TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION

Similar documents
THE COMMUTATIVE RULE AS NEW TEST FOR ADDITIVE CONJOINT MEASURMENT: THEORY AND DATA

Modeling Human Perception

Journal of Mathematical Psychology. Theory and tests of the conjoint commutativity axiom for additive conjoint measurement

Empirical evaluation of axioms fundamental to Stevens s ratio-scaling approach: I. Loudness production

Study of perceptual balance for binaural dichotic presentation

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Spring 2016 Laboratory 2: Perception of Loudness

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion

Auditory fmri correlates of loudness perception for monaural and diotic stimulation

Evaluating a Model of Global Psychophysical Judgments for Brightness: I. Behavioral Properties Summations and Productions

Tactile Communication of Speech

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities

INTRODUCTION. Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA Electronic mail:

Categorical Perception

A SYSTEM FOR CONTROL AND EVALUATION OF ACOUSTIC STARTLE RESPONSES OF ANIMALS

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency

Temporal offset judgments for concurrent vowels by young, middle-aged, and older adults

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

3-D Sound and Spatial Audio. What do these terms mean?

Comments on Plott and on Kahneman, Knetsch, and Thaler

Psychology of Perception PSYC Spring 2017 Laboratory 2: Perception of Loudness

Loudness Processing of Time-Varying Sounds: Recent advances in psychophysics and challenges for future research

THE EFFECT OF A REMINDER STIMULUS ON THE DECISION STRATEGY ADOPTED IN THE TWO-ALTERNATIVE FORCED-CHOICE PROCEDURE.

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency. (Some slides are adapted from Zhiyao Duan s course slides on Computer Audition and Its Applications in Music)

Lecture 3: Perception

Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception

TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS

Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Volume 27, , June tleseareh Note RELATION BETWEEN REACTION TIME AND LOUDNESS

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Perceptual pitch shift for sounds with similar waveform autocorrelation

The basic hearing abilities of absolute pitch possessors

MODELING WITHIN-PAIR ORDER EFFECTS IN PAIRED-COMPARISON JUDGMENTS.

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

Jitter, Shimmer, and Noise in Pathological Voice Quality Perception

AUTOCORRELATION AND CROSS-CORRELARION ANALYSES OF ALPHA WAVES IN RELATION TO SUBJECTIVE PREFERENCE OF A FLICKERING LIGHT

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

CONTRIBUTION OF DIRECTIONAL ENERGY COMPONENTS OF LATE SOUND TO LISTENER ENVELOPMENT

Consonant Perception test

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths

ID# Exam 2 PS 325, Fall 2009

STUDY OF THE BISECTION OPERATION

Sensation is the conscious experience associated with an environmental stimulus. It is the acquisition of raw information by the body s sense organs

MODELS FOR THE ADJUSTMENT OF RATING SCALES 1

Attention bands in absolute identification

Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli

EEL 6586, Project - Hearing Aids algorithms

I. INTRODUCTION A. The concept of noise sensitivity. B. Evidence for a sensory component in noise sensitivity

Visual Transformation of Size

Abstract. 1 Introduction

EFFECTS OF TEMPORAL FINE STRUCTURE ON THE LOCALIZATION OF BROADBAND SOUNDS: POTENTIAL IMPLICATIONS FOR THE DESIGN OF SPATIAL AUDIO DISPLAYS

Information Integration and the Identification of Stimulus Noise and Criteria! Noise in Absolute Judgment

Lab 4: Perception of Loudness

MEASUREMENT, SCALING, AND PSYCHOPHYSICS

BINAURAL DICHOTIC PRESENTATION FOR MODERATE BILATERAL SENSORINEURAL HEARING-IMPAIRED

Time Varying Comb Filters to Reduce Spectral and Temporal Masking in Sensorineural Hearing Impairment

Auditory Physiology PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 30. Organ of Corti

Assessment of auditory temporal-order thresholds A comparison of different measurement procedures and the influences of age and gender

Reply to Reconsidering evidence for the suppression model of the octave illusion, by C. D. Chambers, J. B. Mattingley, and S. A.

