Lassaline (1996) applied structural alignment directly to the issue of category-based inference. She demonstrated that adding a causal relation to an

Similar documents
However, as Sloman (1993) noted, there are other instances of nonmonotonicity that are not explainable by dilution of category coverage. His participa

Analogical Inferences in Causal Systems

Relations between premise similarity and inductive strength

The Role of Causal Models in Analogical Inference

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Features of Similarity and Category-Based Induction

Durham E-Theses. Conceptual centrality and property induction. Hadjichristidis, Constantinos

Causal Schema-Based Inductive Reasoning

Systematicity as a Selection Constraint in Analogical Mapping Catherine Clement and Dedre Gentner Psychology Department, Univeristy of Illinois

Non-categorical approaches to property induction with uncertain categories

Explanatory Reasoning for Inductive Confidence

Bayesian models of inductive generalization

A Bayesian Analysis of Some Forms of Inductive Reasoning. Evan Heit University of Warwick

Running Head: A Relevance Theory of Induction

Modeling Qualitative Differences in Symmetry Judgments

Use of Structure Mapping Theory for Complex Systems

Negative observations, induction and the generation of hypotheses

Models of Inductive Reasoning. Evan Heit. To appear in Sun, R. (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of computational psychology. Cambridge

Context-Sensitive Induction

Bayesian models of inductive generalization

Category Size and Category-Based Induction

Induction in Children and Adults

Labeling Bias and Categorical Induction: Generative Aspects of Category Information

Alternative Explanations for Changes in Similarity Judgments and MDS Structure

A Comparison of Three Measures of the Association Between a Feature and a Concept

Alignment and Category Learning

Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints

Knowledge and Category-Based Induction 1. John D. Coley, Patrick Shafto & Olga Stepanova Northeastern University

Are There Two Kinds of Reasoning?

Mutability, Conceptual Transformation, and Context

CogS with working at an office desk. Thematic categories consist of examples that tend to cluster or co-occur in particular contexts. As Wisn

t Matching Mapping Matching-then-mapping Icuts flesh I cuts flesh sloppily r I surgeon = butcher surgeon butcher surgeon a butcher Urge bare Una bm W6

Content Effects in Conditional Reasoning: Evaluating the Container Schema

The Role of Expectations in Comparisons

Representation over time: The effects of temporal distance on similarity q

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge

On the Evaluation of If p then q Conditionals

Theory-based categorization under speeded conditions

Induction with uncertain categories: When do people consider the category alternatives?

Developmental changes in children s understanding of the similarity between photographs and their referents

Argument Scope in Inductive Reasoning: Evidence for an Abductive Account of Induction

Causal Models Interact with Structure Mapping to Guide Analogical Inference

Day, S. B., & Gentner, D. (in press). Nonintentional analogical inference in text comprehension. Memory & Cognition.

The two faces of typicality in category-based induction *

When Sample Size Matters: The Influence of Sample Size and Category Variability on Children s and Adults Inductive Reasoning

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Feature centrality and property induction

Development of Categorization and Reasoning in the Natural World: Novices to Experts, Naive Similarity to Ecological Knowledge

This is part of a BBS Special Issue on the Work of Roger Shepard containing the following articles:

Inductive Reasoning. Induction, Analogy, Metaphor & Blending. Abduction Schema. Abduction or Specific Induction

The effect of causal knowledge on judgments of the likelihood of unknown features

Analogical Transfer from Interaction with a Simulated Physical System

Cognitive Psychology

Does pure water boil, when it s heated to 100 C? : The Associative Strength of Disabling Conditions in Conditional Reasoning

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 5. CLINICAL APPROACH TO INTERVIEWING PART 1

Structure mapping in spatial reasoning

Running Head: SENSITIVITY TO TAXNOMIC ASSOCIATIONS

The Trajectory of Psychology within Cognitive Science. Dedre Gentner Northwestern University

Concepts. Edouard Machery. History and Philosophy of Science University of Pittsburgh

Category coherence and category-based property induction

Learning a Hierarchical Organization of Categories

Chapter 7. Mental Representation

Relations between Associative and Structured Knowledge in Category-based Induction

Evaluating the Causal Role of Unobserved Variables

Finding the Cause: Examining the Role of Qualitative Causal Inference through Categorical Judgments

Inference Using Categories

Functionalist theories of content

THEORY DEVELOPMENT PROCESS

Structural Evaluation of Analogies : What Counts?

