Modulation of prefrontal cortex activation by emotional words in recognition memory

Similar documents
Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information

Neural correlates of retrieval processing in the prefrontal cortex during recognition and exclusion tasks

Material-specific lateralization of prefrontal activation during episodic encoding and retrieval

Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes

Retrieval Success and the Prefrontal Cortex Evidence from fmri

Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times

Supplemental information online for

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

The effect of emotional arousal and retention delay on subsequent-memory effects

Neuropsychologia 49 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

In Search of Recollection and Familiarity Signals in the Hippocampus

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Amygdala Activity Is Associated with the Successful Encoding of Item, But Not Source, Information for Positive and Negative Stimuli

The hippocampus operates in a threshold manner during spatial source memory Scott D. Slotnick a and Preston P. Thakral b

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri

The Neural Correlates of Moral Decision-Making in Psychopathy

Supplementary Information

Selective attention to emotional stimuli in a verbal go/no-go task: an fmri study

The coupling of emotion and cognition in the eye: Introducing the pupil old/new effect

The neural correlates of conceptual and perceptual false recognition

Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition

Material-speci c neural correlates of memory retrieval

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention

Research Article. Industrial Science and Technology (AIST), Tsukuba, Japan

Neural correlates of recognition memory for emotional faces and scenes

Chantal E. Stern,*, Adrian M. Owen, Irene Tracey,*, Rodney B. Look,* Bruce R. Rosen,* and Michael Petrides

Incidental effects of emotional valence in single word processing: An fmri study

Neural Correlates of Temporal Context Retrieval. Fang Wang. Thesis submitted to the faculty of the

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri

Andy C.H. Lee a,b,, Trevor W. Robbins b, Stephen Smith c, Gemma A. Calvert c, Irene Tracey c, Paul Matthews c, Adrian M. Owen a. 1.

Supporting Online Material for

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

Anterior prefrontal cortex and the recollection of contextual information

Supplementary Results: Age Differences in Participants Matched on Performance

Distinct roles for lateral and medial rostral prefrontal cortex in source monitoring of perceived and imagined events

Reasoning and working memory: common and distinct neuronal processes

Giuseppina Rota Graduiertenkolleg Sprachliche Repräsentation und ihre Interpretation Klausurtagung, July 2003

Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration

Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe

The Cognitive Control of Memory: Age Differences in the Neural Correlates of Successful Remembering and Intentional Forgetting

Cultural objects modulate reward circuitry

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease

Distinct Roles for Lateral and Medial Anterior Prefrontal Cortex in Contextual Recollection

NeuroReport 2014, 25: a Department of Education and Psychology, b Languages of Emotion, c Dahlem

Neural Correlates of Successful Encoding Identified Using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion

Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging

A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of short-term source and item memory for negative pictures

Activity in Both Hippocampus and Perirhinal Cortex Predicts the Memory Strength of Subsequently Remembered Information

Parametric functional MR imaging (fmri) studies using block

Dissociating prefrontal contributions during a recency memory task

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Analogical Reasoning and Prefrontal Cortex: Evidence for Separable Retrieval and Integration Mechanisms

Brain Imaging Applied to Memory & Learning

Neural Markers of Inhibition in Human Memory Retrieval

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri

positron-emission tomography study of encoding and retrieval processes

performance of verbal working memory tasks

Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex

Not All False Memories Are Created Equal: The Neural Basis of False Recognition

Frontal Contributions to Memory Encoding Before and After Unilateral Medial Temporal Lobectomy

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance

Face encoding and recognition in the human brain

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Medial Temporal Lobe Activations in fmri and PET Studies of Episodic Encoding and Retrieval

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Role of the ventral striatum in developing anorexia nervosa

Selective Tuning of the Blood Oxygenation Level-Dependent Response during Simple Target Detection Dissociates Human Frontoparietal Subregions

The face-name paired-associates task: an fmri protocol that reliably elicits. hippocampus activation

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Mathematical models of visual category learning enhance fmri data analysis

Stimulus content and the neural correlates of source memory

Lateral and medial prefrontal contributions to emotion generation by semantic elaboration during episodic encoding

Age-related changes in neural activity associated with familiarity, recollection and false recognition

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

The Impact of Anxiety-Inducing Distraction on Cognitive Performance: A Combined Brain Imaging and Personality Investigation

Processing emotional pictures and words: Effects of valence and arousal

Brain regions associated with successful and unsuccessful retrieval of verbal episodic memory as revealed by divided attention

