NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neural Netw. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2007 November 1.

Similar documents
FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

Attentional Control 1. Identifying the neural systems of top-down attentional control: A meta-analytic approach

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response

Visual perception depends on the quality of sensory information

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso

Pre-target activity in visual cortex predicts behavioral performance on spatial and feature attention tasks

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2

1 Decoding attentional orientation from EEG spectra. Ramesh Srinivasan, Samuel Thorpe, Siyi Deng, Tom Lappas & Michael D'Zmura

The Effect of Spatial Attention on Contrast Response Functions in Human Visual Cortex

Models of Attention. Models of Attention

Hebbian Plasticity for Improving Perceptual Decisions

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131

Two-object attentional interference depends on attentional set

Corrections PNAS September 5, 2006 vol. 103 no cgi doi pnas

University of California- Los Angeles. Affiliations: University of California- Los Angeles

Signal detection theory. Direct comparison of perception and neural activity. ? Choice probability. Visual stimulus. Behavioral judgement

A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences

Sensory Physiology Bi353 Fall Term 2016

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Selective Attention. Modes of Control. Domains of Selection

A coincidence detector neural network model of selective attention

Visual area MT responds to local motion. Visual area MST responds to optic flow. Visual area STS responds to biological motion.

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

Visual Attention and the Role of Normalization

Anticipatory and Stimulus-Evoked Blood Oxygenation Level-Dependent Modulations Related to Spatial Attention Reflect a Common Additive Signal

Neurons and Perception March 17, 2009

Neural fate of ignored stimuli: dissociable effects of perceptual and working memory load

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

Spontaneous Cortical Activity Reveals Hallmarks of an Optimal Internal Model of the Environment. Berkes, Orban, Lengyel, Fiser.

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Report. Attention Narrows Position Tuning of Population Responses in V1

How does the ventral pathway contribute to spatial attention and the planning of eye movements?

Key questions about attention

Pushing around the Locus of Selection: Evidence for the Flexible-selection Hypothesis

Selective Attention to Face Identity and Color Studied With fmri

Neural response time integration subserves. perceptual decisions - K-F Wong and X-J Wang s. reduced model

Intrinsic Fluctuations within Cortical Systems Account for Intertrial Variability in Human Behavior

Position invariant recognition in the visual system with cluttered environments

Attention improves performance primarily by reducing interneuronal correlations

Visual attention: the where, what, how and why of saliency Stefan Treue

5th Mini-Symposium on Cognition, Decision-making and Social Function: In Memory of Kang Cheng

Resting-State functional Connectivity MRI (fcmri) NeuroImaging

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

EDGE DETECTION. Edge Detectors. ICS 280: Visual Perception

Vision Research 49 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Single cell tuning curves vs population response. Encoding: Summary. Overview of the visual cortex. Overview of the visual cortex

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Prestimulus hemodynamic activity in dorsal attention network is negatively associated with decision confidence in visual perception

The Role of Visual Short Term Memory Load. in Visual Sensory Detection

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural mechanisms of top-down control during spatial and feature attention

Neural Strategies for Selective Attention Distinguish Fast-Action Video Game Players

Limber Neurons for a Nimble Mind

Preparatory attention in visual cortex

ATTENTIONAL MODULATION OF VISUAL PROCESSING

Supplemental Information: Adaptation can explain evidence for encoding of probabilistic. information in macaque inferior temporal cortex

On the implementation of Visual Attention Architectures

The Representation of Behavioral Choice for Motion in Human Visual Cortex

Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC)

Disparity- and velocity- based signals for 3D motion perception in human MT+ Bas Rokers, Lawrence K. Cormack, and Alexander C. Huk

Push-Pull Mechanism of Selective Attention in Human Extrastriate Cortex

A multi-level account of selective attention

Quantitative Analysis of Attention and Detection Signals During Visual Search

Attention modulates spatial priority maps in the human occipital, parietal and frontal cortices

Report. Decoding Successive Computational Stages of Saliency Processing

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Brain Areas Involved in Rapid Categorization of Natural Images: An Event-Related fmri Study

The neural systems that mediate human perceptual decision making

NeuroImage 53 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage: www. elsevier. com/ locate/ ynimg

Computational Cognitive Neuroscience of the Visual System Stephen A. Engel

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14

Selective visual attention and perceptual coherence

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1

The detection of visual signals by macaque frontal eye field during masking

Efficiently representing visual information requires selecting

Attention March 4, 2009

Spatially Global Representations in Human Primary Visual Cortex during Working Memory Maintenance

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

Report. A Causal Role for V5/MT Neurons Coding Motion-Disparity Conjunctions in Resolving Perceptual Ambiguity

A Push-pull Mechanism of Selective Attention in Human Extrastriate Cortex

Carnegie Mellon University Annual Progress Report: 2011 Formula Grant

EPS Mid-Career Award 2014 The control of attention in visual search: Cognitive and neural mechanisms

Neuroscience Letters

Is contrast just another feature for visual selective attention?