How Categories Shape Causality

Diversity-Based Reasoning in Children

How Analogies are Generated: The Roles of Structural and Superficial Similarity

Ideals Aren t Always Typical: Dissociating Goodness-of-Exemplar From Typicality Judgments

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

Negations Without Not : Alternative forms of Negation and Contrast Classes in Conditional Inference. James Richard Hazlett Vance

Douglas L. Medin and Sandra Waxman Northwestern University

Generic Priors Yield Competition Between Independently-Occurring Preventive Causes

Analogical Inference

The role of sampling assumptions in generalization with multiple categories

Comparison and choice: Relations between similarity processes and decision processes

Black 1 White 5 Black

Progressive Alignment Facilitates Learning of Deterministic But Not Probabilistic Relational Categories

Learning Deterministic Causal Networks from Observational Data

Attentional Theory Is a Viable Explanation of the Inverse Base Rate Effect: A Reply to Winman, Wennerholm, and Juslin (2003)

Inferring Hidden Causes

24.500/Phil253 topics in philosophy of mind/perceptual experience

An analysis of the use of animals in predicting human toxicology and drug safety: a review

Processing Emergent Features in Metaphor Comprehension

Running head: DEVELOPMENT OF DIVERSITY-BASED REASONING. Continuity and change in the development of category-based induction

T. Kushnir & A. Gopnik (2005 ). Young children infer causal strength from probabilities and interventions. Psychological Science 16 (9):

Inductive inference 1 Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning. The Problem of Induction. Steven A. Sloman. Brown University. David A.

Comparing Pluralities. Gregory Scontras Department of Linguistics, Harvard University. Peter Graff

Towards a Domain-independent Method for Evaluating and Scoring Analogical Inferences

Distributional Expectations and the Induction of Category Structure

The Regression-Discontinuity Design

Episodic and prototype models of category learning

Models of Parent-Offspring Conflict Ethology and Behavioral Ecology

Transcription:

