Event-related potentials in a Go/Nogo task of abnormal response inhibition in heroin addicts

Similar documents
Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials

Beware misleading cues: Perceptual similarity modulates the N2/P3 complex

The effects of covert attention and stimulus complexity on the P3 response during an auditory continuous performance task

ERP Correlates of Feedback and Reward Processing in the Presence and Absence of Response Choice

ARTICLE IN PRESS Biological Psychology xxx (2011) xxx xxx

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

International Journal of Psychophysiology


Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized. Readiness Potential. Dianna Monteith. Saint Thomas University

ANALYZING EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS

The maturation of interference suppression and response inhibition: ERP analysis of a cued Go/Nogo task.

Neural Correlates of Response Inhibition in Early Childhood: Evidence from a Go/No-Go Task

Asymmetry between the upper and lower visual fields: An event-related potential study

Resisting recently acted-on cues: Compatibility of Go/NoGo responses to response history modulates (frontal P3) event-related potentials

Electrophysiological evidence of two different types of error in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals

Biological Risk Factors

Sensitivity of Electrophysiological Activity from Medial Frontal Cortex to Utilitarian and Performance Feedback

Material-speci c neural correlates of memory retrieval

Analysis and Brain Mechanism of English Learning Characteristics of College Students Considering Self-Efficacy Regulation

Ingroup categorization and response conflict: Interactive effects of target race, flanker compatibility, and infrequency on N2 amplitude

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry

Internal Consistency of Event-Related Potentials Associated with Cognitive Control: N2/P3 and ERN/Pe

Prefrontal dysfunction in drug addiction: Cause or consequence? Christa Nijnens

Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

Event-related brain activity associated with auditory pattern processing

Development of action monitoring through adolescence into adulthood: ERP and source localization

BRAIN RESEARCH 1104 (2006) available at

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): ISSN X

An investigation of the effect of preparation on response execution and inhibition in the go/nogo task

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

NeuroImage 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

Reward positivity is elicited by monetary reward in the absence of response choice Sergio Varona-Moya a,b, Joaquín Morís b,c and David Luque b,d

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention

Title change detection system in the visu

The Influence of Induced Anxiety on the P3 Event Related Brain Potentials of Athletes and Novices in a Go/NoGo Task

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory?

Activation of brain attention systems in individuals with symptoms of ADHD

Running Head: CONFLICT ADAPTATION FOLLOWING ERRONEOUS RESPONSE

Research Article Mindfulness Trait Predicts Neurophysiological Reactivity Associated with Negativity Bias: An ERP Study

1. Department of clinical neurology, Tbilisi State Medical University, Tbilisi, Georgia.

Neuroscience Letters

Cognitive Control Mechanisms Revealed by ERP and fmri: Evidence from Repeated Task-Switching

Supporting Information

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory?

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

Sun, D; Chan, CCH; Hu, Y; Wang, Z; Lee, TMC

Twenty subjects (11 females) participated in this study. None of the subjects had

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

Self-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions: an event-related brain potential study

Cardiac and electro-cortical responses to performance feedback reflect different aspects of feedback processing

Nonaffected ADHD (A) Siblings (S)

Do P1 and N1 evoked by the ERP task reflect primary visual processing in Parkinson s disease?

The Neurobiology of Addiction

The mind s eye, looking inward? In search of executive control in internal attention shifting

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

Neurophysiological correlates of memory illusion in both encoding and retrieval phases

Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia

Developmental Course of Auditory Event-Related Potentials in Autism

Frontal Cortex Mediates Unconsciously Triggered Inhibitory Control

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude

Working Memory Impairments Limitations of Normal Children s in Visual Stimuli using Event-Related Potentials

AUTOCORRELATION AND CROSS-CORRELARION ANALYSES OF ALPHA WAVES IN RELATION TO SUBJECTIVE PREFERENCE OF A FLICKERING LIGHT

The auditory P3 from passive and active three-stimulus oddball paradigm

Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction

In what way does the parietal ERP old new effect index recollection?