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Selective Abnormal Modulation of Hippocampal Activity During Memory Formation in First-Episode Psychosis

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

Tasks that involve digit series are among the primary tools of neuropsychology

BRAIN RESEARCH 1429 (2012) Available online at

There are, in total, four free parameters. The learning rate a controls how sharply the model

Functional magnetic resonance imaging of executive control in bipolar disorder

Dissociation of human mid-dorsolateral from posterior dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory processing

Experimental design of fmri studies

Table 1. Summary of PET and fmri Methods. What is imaged PET fmri BOLD (T2*) Regional brain activation. Blood flow ( 15 O) Arterial spin tagging (AST)

Brain Regions Associated with the Cambridge Brain Sciences Tests

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1

Neural Bases of Autobiographical Support for Episodic Recollection of Faces

during mnemonic retrieval of verbal information

Prediction of Successful Memory Encoding from fmri Data

Behavioural Brain Research

Transcription:

BRAIN IMAGING Modulation of prefrontal cortex activation by emotional words in recognition memory Lars Kuchinke a,arthurm.jacobs a,melissal.-h.voì a, Markus Conrad a,claudiagrubich b and Manfred Herrmann c,d a Department of Psychology, Freie UniversitÌt Berlin, b Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, K nigin Elisabeth Herzberge Krankenhaus, Berlin, Germany, c Department of Neuropsychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience and d Center for Advanced Imaging, Bremen University, Germany Correspondence and requests for reprints to Lars Kuchinke, Allgemeine Psychologie, Freie UniversitÌt Berlin, Habelschwerdter Allee 45, 14195 Berlin,Germany Tel: + 49 (0)30 83855776; fax: + 49 (0)30 83855620; e-mail: kuchinke@zedat.fu-berlin.de Sponsorship: LKwas supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (JA 823/3-1) and the research group Neuroscience of Emotion at the Hanse Institute for Advanced Study, Delmenhorst/Bremen,Germany. Received 20 March 2006; accepted 28 March 2006 We employed event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine emotional valence e ects on verbal recognition memory. Using a yes/no recognition task, we focussed on prefrontal cortex responses to positive, negative and neutral words. Behavioral data con rmed enhanced processing of emotional items and functional magnetic resonance imaging revealed di erent subregions in the prefrontal cortex supporting retrieval of emotional words. Activations in the right midventrolateral prefrontal cortex correlated with the correct retrieval conditions for negative words, whereas the right ventromedial and orbitofrontal prefrontal cortex showed enhanced responses to positive words. Additionally, di erences between old and new items mainly a ected bilateral orbitofrontal regions when processing positive words. The results are discussed in terms of higher monitoring demands owing to familiarity-based recognition bias for emotional words. NeuroReport 17:1037^1041 c 2006 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Keywords: emotional valence, functional magnetic resonance imaging, negative words, neuroimaging, neutral, prefrontal cortex, positive, recognition memory Introduction Recognition memory is influenced by the affective or emotional valence of the test items. Whereas other imaging studies focus on basic processes underlying episodic retrieval of verbal information [1], the present study was designed to investigate the role of emotional valence for visual word processing in the prefrontal cortex (PFC). In particular, we examined memory performance for positively and negatively valenced words in a yes/no recognition task. Besides the role of medial temporal lobe during episodic memory retrieval [2], PFC is thought to subserve monitoring processes related to familiarity-based judgements at time of retrieval [1]. Orbitofrontal PFC activity is proposed to reflect retrieval success and bilateral mid-dorsolateral PFC was shown to be related to monitoring processes such as evaluation of the retrieved information [3]. If recognition judgements are influenced by the higher familiarity of emotional items [4], one can expect dorsolateral PFC activity to be modulated by emotional valence in the present study. In an event-related potentials (ERPs) study, Windmann and Kutas [5] related an early prefrontal negativity for unstudied negative items judged as old to an automatic bias for emotional material, possibly reflecting more liberal decision criteria for verifying retrieved information. So far, only a few neuroimaging studies attempt to examine the effects of emotional valence on recognition memory for single words, mainly focussing on emotional context effects in sentence processing [6] or pictorial backgrounds [7,8]. According to these studies, orbitofrontal PFC activation was observed for positive emotional contexts, suggesting a role in combination with hippocampus and anterior cingulate gyrus in a network subserving the processing of memories associated with positive affect [8,9]. In contrast, left ventrolateral PFC involvement was found in retrieving items from negative contexts [6,8]. To our knowledge, no other functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) study has examined the contribution of emotional valence to the correct retrieval of items in a standard yes/no recognition task with positive, neutral and negative words. Using an event-related design, we report that the differential effect emotional valence takes on the difference between successful and unsuccessful retrieval of old items. Additionally, we investigated the influence of emotional valence on the so-called old/new effect (the relation between correctly classified old items and correctly rejected new items), previously associated with successful item retrieval mechanisms [5]. Both behavioural and ERP data suggest a modulation of the old/new effect for negative as compared with neutral items. A diminished old/new effect was found for negative items at a late time 0959-4965 c Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Vol 17 No 10 17 July 2006 1037