Wu, M., & Gentner, D. (1998, August). Structure in category-based induction. Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 1154-115$, Structure in Category-Based Induction Melissa Lin Wu ( m-wu@nwu.ed u ) Department of Psychology, Northwestern University 2029 Sheridan Rd., Evanston, IL 60208 USA Dedre Gentner (g entner@ils.nwu.ed u ) Department of Psychology, Northwestern University 2029 Sheridan Rd., Evanston, IL 60208 USA Abstract We investigated category-based inference tasks, contrasting the predictions of structural alignment theory as applied to categorization with those of feature-overlap models of similarity. We provide evidence for the differential level of importance of causal information in category-based inference tasks, as predicted by the systematicity principle (Gentner, 1983). Our basic paradigm consists of a task in which participants decide between inferences based on shared causal antecedents or shared attributes. Experiment I demonstrated a preference for the causal inference when the target animal shares one attribute with one of the base animals and one causal antecedent with the other base. Iii Experiment 2, we found that this preference holds even when the target animal shares greater attribute similarity with the noncausal base (i.e., the target shares two attributes with one base and one causal antecedent with the other). Experiment 2 also served to demonstrate that this result can indeed be attributed to the influence of causal structure, and not to surface stimulus properties, such as sentence length. Overall, the results agreed with the predictions of structural alignment theory and were inconsistent with a feature-overlap account. Introduction One important function of concepts is to support inferences --deriving new information from previous knowledge. For instance, given that you know that your dog has an appendix, you might infer this property for your cat. But you would probably not infer it for your vacuum cleaner. Given this, what are the constraints on the category-based inference process? Two prominent theories of category-based induction posit that featural similarity is important for arguments like Dogs have an appendix; therefore. cats have an appendix. The similarity-coverage model (Osherson, Smith, Wilkie, Lopez, & Shafir, 1990 ; Osherson, Stern, Wilkie, Stob, & Smith, 1991) encodes categories as exemplars, which are ultimately comprised of features (e.g., "has fur," "has four legs"). The strength of the above argument (i.e., people's confidence in the truth of the conclusion given the truth of the premises) would be a function of the similarity between cat and dog, which is directly related to the number of shared features of their exemplars and inversely related to the number of distinctive features. The model also makes use of hierarchical relations between categories. Exemplars of the lowest-level category that includes both dogs and cats, mammal, are compared to exemplars of the premise category, dog. In contrast to the similarity-coverage model, the featurecoverage model (Sloman, 1993) does not assume hierarchical relations. Rather, categories at all levels (i.e., superordinate, basic, and subordinate) are represented by features. Argument strength is directly related to the teatural overlap between premise and conclusion categories and inversely related to the familiarity and complexity of the conclusion category. Although both of these models account for an impressive array of phenomena (see Osherson et al, 1990 and Sloman, 1993), there is room for an alternative. These models share the assumption that exemplars are represented as sets of features, which form the basis for the computation of feature-overlap or similarity. However, some recent evidence suggests that exemplar comparison is accomplished by alignment of structural representations (Clement & Gentner, 1991 ; Lassaline, 1996 ; Heit & Rubinstein, 1994). These results follow from an approach to similarity based on representations consisting of features and relations between them (Gentner, 1989 ; Medin, Goldstone, & Gentner, 1994). In structural alignment, parts of two structured representations are put into correspondence. Inferences are possible when predicates exist that are absent in one representation but connected to the common aligned system. According to the systematicity principle, systems of relations are preferred over individual features and higherorder relations (i.e., relations between relations), especially causal relations, are preferred over lower-order relations (Gentner, 1983). For instance, the fact that both the solar system and a hydrogen atom involve attraction and revolution is more important than the fact that both have spherical entities. Assuming the systematicity principle is at work in inference tasks, inferences connected to causal structure should be preferred over those unconnected. In accord with this prediction, Clement and Gentner (1991) found that participants who read analogous stories preferred to make an inference from a shared causal antecedent, relative to an unconnected fact. They were also more confident in this inference. 1154