Schwartz and Krantz, Sensation and Perception

Auditory Weighting Functions and Frequency-Dependent Effects of Sound in Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)

Lab 3: Perception of Loudness

Sound localization psychophysics

Perception Lecture 1

Digital. hearing instruments have burst on the

Empirical Quantification Is the Key to Measurement Including Psychological Measurement

Introduction. Chapter The Perceptual Process

A. SEK, E. SKRODZKA, E. OZIMEK and A. WICHER

Thresholds for different mammals

Separate What and Where Decision Mechanisms In Processing a Dichotic Tonal Sequence

Introducing ReSound LiNX Quattro Unprecedented Layers of Sound and the world s most advanced rechargeable hearing aid solution.

psychology of visual perception C O M M U N I C A T I O N D E S I G N, A N I M A T E D I M A G E 2014/2015

Introduction to Sensation and Perception

SENSATION AND PERCEPTION KEY TERMS

CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS IN INFORMATION INTEGRATION

PERCEPTION OF AUDITORY-VISUAL SIMULTANEITY CHANGES BY ILLUMINANCE AT THE EYES

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization

Perceptual Effects of Nasal Cue Modification

Psychoacoustical Models WS 2016/17

Sensory Thresholds and Signal Detection. Lecture 13

Learning to detect a tone in unpredictable noise

A linear relation between loudness and decibels

What Is the Difference between db HL and db SPL?

USING AUDITORY SALIENCY TO UNDERSTAND COMPLEX AUDITORY SCENES

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

Characterizing individual hearing loss using narrow-band loudness compensation

3-D SOUND IMAGE LOCALIZATION BY INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES AND THE MEDIAN PLANE HRTF. Masayuki Morimoto Motokuni Itoh Kazuhiro Iida

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

Linguistic Phonetics. Basic Audition. Diagram of the inner ear removed due to copyright restrictions.

Hearing. istockphoto/thinkstock

Perceptual overestimation of rising intensity: Is stimulus continuity necessary?

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080

The development of a modified spectral ripple test

Signal identification: The comparison of two competitive theories

Beltone Electronics 2601 Patriot Boulevard Glenview, IL U.S.A. (800)

Effect of spectral content and learning on auditory distance perception

Hearing II Perceptual Aspects

Transcription:

TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION Karin Zimmer, R. Duncan Luce and Wolfgang Ellermeier Institut für Kognitionsforschung der Universität Oldenburg, Germany Institute for Mathematical Behavioral Sciences, University of California, Irvine, USA Institut für Psychologie der Universität Regensburg, Germany e-mail: karin.zimmer@uni-oldenburg.de Abstract A theory recently proposed by Luce (2001) details qualitative assumptions underlying methods of sensory integration, magnitude scaling, and cross-modality matching. Apart from judgments of ordering and proportion, the theory invokes a psychological concatenation operation, something that does not often appear in psychological applications. One such operation is the focus of the present investigation in which concatenation is realized as the sequential presentation of two noise samples which the auditory system is thought to integrate temporally with respect to loudness. To that end, two 50 ms-bursts of white noise in immediate succession but differing in intensity were presented diotically to each participant. The task was to adjust the loudness of a third 50 ms-noise burst so that its loudness matched that of the composite noise. The theory leads to two distinct pairs of properties for the operation: either commutative and associative or non-commutative and bisymmetric. Commutativity means that the temporal order of a stimulus pair does not alter the match; associativity means that the two groupings of three stimuli in fixed order have the same match; and bisymmetry means that in a grouping of four ordered stimuli into two pairs, an interchange of the two middle stimuli leads to the same match. Results of Experiment I showed that, except for one participant, loudness adjustments tended to be influenced more by the intensity of the first component than by that of the second. Using a 7-dB range of noise levels and 6 participants, commutativity was not directly rejected for anyone; however its theoretical consequence, associativity, was rejected for 5 of 6 participants (Mann-Whitney U-tests, α=0.10). The magnitude of the effect depends upon the range of stimulus intensities used. With 3, 7, 15, and 20 db-ranges in Experiment II, clearer violations of both commutativity and assocativity were found as the range of levels increased. Experiment III served to establish that this finding does not change when the matching stimulus is increased from 50 ms to 100 ms, the overall duration of the composite noise to be matched. Preliminary results of a fourth experiment currently in progress indicate that the bisymmetric property better describes participants' loudness adjustments than does commutativity and associativity. Scaling constitutes the assignment of numbers to magnitudes of sensation that are evoked by physical stimuli. Following an approach advocated by Stevens (1956, 1975), subjects either produce their numerical estimates of sensation magnitudes evoked by stimuli (magnitude