P300 Component Modulation During a Go/Nogo Task in Healthy Children

Electrophysiological evidence of enhanced distractibility in ADHD children

Affective-motivational influences on feedback-related ERPs in a gambling task

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS

International Journal of Neurology Research

Attention modulates the processing of emotional expression triggered by foveal faces

Processed by HBI: Russia/Switzerland/USA

BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

EEG-Rhythm Dynamics during a 2-back Working Memory Task and Performance

Generalization Effect of Conditioned Sadness: An Innovation of the Approach to Elicit Implicit Sadness

Pathological Gambling: Neurobiological research and its relevance for treatment and relapse prevention: Recent research and future questions

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Teaching brain-machine interfaces as an alternative paradigm to neuroprosthetics control

ERP correlates of retrieval orientation: Direct versus indirect memory tasks

Behavioural and electrophysiological measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions

Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse. Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver

The face-sensitive N170 encodes social category information Jonathan B. Freeman a, Nalini Ambady a and Phillip J. Holcomb a

Redmond G. O Connell 1,2, Paul M. Dockree 1,2, Mark A. Bellgrove 3, Alessandra Turin 1,2, Seamus Ward 4, John J. Foxe 2,5,6, and Ian H.

Transcription:

Science in China Series C: Life Sciences 2009 SCIENCE IN CHINA PRESS Springer Article www.scichina.com life.scichina.com www.springer.com/scp www.springerlink.com Event-related potentials in a Go/Nogo task of abnormal response inhibition in heroin addicts YANG Bo 1*, YANG SuYong 2*, ZHAO Lun 3, YIN LiuHua 1, LIU Xu 1 & AN ShaSha 1 1 Laboratory of Psychology, School of Sociology, China University of Political Science and Law, Beijing 102249, China; 2 National Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neurosciences and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China; 3 Institute of Language, Xuzhou Normal University, Xuzhou 221116, China Inhibitory control dysfunction is regarded as a core feature in addicts. The major objective of this study was to explore the time course of response inhibition in chronic heroin addicts and provide the neurophysiological evidence of their inhibitory control dysfunction. The amplitudes and latencies of ERP components were studied in fourteen heroin addicts (mean duration of heroin use being (13.54±5.71) years (Mean±SD), average abstinence being ((4.67±6.44) months)) and fourteen matched healthy controls with a visual Go/Nogo task. Our results showed that heroin addicts demonstrated significantly larger Go-N2 amplitudes which results in a decreased N2 Go/Nogo effect, but no statistically significant differences were found between heroin addicts and controls in P3. The ERP data suggest that fronto-central areas of heroin addicts were impaired during the inhibition process (200 300 ms) and over-activated to targets. The impaired early process might reflect an abnormal conflict monitoring process in heroin addicts. These results consolidate the inhibitory control dysfunction hypothesis in chronic heroin users. heroin addicts, events-related potentials, inhibitory control, conflict monitoring, response inhibition Inhibitory control is an essential ability for suppressing irrelevant or interfering stimuli and impulses for successful living [1]. Impaired inhibitory control contributes to severe behavioral disinhibition and impulsivity. Drugs Seeking and relapse are strong impulsive behaviors, which are core symptoms of drug addiction [2]. Relapse to heroin use occurs even after long periods of abstinence, induced by drugs and drug-associated cues. Lack of inhibition has been regarded as a key element of drug addiction, including the suppression of emotional, cognitive and behavioral responses [3,4]. One of the primary domains for measuring inhibitory control in the laboratory is response inhibition [5]. Behavioral or motor response inhibition is defined as the process required for stopping a planned movement [6]. In response tasks, behavioral indexes, such as response time, hit rate and false rate, were measured to assess response control ability. Previous research showed that heroin addicts performed inconsistently in different re- sponse inhibition tasks. Significant group differences were found in the Porteus Maze Test (PMT) [7] and in the Stop-Signal task [8], but not in the Go/No go task [9] and Stop-Change tasks [10]. Thus, the neuropsychological substrate of disinhibition of heroin addicts remains unclear. Response inhibition recruits a neural network including the frontal regions and the non-frontal regions, such as the orbital frontal cortex (OFC), the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC),and the parietal, temporal, and striatal lobes [11]. Structural magnetic resonance imaging studies showed that prefrontal and temporal gray matter density decreases in Received January 28, 2009; accepted March 17, 2009 doi: 10.1007/s11427-009-0106-4 * Contibuted equally to this work Corresponding author (email: zsdybo@sina.com )C Supported by Beijing Bureau of Reeducation-through-laber Administration (Grant NO. 2006021515) Citation: YANG B, YANG S Y, ZHAO L, et al. Event-related potentials in a Go/Nogo task of abnormal response inhibition in heroin addicts. Sci China Ser C-Life Sci, 2009, 52(8): 780-788, doi: 10.1007/s11427-009-0106-4