Lassaline (1996) applied structural alignment directly to the issue of category-based inference. She demonstrated that adding a causal relation to an argument about animals increased its rated strength. For example, adding the relation For Animal B, a weak immune system causes an acute sense of smell increased the strength of the argument Animal A has a weak immune system, skin that has no pigment, and dry flaky skin; Animal B has a weak immune system and an acute sense of smell; therefore, Animal A also has an acute sense of smell. In addition, Lassaline found that adding shared features (e.g., "has muscular forearms") to the premises also increased inductive strength. However, she did not address the question of which factor--relational or featural--is more important for inferential strength. If the systematicity principle holds for category-based inference tasks, causal relations should be more important, and we should see a preference for an inference connected to higher-order (causal) structure. Experiments Our experiments attempted to test whether category-based induction tasks are governed by the process of structural alignment.' In particular, we tested the systematicity principle. We created a category-based induction task similar to Lassaline's (1996). For both experiments, we followed Lassaline in using fictional animals (e.g., Animal A). Stimulus items consisted of descriptions of one target animal and two base animals, as shown in Table l. The "causal" base consisted of an unfamiliar but plausible causal relation and the "attribute" base consisted of a list of features. The target shared the causal antecedent of the causal animal and all but one attribute of the attribute animal. Participants were told to choose the stronger of two possible inferences that could be made about the target using the information about the bases. There were two obvious choices : the causal consequent from the causal base or the remaining attribute from the attribute base. Attributes were grouped such that they were independent of each other, the causal antecedent, and the causal consequent. We varied which of the attribute animal's features were shared versus inferred. For instance, "has protruding canines" was a shared feature for some participants and a potential inference for others. The order of stimulus items in the packet was also varied between subjects. In using the term "category-based induction," we do not necessarily assume that participants are basing inferences on knowledge of taxonomic relationships or on a categorical representation of "animal." Rather, the process of structural alignment may operate on instances, species or perhaps at an intermediate abstraction of a group of species (e.g., at a level akin to pragmatic reasoning schemas, Cheng & Holyoak, 1985). Experiment 1 Our objective in this experiment was to test the prediction that participants would prefer the systematic (causal) inference. This prediction follows from a structural account of similarity. In this study, the target animal shared one of the two listed attributes with the attribute animal. According to a "flat" or featural account of similarity, there should be no preference for either the attribute or causal inference, since both base animals are equally similar to the target. In drawing predictions from the flat similarity account, we assumed a dot product similarity computation with equal weighting of features, over features explicitly present in the three exemplars only. The causal animal description consisted of a statement that a causal antecedent caused a consequent (A -4 C). Although not explicitly stated, it seemed likely that participants would assume that the causal animal had the causal antecedent (A) and the consequent (C). Had they done so, then the causal and attribute animals would be equally similar to the target. However, one might argue that participants' representation of the causal animal differed from that assumed. For instance, they may have assumed the base contained only the causal relation (A 4 C) and the antecedent (A), or that it contained only the causal relation and neither the antecedent nor the consequent. However, in none of these cases does the computed similarity of the causal animal to the target exceed that of the attribute animal to the target. This is true assuming either a dot product or Tverskian computation (Tversky, 1977). Sixteen Northwestern University undergraduates participated in partial fulfillment of a course requirement. Materials consisted of a packet of 16 of the stimulus items previously described. Table 1 : Sample Item from Experiment 1 The following facts are true of Animal A [the Attribute base]: Has large oil glands Has protruding canines The following facts are true of Animal B [the Causal base) : Adenoviruses cause an increased risk for tumors The following facts are true of Animal C [the Target] : Has large oil glands Has an adenovirus Please infer only one property for Animal C (circle one only) : Has protruding canines Has an increased risk for tumors Note : Information in brackets was not presented in experimental materials. 1 15 5