estimation) or they are presented with numerals and are asked to adjust stimulus intensity so that its sensation magnitude matches the given numeral (magnitude production). In Stevens approach, these numerals are taken at face value in the sense of assuming the numerals to exhibit all the mathematical properties of numbers, and are found to form approximately power functions of signal intensity. Different representational systems have been proposed that specify fundamental conditions that have to be met in order to justify this procedure of treating numerals as numbers (Krantz, 1972, Luce, 1959, 1990, Shepard, 1981, Narens, 1996). All of these structures have been restricted in applicability to one of several scaling methods frequently used. Recently, however, Luce (2001) has proposed a comprehensive theoretical framework of psychophysical scaling that treats, and seeks to unify, its most important methods, namely sensory integration of stimuli, magnitude (or proportion) scaling, and cross-modality matching. Moreover, this theory not only makes it possible to test whether these methods can be validly employed at all, but provides for the construction of a scale. This is done by identifying conditions that allow for a specification of the parameters both of the psychophysical function relating stimulus intensity to sensation strength, and of the weighting function relating overt numerical judgments to the underlying (mathematical) numbers. The present investigation focuses on the psychological concatenation operation employed in the theory because, in general, such an operation is not often studied in psychological applications. Here, concatenation is realized as the sequential presentation of two noise samples to the auditory system, which is thought to perform some sort of temporal integration with respect to loudness. It is an operation in the sense that if a b ~ u, (a b) c ~ u c ~ v is an intensity, where ~ denotes a subjective match. Luce' s sensory-integration structure involves three fundamental properties: (1) Commutativity: the temporal order of a stimulus pair does not matter, e.g., presenting a 70dB stimulus, a, followed by a 60 db stimulus, b, should lead to the same loudness estimate as presenting the 60 db stimulus, b, first, and the 70 db stimulus, a, second, i.e., a b ~ b a. (2) Associativity: two groupings of three stimuli a, b, c, in fixed order - either concatenating a and b first, then concatenating the resultant with c, or concatenating b and c first, and then concatenating the resultant with a - are matched by the same stimulus, i.e., (a b) c ~ a (b c). (3) Bisymmetry: in grouping four ordered stimuli a, b, c, d, into two pairs, interchanging the two middle stimuli leads to the same match, i.e., (a b) (c d) ~ (a c) (b d). The theory arrives at two types of sensory-integration structures: either commutative and associative or non-commutative and bisymmetric. In the presence of the other assumptions of the theory, commutativity implies associativity, which establish order-invariance of stimuli, whereas the assumption of non-commutativity implies bisymmetry, which allows for a differential weighting of stimuli with respect to the order in which they are presented. In four experiments, the empirical validity of these three properties was investigated, using as stimulus material 50-ms noise samples differing in sound pressure level. Experiment I served to test commutativity and associativity in 7 and 15 db ranges of stimulus levels. In Experiments II and III, the influence of stimulus range, and of the duration of the noise samples on axiom validity was investigated, using 3, 7, 15, and 20 db intensity ranges. In Experiment IV, in addition to commutativity and associativity, the bisymmetry condition was tested in 15 and 20 db-ranges of stimulus intensities. Data collection for this experiment has not been completed yet, therefore the present report focuses on the first three experiments only.

Method Subjects A total of 12 subjects between 21 and 43 years of age participated in the experiments. Six participated in Experiment I, and six participated in both Experiment II and III. Using Békésy tracking, all participants were within 25 db of the hearing norm in a range of 500 to 6000 Hz. Except for the first and third authors, who took part in the first experiment, all subjects were naive with respect to the goals of the investigation. Stimuli and Apparatus Stimuli consisted of 50 ms-bursts of white noise (including 2.5 ms rise and decay ramps) which were generated digitally by a TDT AP2 signal processor, and converted with a 50 Hzsampling-rate to analogue signals by a 16-bit TDT DD1 converter. Signals were then passed through a low-pass filter set at 20 khz (TDT FT6), and were set to appropriate sound pressure levels by a programmable attenuator (TDT PA4), before being delivered diotically via headphone amplifier (TDT HB6) to AKG-K501 headphones. All levels, as specified in table I, for example, are overall sound pressure levels of the noise, as measured at the earphones. Procedure Subjects, who were seated in a sound-proof chamber, were presented diotically with two 50 ms-bursts of white noise that were different in level and succeeded each other without temporal separation. The participants' task was to adjust the loudness of a third 50 ms-noise burst, the comparison sound, presented 400 ms later, so that its loudness matched that of the composite noise. Participants indicated via a button press whether the comparison should be made louder or softer. A modified method of adjustments, in which step-sizes successively decreased from 4 db to 0.5 db, was used to obtain loudness matches. Experiments I and II tested both commutativity and associativity with two and four stimulus sets, respectively. Subjects had to produce all adjustments in every block of trials. The trial order was random. After one practice block, participants completed 15 blocks per experimental condition. The stimulus levels employed in the experiments are given in table I. Table I. Overall sound pressure levels [db SPL] used in Experiments I and II. Experiment stimulus a stimulus b stimulus c stimulus range I 67 71 74 7 62 69 77 15 II 64 66 67 3 67 71 74 7 62 69 77 15 60 70 80 20 Note. The (a,b) stimulus pairs were used in testing commutativity. Experiment III evaluated the commutative property again using the same four stimulus pairs as in Experiment II (s. Table I), but with the comparison sound lengthened to 100 ms, the

