WITH A TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY: A SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGIST S PERSPECTIVE

Clinical Aphasiology Submission 2006

Does aromatherapy actually work? Erica Cousins. The autonomic nervous system is made up of two parts. The sympathetic

Repetition-priming effect: a cognitive task for the definition of a clinical assessment

VISUAL MEMORY. Visual Perception

Validation of ipad based treatment 1

Deciphering Psychological-Physiological Mappings While Driving and Performing a Secondary Memory Task

Jan Kaiser, Andrzej Beauvale and Jarostaw Bener. Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, 13 Golcbia St., ?

References. Embretson, S. E. & Reise, S. P. (2000). Item response theory for psychologists. Mahwah,

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

CHAPTER 6: Memory model Practice questions at - text book pages 112 to 113

Critical Review: Effectiveness of EMA in improving articulatory accuracy in adults with AOS

Combination Treatment in Aphasia: Unilateral Nostril Breathing and Language Therapy

Repetition blindness is immune to the central bottleneck

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description

Measuring Changes in Quality of Life in Persons with Aphasia: Is Communication Confidence a Good Measure?

Sensory Memory Systems. Visual Store. PDF created with pdffactory trial version

Methods. Participants

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1

Effects of Task Relevance and Stimulus-Driven Salience in Feature-Search Mode

(SAT). d) inhibiting automatized responses.

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1

Laboratory Investigation of Human Cognition. Name. Reg. no. Date of submission

1. Processes nutrients and provides energy for the neuron to function; contains the cell's nucleus; also called the soma.

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set

Perceptual Load in Different Regions of the Visual Field and its Effect on Attentional Selectivity. Hadas Marciano Advisor: Yaffa Yeshurun

Enhanced visual perception near the hands

Attention and Scene Perception

Pushing around the Locus of Selection: Evidence for the Flexible-selection Hypothesis

Introduction Stories or narratives are a common discourse genre embedded in everyday conversation (Norrick, 2010). It has been proposed that a key

Verbal Working Memory. The left temporoparietal junction in verbal working memory: Storage or attention. Baddelely s Multiple-Component Model

Memory search for the first target modulates the magnitude of the attentional blink

Cognitive Communication Disorders

A Return to the Gorilla What Effects What

Using contextual analysis to investigate the nature of spatial memory

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Perceptual grouping determines the locus of attentional selection

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

The Gaze Cueing Paradigm with Eye Tracking Background Set-up Lab

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

The Neuropsychology of

Word priming in schizophrenia: Associational and semantic influences

Research Review: Multiple Resource Theory. out in multi-task environments. Specifically, multiple display layout and control design

Selective attention and cyclopean motion processing

Does Wernicke's Aphasia necessitate pure word deafness? Or the other way around? Or can they be independent? Or is that completely uncertain yet?

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

Timing & Schizophrenia. Deana Davalos Colorado State University

Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms

Modules 4 & 6. The Biology of Mind

LANGUAGE IN INDIA Strength for Today and Bright Hope for Tomorrow Volume 8 : 2 February 2008

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

Method. Procedures and Measures.

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK

The number line effect reflects top-down control

Long-Term Recovery Outcomes in Aphasia

observed. In spite of the fact that D.H. demonstrated minimal definitive changes on the conversational measures selected, by the end of both

A model of parallel time estimation

The rapid extraction of numeric meaning

CENTRAL EXECUTIVE FUNCTION IN INDIVIDUALS WITH APHASIA. Artemis Alexandra Basilakos. Under the direction of Rebecca J. Shisler Marshall ABSTRACT

On the failure of distractor inhibition in the attentional blink

Practical Application using Behavior-Based Ergonomics Therapy (BBET) Program

Beyond the Psychologist s Report. Nancy Foster, PhD Institute for Brain-Behavior Integration

Chapter 4. Two Types of Attention. Selective Listening 25/09/2012. Paying Attention. How does selective attention work?

Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation

Revealing The Brain s Hidden Potential: Cognitive Training & Neurocognitive Plasticity. Introduction

Chen, Z., & O Neill, P. (2001). Processing demand modulates the effect of spatial

birds were used. Every two weeks during treatment, all thirty-two items were probed. A final probe was collected six to seven weeks post-treatment.