KUCHINKE ETAL. window on right prefrontal electrodes [4]. On the basis of these results for contextual information, we expected to observe modulated old/new effects for negative items in right PFC in the present study. As this effect is supposed to rely on postretrieval processes, similar neural correlates for positive items should be observed in right PFC. Methods Subjects Twenty right-handed native German-speaking participants (12 females, aged between 20 and 36 years, Ø 25.8) with no history of neurological or psychiatric illness were employed in the study after giving written informed consent in accordance with guidelines set by the local ethics committee. Procedure Immediately after solving a lexical decision task in the scanner [9], participants were scanned during two sessions (study and test phases of the recognition memory task), separated by a short (B5 min) verbal filler task (data not reported here). Target ( old ) stimuli consisted of the 150 German nouns (50 positive, 50 neutral and 50 negative) that had already been presented during the lexical decision task. Additionally, 150 nouns were selected as new distractors. Targets and distractors were selected from the Berlin Affective Word List [10] and did not differ significantly across emotional valence lists on word frequency (M¼20.92/1Mio), word length (5 8 letters), frequency of orthographic neighbours and absolute emotionality ratings (all P40.79, see also [9]). The study phase consisted of a random presentation of the target words centred on a backprojection screen for 1 s, replaced by a fixation cross with a jittered interstimulus interval of 1.2 s (70.2 s). Participants were instructed to memorize the presented words without any button press. During the test phase, participants were asked to decide whether a presented word was old or new to them. Every test trial was initiated by the presentation of a fixation cross in the centre of the screen, randomly followed by either one of 150 old target words or 150 new distractor words displayed for 0.5 s. After this, a fixation cross was displayed until the next appearance of a stimulus (interstimulus interval jittered around 2.870.2 s). Before the next stimulus presentation, participants had to make an old/new judgement by pressing one of two response buttons with either index or middle finger of the right hand. Reaction times and error rates were recorded according to standard recognition memory procedures. Thus, correctly recognized old words were classified as Hits, unrecognized old words as Misses, correctly classified new items as correct rejections (CRs), and new items judged old as false alarms. Imaging and image processing fmri data were acquired from a 3 T Siemens Magnetom Allegra scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) using a whole head, local gradient coil. A single-shot echoplanar imaging sequence was used incorporating the following parameters: echo time 30 ms, repetition time 2.5 s, 64 64 mosaic images with a field of view of 192 mm, field angle 901. Echoplanar imaging comprised 38 slices covering the whole brain taken every 3 mm with no interslice gap. Functional images were slice time corrected, realigned, normalized spatially to the Montreal Neurological Institute template and smoothed with an 8 mm full width at halfmaximum Gaussian kernel using statistical parametric map 2 [11]. The first two images of each session were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration effects. Statistical analysis At the first level, nine event types were modelled: three events describing each emotional category of correctly judged old items (Hits), three events for the correctly rejected new items (CR), two events comprising Misses and false alarms, and a rest category containing missing responses and response latency outliers. Data were highpass filtered (1/128 Hz), corrected for intrinsic autocorrelations and convolved with a standard HRF and its temporal derivative. Contrasts of interest for the three valence categories were then entered into a random effects analysis of variance to provide inferences on the population level. Reported results are based on the canonical HRF alone. To reveal emotion-specific effects, conjunction analyses were performed following a procedure described by Dolcos et al. [12]. Conjunction analysis is used here to identify several activations jointly significant in a series of subtraction contrasts [13]. For instance, the contribution of positive valence on the old/new effect was defined as the joint activation of two Hits4CR contrasts in which (1) positive items elicited higher activations than negative items and (2) positive items showed higher activations than neutral items [(positive4negative) conj (positive4neutral)]. To ensure that this difference was due to positive activations, this conjunction contrast was then inclusively masked (at Po0.05) by the Hits4CR contrast for positive items [12]. A valence-independent contrast was defined as the conjunction of [(positive4neutral) conj (negative neutral)]. Within-PFC regions were identified by thresholding statistical parametric maps with the requirement of six contiguous voxels (Po0.005) to control for type I error. In selected regions of interest, percent signal change was computed using Marsbar Toolbox [14]. Stereotactic Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates are translated into standard Talairach space [15] following nonlinear transformations. Results Participants took longer to respond correctly to new items than to old items, as revealed by a significant main effect of the old/new manipulation (Po0.001) in a repeatedmeasures analysis of variance. Additionally, hit and false alarm rates showed main effects of emotion (P¼0.031 and Po0.001) owing to both higher hit rates for positive (0.749) and negative (0.749) as compared with neutral items (0.715), and higher false alarm rates for positive (0.232) and negative (0.250) as compared with neutral words (0.159; all pairwise Po0.025). In the present study, we focus on emotion-related modulation of PFC activity (whole brain results can be obtained from the authors by request). Valence-specific effects related to correct retrieval (Hits4Misses contrasts) were only observable in right hemisphere PFC. While retrieval of positive words was associated with right ventromedial PFC, negative items showed an activation cluster in ventral part of the right inferior frontal gyrus. A small region in the caudal dorsolateral PFC was more 1038 Vol17 No1017July2006

RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR EMOTIONAL WORDS Table 1 Brain activation during recognition task Region of activation Voxels X Y Z Z-value (a) Hits>Misses Pos4Neg and Neu R posterior orbitofrontal cortex 66 2 36 17 3.35 (BA11) R medial frontal (BA 25) 36 12 28 13 3.04 R medial frontal (BA 25) 7 6 21 16 2.68 Neg4Pos and Neu R mid-ventrolateral PFC 13 4 0 16 12 2.95 (BA13, 47) Neu4Pos and Neg R middle frontal gyrus 6 24 6 35 2.88 Pos and Neg4Neu None (b) HITS>( old/new e ect) Pos4Neg and Neu L orbitofrontal cortex (BA11) 14 32 58 11 3.20 L medial superior frontal (BA 9) 47 10 60 26 3.09 R mid-dorsolateral PFC (BA9) 15 34 21 32 2.93 R orbitofrontal cortex (BA10) 8 42 56 10 2.93 R middle frontal gyrus 26 26 11 31 2.89 R orbitofrontal cortex (BA10) 6 14 34 12 2.71 Neg4Pos and Neu R mid-dorsolateral PFC (BA46) 64 42 30 21 2.97 Neu4Pos and Neg R frontal operculum (BA 45/47) 23 46 16 1 3.18 Pos and Neg4Neu R superior frontal (BA 9) 10 16 47 42 2.96 R mid-dorsolateral PFC 24 32 23 23 3.01 X,Y, Z, stereotaxic coordinates intalairach space [15]. BA, Brodmann s area; L, left; Neg, negative; Neu, neutral; PFC, prefrontal cortex; Pos, positive; R, right. activated during retrieval of neutral items than during retrieval of emotionally valenced words, whereas no region exceeded significance threshold in a valence-independent contrast (conjunction of positive and negative items compared with neutral ones), suggesting a functional dissociation in retrieval of positive and negative words in the PFC (Table 1). Similarly, valence-specific old/new effects were mainly observed in right hemisphere PFC, except for positive words for which significant clusters in left hemisphere orbitofrontal regions were identified with greater activation for old as compared with new items (Fig. 1). Both positive and negative words showed old/new effects in right middorsolateral locations [Brodmann s area (BA) 9/46], accompanied by a more caudal activation in the right frontal operculum for neutral words (BA 45/47). Additionally, a valence-independent contrast revealed right medial activation clusters in superior frontal gyrus (Table 1, Fig. 1). Discussion The present results provide further evidence for valencedependent modulation of neural processing in PFC during word recognition. We identified right ventromedial and middorsolateral PFC regions with valence-dependent activations in retrieval when contrasting responses to correctly recognized and unrecognized items. Additionally, emotional valence effects were elicited in right dorsolateral and bilateral orbitofrontal regions, showing a valence-dependent old/new effect. Thus, the present study extends the findings of ERP studies on emotion-based verbal yes/no recognition tasks [4,5] in two directions: first, using fmri we are able to more precisely specify the frontal regions involved. Second, our (a) Neg>pos&neu (b) Pos&neg>neu (c) Pos>neg&neu L R +30 +46 +60 0.16 0.08 0.00 0.08 42, 30, 21 16, 47, 42 42, 56, 10 0.40 0.45 0.20 0.00 0.30 0.15 0.20 0.00 pos neu neg pos neu neg pos neu neg Fig. 1 Prefrontal regions showing above threshold event-related responses for old compared with new responses. (a) Old/new e ect was greater for negative words compared with both positive and neutral words in right dorsolateral [Brodmann s area (BA) 46] prefrontal cortex (PFC), (b) greater valence-independent old/new e ect in superior (BA 9) PFC, (c) left anterior PFC regions associated with greater old/new e ect for positive words compared with negative and neutral words. Upper panel shows statistical parametric maps displayed on single subject anatomical images. Numbers indicate y-coordinates intalairach space [15]. Lower panel, bar graphs show percent signal change and SEMs for the selected old/new contrasts across participants in regions of interest (coordinates intalairach space). L, left; neg, negative words; neu, neutral words; pos, positive words; R, right. Vol17 No1017July2006 1039