The results show a strong preference for causal inference. We assigned participants to the "predominantly causal" group if they chose the causal consequent on at least 13 of 16 trials, a significantly greater number that would be expected by chance, p <.01. Figure I shows the results of the sign test on participants. Thirteen out of 16 participants had predominantly causal inferences, a significantly greater number of participants than would be expected by chance, p <.05. No participant had predominantly attribute inferences (13 or more attribute inferences), and the remaining three participants demonstrated a pattern that did not differ from chance. The same procedure was done on items, with similar results. N c CL v r M IL 0 8-2- 0 Causal Attribute Response Class Chance Figure 1 : Number of participants in each response class, Experiment 1. These results bear out the prediction of structural alignment theory : there appears to be a preference to infer from a base connected to causal structure, as predicted by the systematicity principle. The finding of a causal preference (Clement & Gentner, 1991) appears to apply within a category-based inference task (Lassaline, 1996), even when featural similarity is equalized. However, before drawing conclusions, we wished to address some possible concerns of this study. First, perhaps participants were initially attending to only the featural similarity between the target and each base, but since the bases did not differ in this regard, they felt compelled to use another strategy. Under this scenario, participants inferred from the causal base because it was more distinctive. A more definitive test for the preference for causal inference would involve constructing stimuli in which the attribute base is more featurally similar to the target than is the causal base. Another concern is with the syntactic differences between the descriptions of the causal and attribute animals. The causal animal description consisted of one long statement, while the attribute animal description consisted of facts on separate lines. Perhaps participants chose the causal consequent simply because the causal information was presented in a way that made it seem more important or salient. A final concern is with generalizability. The same set of 16 causal consequents was used for all participants. This leaves open the possibility of stimulus effects. For example, if the causal choices happened to yield more applicable or enduring inference choices, then the preference for the causal choice could not be taken to imply a preference for higher-order structure per se. For instance, people may be more willing to infer "bears many offspring" than "lives in Madagascar" because the first fact is true of more animals than the second. Likewise, the first fact seems like a better inference than "is sleepy" because it is a less transient' property. Although the differences in our stimuli were never this extreme, it may be the case that the facts used as causal consequents were slightly more applicable and enduring, and were favored by participants as inferences because of it. Experiment 2 This experiment was designed to determine whether a preference for inferences from a base connected to causal structure holds when using a stronger test. We contrasted the causal animal with an animal with high attribute similarity to the target animal. As in Experiment 1, the target animal shared a causal antecedent with the causal animal. However, this time, the target shared two out of three independent features with the attribute animal, rather than one of two independent features, as in Experiment 1. In this design, the attribute base exceeds the causal base in its degree of feature overlap to the target. Thus, a flat similarity' view predicts a preference to infer from the attribute base,, whereas a structural similarity account predicts a preference to infer from the causal base. A second objective was to rule out alternative interpretations of the data from the first experiment. To, make the causal and attribute base descriptions more similar : in appearance to each other, and thus eliminate possible differences in salience, we added the information that the' causal base animal had the causal antecedent and consequent' properties to the causal base description. We also wished to, ensure that the causal consequent property was chosen as an! inference because of the causal relation connecting it to the l shared causal antecedent, and not because of any otheri conceptual differences between it and the second feature of the attribute base. To this end, we varied whether a given, possible response was associated with the causal base or the' attribute base. Specifically, for a given item, half of the subjects saw one of the inference choices (e.g., "high risk for strokes") as the causal consequent and the other choice (e.g., "underdeveloped visual system") as the second attribute feature, while for the other half, the correspondences were, reversed. 1 156

Sixteen paid Northwestern University undergraduates participated. Materials consisted of a packet of 12 stimulus items like the sample shown in Table 2. The data again demonstrate a preference for the causal inference. We assigned participants to the "predominantly causal" group if they chose the causal consequent on at least 11 of the 12 experimental trials, a significantly greater number than would be expected by chance, p <.01. As shown in Figure 2, 14 out of 16 participants had predominantly causal inferences, a significantly greater number. of participants than would be expected by chance, p <.01. One participant had predominantly attribute inferences, and the remaining participant had a pattern of responses that did not differ from chance, The same analysis was done on items, with similar results. Table 2 : Sample Item from Experiment 2 Causal Attribute Response Class Chance The following facts are true of Animal A [the Attribute base] : Has muscular forearms Has sensitive gums Has high risk for strokes The following facts are true of Animal B [the Causal base] : Has an overactive thyroid Has an underdeveloped visual system An overactive thyroid causes an underdeveloped visual system The following facts are true of Animal C [the Target] : Has muscular forearms Has sensitive gums Has an overactive thyroid Please infer only one property for Animal C (circle one only) : Has high risk for strokes Has an underdeveloped visual system These results rule out the alternative interpretations for our earlier findings and support the systematicity principle. If we count all shared assertions, then by flat similarity models, participants should prefer the inference from the attribute animal, because it shares two of three features with the target, whereas the causal animal shares only one of three features. Thus, in a situation where a flat similarity account would predict a clear preference for a particular attribute inference, participants overwhelmingly choose the causal consequent. These results are evidence for the role of structural similarity in category-based induction. Figure 2 : Number of participants in each response class. Experiment 2. Discussion These studies attempted to shed light on the issue of whether featural or structural similarity is more crucial in evaluation of category-based inferences. The two experiments reported in this paper suggest that connection to causal structure drives inference. When given two inference choices, people prefer to make the inference from the base that is connected to causal structure in the target, over the base that shares independent features. This generalization is true even when the alternative inference is from a target with : eater attribute similarity with the base (i.e., with a larger set of shared independent features). Taken together, these findings would appear to contradict the predictions from featureoverlap versions of a similarity-based account of categorybased induction (i.e., Sloman, 1993 ; Osherson et al., 1991). Our findings add to the body of literature demonstrating the influence of systematicity and structural similarity on inference. Gentner, Rattermann, and Forbus (1993) asked participants to rate the inferential soundness, or degree to which one could make accurate inferences, of matched stories. Rated soundness was positively related to the degree of shared relational structure. Bowdle and Gentner (in press) found that people were more likely to make an inference from a more systematic story to a less systematic story than the other way around. Moreover, the inferences tended to be information connected to common causal structure. Other evidence points to the role of systems of interrelated relations in category-based induction. Heit and Rubinstein (1994) found that people make stronger inferences in a category-based inference task when the kind of property to be inferred (anatomical or behavioral) matches the kind of similarity between the animal in the premise and the animal 1 157