3/25/2010. Multitasking

Short-term memory refers to the ability to temporarily

Effect of Exemplar Typicality on Naming Deficits in Aphasia

AUDITORY GRAPHS: THE EFFECTS OF REDUNDANT DIMENSIONS AND DIVIDED ATTENTION. S. Camille Peres and David M. Lane

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

The path of visual attention

Evidence against a central bottleneck during the attentional blink: Multiple channels for configural and featural processing q

Process of a neuropsychological assessment

Mitigation of Proper Name Retrieval Impairments in Temporal Lobe Epilepsy

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences

Transcription:

Effort Invested in Cognitive Tasks by Adults with Aphasia: A Pilot Study Introduction Impaired performance by individuals with aphasia (IWA) on language tasks may be partially due to an impaired ability to allocate cognitive resources to the tasks (McNeil, 1981, McNeil et al., 1991). Others have explained this deficit as an impaired ability to match effort with task demands (e.g. Clark & Robin, 1991). The terms effort and resources are frequently used interchangeably (Kahneman, 1973). Kahneman (1973) states that effort refers to mental energy or attentional resources allocated to a task. According to Brehm s theory (Brehm & Self, 1989) effort expenditure is directly dependent on task difficulty. Clark and Robin (1991) found that their IWA, compared to control participants, had no change in sense of effort on a lexical decision (LD) task that increased in difficulty. Murray and colleagues (1997) assessed IWA s ratings of task difficulty. They also compared participants performance on a LD task to performance on the LD task when combined with a semantic distractor task. Although performance declined in the dual task condition, IWA s ratings of task difficulty did not change. Findings from these studies suggest that IWA do not invest effort appropriately because they inadequately perceive the task demands. If language performance by IWA is affected by effort allocated, this could have important clinical implications. To date, only subjective ratings of effort and task difficulty have been investigated. A direct, physiological measure of effort is needed to explore this disconnect between performance and IWA s ratings of perceived effort and task difficulty. A well-studied measure of effort allocated to cognitively demanding tasks is heart rate variability (HRV). HRV is the amount of fluctuation around the mean heart rate. It has been shown to reflect the mental workload required during cognitive tasks (e.g., Hansen et al., 2003) and is a commonly used measure of effort (e.g., Aasman et al., 1987). Cognitively challenging tasks elicit a stress response measured as a drop in HRV from baseline to task conditions (Kalsbeek, 1971). The resulting decrease in HRV provides an objective physiological measure of the effort allocated to the tasks and should provide insight into the effort allocated by IWA on verbal and spatial working memory (WM) tasks. We have adopted the operational definition of effort as the difference in HRV (.07-.14 Hz range) measured during the WM tasks from HRV measured during a resting state. The objective of this study was to quantify cognitive effort IWA and control participants dedicate to verbal compared with spatial working memory tasks using HRV. Researchers have found that non-brain injured adults physiological stress response is not affected by task type (Callister, Suwarno, & Seals, 1992), but by task difficulty (Fairclough & Houston, 2004; Gendolla & Richter, 2006; Iani, et al., 2004; Ryu & Myung, 2005). Assuming IWA have an impaired ability to allocate effort to the tasks, it was predicted that they would demonstrate no change in HRV from baseline to task for either verbal or spatial tasks. Further, a significant positive relationship between change in HRV and task performance was predicted for control participants, but none was expected for IWA. Method Participants Participants included 13 IWA and 21 age- and education-matched control participants. All participants demonstrated normal hearing and vision and no depression at the time of testing. Additional inclusion criteria for IWA included a history of unilateral left hemisphere stroke and

presence of aphasia as indicated by performance on the Western Aphasia Battery-Revised (WAB-R; Kertesz, 2006). Tasks The experimental tasks included verbal and spatial n-back tasks that varied in processing load. The n-back task requires participants to decide whether each stimulus in a sequence matches the one that appeared n items ago; it requires temporary storage and manipulation of information, while, continuously updating the contents in WM (Jonides, et al., 1997). All participants completed three n-back tasks a 0-back, 1-back, and 2-back for verbal and spatial stimuli. The verbal stimuli consisted of eight letters presented one at a time in the center of the screen. Participants responded as quickly as possible by pressing the spacebar when the letter was the same as the letter n back. The spatial stimuli included black circles presented on a white background in eight different locations spaced in an octagon fashion around a central fixation point. One dot appeared on the screen at a time. Participants respond to the spatial tasks by pressing the spacebar when the dot was in the same location as the one n back. Stimulus onset asynchronies (SOA) of 3500 ms, shown to be effective for use with IWA (Wright, et al., 2007), were used in all n-back tasks. Each n-back contained a practice block. The experimental conditions included 33 targets presented in a single block containing 100 stimuli. Task difficulty order was counterbalanced, and stimuli type was counterbalanced within difficulty levels. Each n-back was preceded and followed by a 5-minute baseline rest period for baseline HRV data recording. Procedures Participants were instructed to avoid smoking, caffeine, alcohol, and strenuous exercising on the testing day, and also completed a brief questionnaire describing any possible deviations. Prior to testing, 3 surface electrodes were placed on the torso to record ECG activity. ECG activity was recorded continuously using BIOPAC Student Labs (BSL) PRO MP35 recording unit with BSLPro software (500 samples/second, Filter.05 -.35 Hz). Participants sat in a comfortable, high back chair during recording. During pre- and post-task baseline conditions, participants rested quietly with their eyes open. The start and end of experimental and rest conditions were marked using the BSLPro software. Results and Conclusions One-way repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted for each group to determine whether the.07 -.14 HRV frequency range was sensitive to differences between baseline and task conditions. The.07 -.14 Hz range was significant for both the control, F (1, 112) = 121.06, p <.001, partial eta squared =.52, and aphasia groups, F (1, 55) = 19.51, p <.001, partial eta squared =.26. HRV significantly decreased from baseline to task conditions indicating both groups allocated effort to the tasks. Correlation analyses were conducted to determine the relationship between task difficulty and change in HRV in the.07-.14 frequency range. Correlations were non-significant for both groups for verbal and spatial tasks. Both participant groups allocated effort to the verbal and experimental tasks as demonstrated by a drop in HRV from baseline to task. However, HRV was not sensitive to