duration of the composite noise. The order in which sessions for Experiment III, and the 7 db/15 db-, and 3 db/20 db-conditions of Experiment II were run followed a latin-square design across participants. Results Data were evaluated on an individual level. For all subjects and all SPL combinations, the comparison sound level was higher than that of the lower level in the composite. Experiment I In testing the commutative property, median differences between loudness matches for a b and b a ranged from 1 to +1 db and from 1 to +2 db, when noise bursts were 2 db and 7 db apart, respectively. With the exception of one participant, median differences were rather small, and, if present at all, they were negative for both ranges. This means that, presenting the less intense composite of a stimulus pair first resulted in a lower-level match, than when the more intense signal was first. Likewise, in testing associativity median differences between matches for (a b) c vs. matches for a (b c) ranged between 0.5 and 1.5 db, when stimulus intensities spanned a 7 db-range, and between 1.5 and 3 db when stimuli were in the 15 db-range of stimulus intensities. For all subjects and for both ranges, median differences were positive, again showing a trend for lower loudness matches when the first composite of the stimulus was lower in level than the second. In using a 7-dB overall range of noise levels, commutativity was not violated by anyone and associativity was violated by 5 of the 6 participants in a statistically significant way (Mann- Whitney U-tests; overall α=0.10, Bonferroni-corrected for multiple testing of participants to test-wise α=0.026). In the broader, 15 db-range of stimulus intensities, commutativity and associativity failed in 1 of 6 and in all of 6 participants, respectively. Thus, with the exception of one participant, loudness adjustments tended to be influenced more by the intensity of the first than by the intensity of the second component. This trend shows in testing both properties, and is rather clear-cut for the associative axiom, whereas, for commutativity, the effect is not sufficiently strong to produce statistically significant results. Note, however, that in the theory rejecting associativity indirectly rejects commutativity. Experiment II Experiment II served to investigate whether violations of the axioms depended on the range of stimulus intensities. Accordingly, 3, 7, 15, and 20 db-ranges of stimulus intensities were employed to evaluate commutativity and associativity of loudness matches. Testing commutativity, three of six participants for all stimulus ranges exhibited negative median differences reaching up to 1.5 db. For two other participants, both positive and negative median differences (between 1 and +1 db) were found. Only one participant showed positive differences, reaching a maximum of +1 db. Median differences reached a statistically significant level for three of (4 ranges x 6 participants =) 24 tests (Mann-Whitney U-tests, α=0.10, Bonferroni-correction for multiple testing of participants: test-wise α=0.013). In all three cases, violations occurred in the two largest stimulus ranges, and median differences were negative. As was already observed in Experiment I, results on the associative property were more clearcut (s. Table II) in that, with one exception out of 24 cases, all median differences were