in the conclusion (anatomical or behavioral). For instance, people judge the likelihood that whales travel in a zig-zag trajectory higher when told that tuna have that property, relative to when they are told that bears have that property. This is presumably because whales and tuna match behaviorally (they both swim) while whales and bears do not (they match anatom ;cally--both are mammals). Heit and Rubinstein's results can be seen as evidence for the importance of shared structure linked to the property to be inferred. Tuna and whales can be represented as structures containing features and relations connecting these features. Moreover, one might argue that participants assume that some (behavioral, and perhaps, anatomical) attributes and relations of tuna are causally related to the property of traveling in a zig-zag trajectory. In order to evaluate the argument, participants align their representations of tuna and whale. They then search -for support for the inference that whales travel in a zig-zag trajectory. They examine attributes and relations that are causally connected to the property in tuna. If these are shared by the target, whale, then they contribute to the support measure. The more shared relevant attributes and relations that are meaningfully connected to the candidate inference, the greater the support for the candidate inference. We have argued that similarity is indeed important for inference, but that similarity involves more than simple feature lists. Rather, representations encode features and background knowledge, theories, and causal relations.. Lassaline, M.E. (1996). Structural alignment in induction and similarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology : Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 754-770. Medin, D.L., Goldstone, R.L., & Gentner, D. (1994) Respects for Similarity. Psychological Review, 100, 254-278. Osherson, D.N., Smith, E.E., Wilkie, 0., Lopez, A., & Shafir, E. (1990). Category-based induction. Psychological Review, 97, 185-200. Osherson, D.N., Stern, J., Wilkie, 0., Stob, M., & Smith, E.E. (1991). Default probability. Cognitive Science, 15, 251-269. Sloman, S.A. (1993). Feature-based induction. Cognitive Psychology, 25,231-280. Tversky, A. (1977). Features of similarity. Psychological Review, 84,327-352. Acknowledgments We thank Ginger Gunn, Phil Wolff, Jeff Loewenstein, Shelli Feist, Ken Kurtz, and Ken Forbus for comments on this research. This research was funded by the National Science Foundation and the Office of Naval Research. References Bowdle, B.F., & Gentner, D. (in press). Informativity and asymmetry in comparisons. Cognitive Psychology. Cheng, P.W., & Holyoak, K.J. (1985). Pragmatic reasoning schemas. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 391-416. Clement, C.A., & Gentner, D. (1991). Systematicity as a selection constraint in analogical mapping. Cognitive Science, 15, 89-132. Gentner, D. (1983). Structure-mapping : A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science, 7, 155-170. Gentner, D., Ratterman, M., & Forbus, K.D. (1993). The roles of similarity in transfer : Separating retrievability from inferential soundness. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 524-575. Heit, E., & Rubinstein, J. (1994). Similarity and property effects in inductive reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 411-422. 1158