differences in task demands. Because HRV is determined by the dynamic relationship among two disparate branches of the autonomic nervous system (sympathetic and parasympathetic), it is important to consider factors not related to the experiment that may affect HRV. Medications with anticholinergic properties can affect parasympathetic activity, and beta-blockers decrease the sympathetic input to the heart. Both types of medications are common in older adults; and, particularly individuals who have had strokes. All of the IWA were taking either anticholinergic medications or beta-blockers. Further, more than half of the control participants (14 of 21) were taking medications with anticholinergic effects or beta-blockers. Possibly, the effects of the medications on HRV mask potential significant findings related to task difficulty. Results will be discussed within a theoretical framework of WM and clinical implications will be considered.

References Aasman, J., Mulder, G., & Mulder, L. J. M. (1987). Operator effort and the measurement of heart rate variability. Human Factors, 29, 161-170. Brehm, J. W., & Self, E. A. (1989). The intensity of motivation. In M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual Review of Psychology (pp.109 131). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews, Inc. Callister, R., Suwarno, N. O., & Seals, D. R. (1992). Sympathetic activity is influenced by task difficulty and stress perception during mental challenge in humans. Journal of Physiology, 454, 373-387. Clark, H. M. & Robin, D. A. (1995). Sense of effort during a lexical decision task: Resource allocation deficits following brain injury. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 4, 143-147. Fairclough, S. H., & Houston, K. (2004). A metabolic measure of mental effort. Biological Psychology, 66, 177 190. Gendolla, G. H. E., & Richter, M. (2006). Cardiovascular reactivity during performance under social observation: The moderating role of task difficulty. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 62,185 192. Hansen, A. L., Johnsen, B. H., & Thayer, J. F., (2003). Vagal influence on working memory and attention. International Journal of Psychophysiology 48, 263 274. Iani, C., Gopher, D., & Lavie, P. (2004). Effects of task difficulty and invested mental effort on peripheral vasoconstriction. Psychophysiology, 41, 789 798. Jonides, J., Lauber, E. J., Awh, E., Satoshi, M., & Koeppe, R. A. (1997). Verbal working memory load affects: Regional brain activation as measured by PET. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 462-475. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Kalsbeek, J. W. (1971). Standards of acceptable load in ATC tasks. Ergonomics, 14, 641-650. Kertesz, A. (2006). Western Aphasia Battery-Revised. New York: Grune and Stratton. McNeil, M. R. (1981). Auditory comprehension in aphasia: A language deficit or reduced efficiency of processes supporting language? Clinical Aphasiology, 10, 342 345. McNeil, M. R., Odell, K., & Tseng, C. H. (1991). Toward an integration of resource allocation into a general theory of aphasia. Clinical Aphasiology, 21, 21-39.

Murray, L. L., Holland, A. L., & Beeson, P. M. (1997). Accuracy monitoring and task demand evaluation in aphasia. Aphasiology, 11, 401-414. Ryu, K., & Myung, R. (2005). Evaluation of mental workload with a combined measure based on physiological indices during a dual task of tracking and mental arithmetic. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 35, 991 1009. Wright, H. H., Downey, R. A., Gravier, M., Love, T., & Shapiro, L. P. (2007). Processing distinct linguistic information types in working memory in aphasia. Aphasiology, 21, 802-813.