Modeling the Deployment of Spatial Attention

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to simultaneous EEG-fMRI

Spontaneous and driven cortical activity: implications for computation Dario L Ringach

Differential Effects of Continuous Flash Suppression at the Fovea and Periphery

HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION

EVects of feature-based attention on the motion afterevect at remote locations

A new perspective on the perceptual selectivity of attention under load

Report. Decoding the Representation of Multiple Simultaneous Objects in Human Occipitotemporal Cortex

Beyond bumps: Spiking networks that store sets of functions

20: c 2009 Wolters Kluwer Health Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. NeuroReport 2009, 20:

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Author's personal copy

Transcription:

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Neural Netw. 2006 November ; 19(9): 1447 1449. Models of human visual attention should consider trial-by-trial variability in preparatory neural signals. Chad M. Sylvester 1,2, Giovanni d Avossa 2, and Maurizio Corbetta 1,2,3 1 Department of Radiology, Washington University School of Medicine 2 Department of Neurology, Washington University School of Medicine 3 Department of Anatomy & Neurobiology, Washington University School of Medicine Several models of attention explain how a priori knowledge about stimulus characteristics results in modulations of neural activity at the time of stimulus presentation and, in turn, enhanced perception of the stimulus (e.g., Desimone & Duncan, 1995; Tsotsos et al., 1995). There has been little focus, however, on how neural signals preceding stimulus presentation encode this a priori information and how these signals affect perception. We argue that a significant portion of behavioral variability on psychophysical tasks may be explained by variability in the neural signals preceding, rather than following, the sensory target. Modelers should consider this important source of behavioral variability. Currently, variability in the observer 's percept of a visual stimulus is thought to reflect variability in the neural signals evoked by that stimulus due to either noise in the stimulus itself ( external noise ) or noise inherent to the evoked neural signals ( internal noise ). Many single unit studies have addressed the statistical nature of the internal noise affecting visually evoked responses and how it affects the clarity of the internal representation of the stimulus (e.g., Britten, Shadlen, Newsome, & Movshon, 1992; Newsome, Britten, & Movshon, 1989). Because of the Poisson-like distribution of spike counts over repeated presentations of the same stimulus, the information present in neural signals is proportional to the mean neural signal. In Figure 1A, we re-plot data from Heuer and Britten (2004) illustrating the relation between average modulations and information content of stimulus-triggered spike trains. The blue line represents average spike rates in a single MST neuron following presentation of a motion stimulus, while the red line portrays the ability of an ideal observer to discriminate the direction of motion at each time point from the activity of the MST neuron. Clearly, the timecourse of signals carrying sensory information follows the mean signal under conditions in which internal noise limits perception. It has been suggested that attention can reduce the effects of internal noise by increasing the gain of the mean neural response (Hillyard, Vogel, & Luck, 1998; Reynolds, Pasternak, & Desimone, 2000). Another source of behavioral variability, less commonly considered, may arise from preparatory processes preceding target presentation. Under conditions of low stimulus visibility, for example, human subjects generate preparatory signals in visual cortex, and variability in these preparatory signals at the time of target presentation predicts accuracy of stimulus detection (Ress, Backus, & Heeger, 2000) 1. Preparatory signals also seem to be Address correspondence to: Chad M Sylvester, Washington University School of Medicine, 4525 Scott Avenue, Department of Radiology Campus Box 8225, Saint Louis, MO 63110, E-mail: chad@npg.wustl.edu, Phone: 314.747.0519, Fax: 314.362.6911. Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Sylvester et al. Page 2 modulated by spatial attention (Corbetta, Kincade, & Shulman, 2002; Jack, Shulman, Snyder, McAvoy, & Corbetta, 2006; Kastner, Pinsk, De Weerd, Desimone, & Ungerleider, 1999; Serences, Yantis, Culberson, & Awh, 2004). Preliminary evidence suggests that one function of preparatory signals may be to reduce external noise (Serences et al., 2004). These observations raise the intriguing possibility that variability in preparatory processes may be related to variability in stimulus-evoked signals. To understand how preparatory signals influence perception, we highlight specific results from recent neuroimaging studies (Pessoa, Gutierrez, Bandettini, & Ungerleider, 2002; Sapir, d'avossa, McAvoy, Shulman, & Corbetta, 2005; Weissman, Roberts, Visscher, & Woldorff, 2006). In each case, the timecourse of preparatory signals affecting behavioral performance did not follow the mean preparatory response, suggesting that performance related variability in preparatory signals does not resemble the variability of stimulus-evoked responses. In Figure 1, we re-plot data from two neuroimaging studies during epochs between the presentation of a sample stimulus or an attentional cue and visual targets. Each panel displays the pre-stimulus mean BOLD response timecourse (blue lines), and the timecourse that is predictive of behavioral performance (red lines). Pessoa et al. (2002) (Figure 1B) showed that variability in preparatory activity in a right intraparietal sulcus region predicted, trial-by-trial, whether subjects would correctly match a memorized sample to an upcoming test display. Sapir et al. (2005) (Figure 1C) showed that preparatory activity in MT following a spatial foveal cue predicted on a trial-by-trial basis whether subjects would correctly identify the direction of motion of a test stimulus at an attended or unattended location. Finally, Weissman et al. (2006) showed that variability in pre-trial neural signals in the anterior cingulate cortex, predicted subjects reaction time on a letter discrimination task. These studies suggest that variations in endogenous signals (memory, attention) affect trialby-trial perceptual performance. From where is this variability arising? Since the timecourses of endogenous signals predictive of behavior and mean signals are not identical, the possibility that variability is a by-product of noisy neural transmission of information seems unlikely. We offer three possible explanations. First, given its low spatial resolution, the BOLD signal may reflect several processes (neural populations), of which only a subset is relevant to behavior. Hence, the predictive components of the BOLD signal would reflect only behaviorally relevant processes (neural signals), whereas the mean signal would represent all task processes. Interestingly, Sapir et al. (2005) found that predictive and mean BOLD signals tend to segregate spatially across the brain (Figure 1D), buttressing the notion of independence between mean and predictive signals. Second, variability may result from patterned ongoing neural signals unrelated to the specific task periods, which nevertheless affect task behavior. Ongoing intrinsic activity in visual cortex has been shown to re-play patterns of activity evoked by previously seen stimuli (Kenet, Bibitchkov, Tsodyks, Grinvald, & Arieli, 2003); it is plausible that the state of this activity immediately preceding stimulus presentation could affect behavior. Recently, coherent intrinsic BOLD activity across brain regions has been found to recapitulate patterns of taskevoked activation (Biswal, Yetkin, Haughton, & Hyde, 1995; Fox et al., 2005). Others have speculated that this intrinsic synchrony may be partly responsible for the behavioral and neural variability observed during active tasks (Fox, Snyder, Zacks, & Raichle, 2006). A third possibility is that this variability reflects the result of subjects changing strategy, trialby-trial, to optimize performance. Sapir et al. (2005), in fact, argued that preparatory signals 1 In this experiment preparatory signals were isolated presenting targets only some trials. However, as preparatory and target-related signals were not temporally separated, it is still possible that the observed neural variability reflects accumulation of sensory information at the time of target detection.