KUCHINKE ETAL. experiment provides additional information on neural correlates of processing positive words. Positive and negative words significantly enhanced participants, behavioural performance as compared with neutral ones. This result replicates earlier findings on the emotion-induced recognition bias [5] by extending it to positive words. Recognition bias is thought to rely on automatic processes underlying implicit emotional evaluation. Convincing evidence points to the role of item familiarity in accounting for this effect [4,5,16]. Participants more often respond old to emotionally valenced items (independently of whether they are actually old or not), because these items seem to be more familiar or more detailed [16,17]. A caudal rostral axis is discussed in recent theories on lateral prefrontal cortex organization [18] with caudal dorsolateral PFC being involved in selection of competing alternatives and rostral parts of the lateral PFC being crucial for monitoring of information in working memory. Rostral right hemisphere PFC activations in the present study for positive and negative Hits might indicate higher postretrieval monitoring processes [1] or additional monitoring related to semantic retrieval [19], whereas more caudally observed regions for neutral Hits point to higher selection demands in working memory. Thus, emotionally valenced words increased activity in regions known to support episodic retrieval or postretrieval monitoring, which for the present study cannot be related to implicitly learned associations with emotional contexts [8]. Possibly, higher familiarity of emotionally valenced words accounts for increased monitoring processes observed in right dorsolateral PFC [1,6,20]. Ventromedial activation as revealed by correctly retrieved positive items in this study goes in line with recent findings concerning its role in the retrieval of positive contextual information [6,8] or the appraisal of reward [21]. A mid-ventrolateral activation as observed for negative Hits is in accordance with a ventral dorsal distinction in lateral PFC [18], relating ventral lateral PFC function to basic explicit mnemonic processes [22]. Successful discrimination between old and new items resulted in bilateral orbitofrontal PFC activations for positive words and anterior dorsolateral regions showing greater responses to the common valence independent contrast. Orbitofrontal cortex is thought to be the highest level on the caudal rostral axis, supporting more abstract control processes [18], for example, being crucial for abstract planning or monitoring of other monitoring processes. Anterior old/new effects for positive items might therefore reflect increased monitoring requirements owing to familiarity/novelty decisions [18,22]. Possibly, orbitofrontal PFC activation reflects changed expectations regarding the significance of the stimuli [22], accompanied by the role of orbitofrontal cortex in a network supporting the processing of positive information [23]. Only positive words elicited activations in anterior parts of PFC in the present study in accordance with its role in a dopaminergic network [24] supporting positive affect. Retrieval of positive items might be subserved through links to positive affective states or feelings during encoding in the study phase. This could result in greater old/new effects in anterior PFC regions as indices of successful access or processing of associated information. It is questionable whether retrieval success is responsible for this pattern [3]. The behavioural data do not show an additional advantage for positive words as compared with negative words. Again, effects of negative words were only observable in the right dorsolateral PFC. Interestingly, by discriminating between the neural responses to old and new items, we could replicate a dissociation between dorsal and ventral parts of the medial PFC seen at encoding of pictoral information [12]. While a dorsal region (BA 9) responded more to a valenceindependent contrast, only ventral regions showed enhanced activity for positive items. Possibly, valence-independent dorso-medial activations can be related to increased self-monitoring processes induced by emotional words [25]; also see [4]. In