positive. Moreover, they tended for all subjects to be larger for larger ranges of stimulus intensities. Except for one subject, differences reached statistical significance for the two largest stimulus ranges, 15 and 20 db for all participants. In addition to that, three subjects already showed statistically significant violations of associativity in the 7 db-range of stimulus intensities. Thus, the larger the range of stimulus levels used, the clearer the violations of associativity. The results for commutativity are more equivocal than that. Since median differences are rather small, any possible effect of stimulus range on axiom validity may have been blurred altogether. Table II. Experiment II: Results of testing the associative property in four stimulus ranges. 3 db range 7 db range 15 db range 20 db range Subject Med diff. z-score Med diff. z-score Med diff. z-score Med diff. z-score BG 0.5 0.04-1.0 0.10 2.0 1.93 1.5 1.77 CB 0.5 1.76 1.5 3.48 3.0 4.68 5.5 4.68 DO 0 0.17 1.0 2.29 3.0 4.24 3.5 2.95 HE 0 0.04 1.0 1.10 2.5 3.90 2.5 3.73 PI 1.0 0.62 1.5 4.46 3.5 4.10 2.0 3.52 VE 1.0 1.84 1.0 2.31 3.0 4.13 3.0 4.67 Note. For every subject, median differences between (a b) c and a (b c), and corresponding z-scores, are given for 3, 7, 15, and 20-dB stimulus ranges. Median differences are based on 15 adjustments each. Statistically significant z-scores are highlighted (Mann-Whitney U-tests, α=0.10; Bonferroni-correction for multiple testing of participants: test-wise α=0.013). Experiment III Using the same stimuli as in Experiment II, commutativity was again evaluated, but with the comparison sound lengthened from 50 ms to 100 ms, the overall duration of the composite noise. Median differences ranged from 1.5 db to +1 db, that is, they were in the same general range as in Experiment II. With one exception in the smallest stimulus range, the results of those three participants who had shown negative differences in all ranges of stimulus intensities in Experiment II were replicated. For the remaining three participants, either no, or exclusively positive differences were found. Commutativity was violated in a statistically significant way for three of 24 cases (Mann-Whitney U-tests, α=0.10, Bonferronicorrection for multiple testing of participants: test-wise α=0.026). Again, in all statistically significant cases, median differences were negative, and violations occurred only in the two largest stimulus ranges. Because the results for commutativity of Experiments II and III are similar, we conclude that the duration of the comparison sound does not have a crucial influence on estimating the validity of the commutative property. Discussion The present investigation shows that the associative property, and by implication also commutativity, is not valid in describing the concatenation of noise-bursts presented

immediately after each other with respect to an overall loudness impression. Furthermore, axiom violations tend to increase with increasing sound-level differences between the stimulus components. Apparently, the sound level of the first noise-component is more influential in determining the loudness judgment. Thus, it may well be that a structure which is able to account for weighting the presentation-order of stimuli will form a better representation. The theoretical alternative, under some smoothness conditions described in Luce (2001), is the bisymmetric property. If it is empirically correct, an invariance argument implies that the psychophysical function should be a power function. Indeed, results for the first three out of six subjects who have been run in an experiment evaluating the bisymmetric property point in this direction. If this finding holds for the other participants as well, one of the fundamental structures of Luce s theory, namely the sensory-ingegration representation, can be thought to hold in this domain. The next step in evaluating other aspects of this psychophysical theory is to test the properties of numerical judgments (or adjustments), which have not been at issue yet in the present sensory-integration experiments. To that end, experiments should be performed, which investigate the internal consistency of these judgments based on Luce s generalization of Narens (1996) axiomatization. Furthermore, conditions which involve both sensory integration and numerical judgments ( binary segregation and simple joint-presentation decomposability ) should also be investigated. If this programme can be successfully completed, a global theory may be established, which specifies the (perceived) loudness of noise-bursts, no matter which of several psychophysical scaling methods is applied. If any of the fundamental conditions assumed in the theory fail, then an appropriate modification of the theory will be called for as occurred, for example, when the original commutative theory of Luce (2001) was rejected both for a temporalintegration (present experiments) and for a binaural-summation interpretation (Steingrimsson, personal communication) of concatenation. References Ellermeier, W. & Faulhammer, G. (2000). Empirical evaluation of axioms fundamental to Stevens s ratio-scaling approach: I. Loudness production. Perception & Psychophysics, 62(8), 1505-1511. Krantz, D. H. (1972). A theory of magnitude estimation and cross-modality matching. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 9, 168-199. Luce, R. D. (1959). On the possible psychophysical laws. Psychological Review, 66, 81-95. Luce, R. D. (1990). On the possible psychophysical laws revisited: Remarks on cross-modal matching. Psychological Review, 97, 66-77. Luce, R.D. (2001). A psychophysical theory of intensity proportions, joint presentations, and matches. Psychological Review. In press. Narens, L. (1996). A theory of ratio magnitude estimation. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 40, 109-129. Shepard, R. N. (1981). Psychological relations and psychophysical scales: On the status of direct psychophysical measurement. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 24, 21-57. Stevens, S.S. (1956). The direct estimation of sensory magnitude loudness. American Journal of Psychology, 69, 1-15. Stevens, S.S. (1975). Psychophysics: Introduction to ist perceptual, neural, and social prospects. New York: Wiley.