Sylvester et al. Page 3 References related to performance reflected the variable use of a probabilistic cue. It should be noted that ideal performance in tasks whose demands are constant would be achieved by using a strategy that does not change from trial to trial. The presence of variable signals would suggest that humans, under some circumstances, use sub-optimal strategies. Variable preparatory signals may accordingly provide insights into how observers update internal representations of a priori information. In conclusion, we reviewed recent evidence that variability in preparatory neural signals may significantly influence trial-by-trial behavioral performance on visual psychophysical tasks. The evidence suggests a departure from the standard view that perception is limited exclusively by variability in neural signals following stimulus presentation. Rather processes preceding stimulus presentation may make a substantial contribution to trial-to-trial variability and therefore accuracy of perception. Models of human visual attention should consider the reasons why preparatory signals are variable. Biswal B, Yetkin F, Haughton V, Hyde J. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine 1995;34:537 541. [PubMed: 8524021] Britten KH, Shadlen MN, Newsome WT, Movshon JA. The analysis of visual motion: A comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance. Journal of Neuroscience 1992;12:4745 4765. [PubMed: 1464765] Corbetta M, Kincade JM, Shulman GL. Neural systems for visual orienting and their relationship with working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2002;14(3):508 523. [PubMed: 11970810] Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 1995;18:193 222. [PubMed: 7605061] Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2005;102(27):9673 9678. [PubMed: 15976020] Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Zacks JM, Raichle ME. Coherent spontaneous activity accounts for trial-to-trial variability in human evoked brain responses. Nat Neurosci 2006;9(1):23 25. [PubMed: 16341210] Heuer HW, Britten KH. Optic flow signals in extrastriate area MST: comparison of perceptual and neuronal sensitivity. J Neurophysiol 2004;91(3):1314 1326. [PubMed: 14534287] Hillyard SA, Vogel EK, Luck SJ. Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 1998;353:1257 1270. Jack AI, Shulman GL, Snyder AZ, McAvoy M, Corbetta M. Separate modulations of human V1 associated with spatial attention and task structure. Neuron 2006;51(1):135 147. [PubMed: 16815338] Kastner S, Pinsk MA, De Weerd P, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG. Increased activity in human visual cortex during directed attention in the absence of visual stimulation. Neuron 1999;22:751 761. [PubMed: 10230795] Kenet T, Bibitchkov D, Tsodyks M, Grinvald A, Arieli A. Spontaneously emerging cortical representations of visual attributes. Nature 2003;425(6961):954 956. [PubMed: 14586468] Newsome WT, Britten KH, Movshon JA. Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision. Nature 1989;341:52 54. [PubMed: 2770878] Pessoa L, Gutierrez E, Bandettini P, Ungerleider L. Neural correlates of visual working memory: fmri amplitude predicts task performance. Neuron 2002;35(5):975 987. [PubMed: 12372290] Ress D, Backus BT, Heeger DJ. Activity in primary visual cortex predicts performance in a visual detection task. Nat Neurosci 2000;3(9):940 945. [PubMed: 10966626] Reynolds JH, Pasternak T, Desimone R. Attention increases sensitivity of V4 neurons. Neuron 2000;26 (3):703 714. [PubMed: 10896165]

Sylvester et al. Page 4 Sapir A, d'avossa G, McAvoy M, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Brain signals for spatial attention predict performance in a motion discrimination task. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2005;102(49):17810 17815. [PubMed: 16306268] Serences JT, Yantis S, Culberson A, Awh E. Preparatory activity in visual cortex indexes distractor suppression during covert spatial orienting. J Neurophysiol 2004;92(6):3538 3545. [PubMed: 15254075] Tsotsos JK, Culhane S, Wai W, Lai Y, Davis N, Nuflo F. Modeling visual attention via selective tuning. Artificial Intelligence 1995;78:507 547. Weissman DH, Roberts KC, Visscher KM, Woldorff MG. The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nature Neuroscience 2006;9:971 978.

Sylvester et al. Page 5 Figure 1. Timecourses of mean activity and the component of mean activity that varies with behavior (A-C). In each panel, blue lines represent average activity modulation over all trials (A) or over all correct trials (B,C) in the epoch of interest. Red lines indicate the component of this signal that varied with performance, as measured by the ability of the signal at each time point to predict trial accuracy. All data were normalized to allow mean and predictive timecourses to be drawn on the same plots. (A) Single-unit recordings from MST following presentation of a motion stimulus (Heuer & Britten, 2004). Note that in this stimulus-evoked activity, the predictive component closely follows the mean signal. (B) BOLD activity in the right intraparietal sulcus following presentation of a visual stimulus to be held in working memory (Pessoa et al., 2002). (C) BOLD activity in MT following a spatial cue indicating the probable location of an upcoming motion stimulus (Sapir et al., 2005). Note that in these preparatory signals (B,C) the predictive component of the signal differs from the mean signal. Panel D displays a map highlighting brain regions with consistent preparatory signals averaged over trials (blue) and regions whose preparatory activity was most predictive of performance (red) (Sapir et al., 2005). Purple indicates overlap. Note the spatial segregation between these signals. Figure 1A modified, with permission, from figures 5A and 9 from Heuer and Britten (2005). Figure 1B modified, with permission, from figures 3B and 4 from Pessoa et al. (2002). Figure

Sylvester et al. Page 6 1C modified, with permission, from figure 5A from Sapir et al. (2005). Figure 1D modified, with permission, from figure 2C from Sapir et al. (2005).