summary, we identified distinct prefrontal regions supporting recognition memory for emotionally valenced words. Previous functional imaging studies either did not distinguish between emotional valence categories [1 3] or examined emotion effects on context memory [6 8]. By identifying anterior PFC responses to positive words and right lateral PFC responses to negative words, we could relate lateral activities to familiarity-based monitoring processes at retrieval and anterior PFC activations to a common network for processing positive information. References 1. Henson RN, Rugg MD, Shallice T, Dolan RJ. Confidence in recognition memory for words: dissociating right prefrontal roles in episodic retrieval. J Cogn Neurosci 2000; 12:913 923. 2. Rugg MD, Otten LJ, Henson RN. The neural basis of episodic memory: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2002; 357:1097 1110. 3. Rugg MD, Henson RN, Robb WG. Neural correlates of retrieval processing in the prefrontal cortex during recognition and exclusion tasks. Neuropsychologia 2003; 41:40 52. 4. Maratos EJ, Allan K, Rugg MD. Recognition memory for emotionally negative and neutral words: an ERP study. Neuropsychologia 2000; 38:1452 1465. 5. Windmann S, Kutas M. Electrophysiological correlates of emotioninduced recognition bias. J Cogn Neurosci 2001; 13:577 592. 6. Maratos EJ, Dolan RJ, Morris JS, Henson RN, Rugg MD. Neural activity associated with episodic memory for emotional context. Neuropsychologia 2001; 39:910 920. 7. Smith AP, Henson RN, Dolan RJ, Rugg MD. fmri correlates of the episodic retrieval of emotional contexts. Neuroimage 2004; 22:868 878. 8. Erk S, Martin S, Walter H. Emotional context during encoding of neutral items modulates brain activation not only during encoding but also during recognition. Neuroimage 2005; 26:829 838. 9. Kuchinke L, Jacobs AM, Grubich C, Vo ML, Conrad M, Herrmann M. Incidental effects of emotional valence in single word processing: an fmri study. Neuroimage 2005; 28:1022 1032. 10. Võ ML-H, Jacobs AM, Conrad M. Crossvalidating the Berlin Affective Word List (BAWL). Behav Res Methods (in press). 11. SPM2 Toolbox (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm2/). 12. Dolcos F, LaBar KS, Cabeza R. Dissociable effects of arousal and valence on prefrontal activity indexing emotional evaluation and subsequent memory: an event-related fmri study. Neuroimage 2004; 23:64 74. 13. Friston KJ, Penny WD, Glaser DE. Conjunction revisited. Neuroimage 2005; 25:661 667. 14. Marsbar Toolbox (http://marsbar.sourceforge.net/). 15. Talairach J, Tournoux P. A co-planar stereotactic atlas of the human brain. Stuttgart: Thieme; 1988. 16. Kensinger EA, Corkin S. Memory enhancement for emotional words: are emotional words more vividly remembered than neutral words? Mem Cogn 2003; 31:1169 1180. 1040 Vol 17 No 10 17 July 2006

RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR EMOTIONAL WORDS 17. Ochsner KN. Are affective events richly recollected or simply familiar? The experience and process of recognizing feelings past. J Exp Psychol Gen 2000; 129:242 261. 18. Petrides M. Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2005; 360:781 795. 19. Wagner AD, Pare-Blagoev EJ, Clark J, Poldrack RA. Recovering meaning: left prefrontal cortex guides controlled semantic retrieval. Neuron 2001; 31:329 338. 20. Henson RN, Rugg MD, Shallice T, Josephs O, Dolan RJ. Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 1999; 19:3962 3972. 21. Rolls E. The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cereb Cortex 2000; 10:284 294. 22. Petrides M, Alivisatos B, Frey S. Differential activation of the human orbital, mid-ventrolateral, and mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during the processing of visual stimuli. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002; 99:5649 5654. 23. Damasio AR. The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1996; 351:1413 1420. 24. Ashby FG, Isen AM, Turken AU. A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychol Rev 1999; 106:529 550. 25. Lane RD, Reiman EM, Bradley MM, Lang PJ, Ahern GL, Davidson RJ, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of pleasant and unpleasant emotion. Neuropsychologia 1997; 35:1437 1444. Vol17 No1017July2006 1041