chronic heroin addicts [12,13]. Furthermore, function magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) results indicated that the decreased activity of the frontal and parietal regions was observed not only in current heroin addicts [14] but also among abstinent [15] in the response inhibition task. These results demonstrated the neural substrates of impulsive behavior in addicts, but the time course requires further research. Event-related potentials (ERPs) reveal the time course of information processes with high temporal resolution. ERP studies have provided electrophysiological indices for cognitive functions in heroin addicts [16,17]. In visual ERP recordings, Bauer has found that opioid-dependent patients exhibited decreased P300 amplitudes in a short-term memory test, suggesting that P300 is a useful measure of central nervous system (CNS) recovery [16]. In the laboratory, inhibition and impulsiveness assessed with the Stop task [18] or with the Go/Nogo task [19]. Both tasks shared the same inhibitory neurocognitive network, but the left hemispheric areas were more involved in a Go/Nogo task than in a Stop task, which indicates a left frontoparietal specialization for response selection [11]. However, event-related potentials might be problematic due to the possible overlap of response signals and stop signals. The Go/Nogo task yields a relatively simple ERP structure [20], which is suitable for comparing differences between clinical groups in ERP research. In a Go/Nogo task, participants are asked to respond to quickly presented Go cues and suppress the prepared response whenever a Nogo cue is presented. The seemingly simple task involves multiple cognitive processes, including stimuli discrimination, response selection, motor preparation, response inhibition, and error monitoring [21]. Two major ERP components have been described in Go/Nogo tasks. The first component, the N2 or (Nogo-N2), is regarded as a phasic negative shift in Nogo compared to Go trials, with a maximum amplitude over frontal scalp locations during 250 and 350 ms post-stimulus. The second component, the Nogo-P3, is a positive deflection of around 300 to 600 ms poststimulus with a larger amplitude than Go-P3 or P3b in the fronto-central area, also called Nogo P300 anteriorization [22,23]. It has been suggested that the difference waves (Nogo- minus Go- ERPs) would reflect the Go/Nogo effect, such as the difference waves of N2 (N2d), which were defined as the results of Go-N2 subtracted from the Nogo-N2 [23,24]. Although both components are considered to be associated with response inhibition, the exact cognitive processes they reflect remains uncertain. One view was that the Nogo-N2 reflected a cognitive top-down inhibition mechanism for suppressing the incorrect tendency to respond to operating at a processing stage prior to motor execution [22,25]. Another view was that the N2 reflected conflict monitoring processing rather than response inhibition P,27]. The Nogo-P3 has been proposed as reflecting response inhibition, as well [28,29]. However, it has also been suggested that an increased frontal P3 in Nogo trials is also caused by the absence of an overlap of movement-related potentials which instead influence the P3 in Go trials [30]. Source localizations of the Nogo-N2 [31,32] and Nogo-P3 [31,33] components in Go/Nogo tasks revealed that the bilateral sources pair were located in OFC and ACC. Although no consensus has been reached, the Nogo-N2 appears to be related to conflict per se or rather the consequences of a conflict, namely inhibition or revision of inappropriate response tendencies and the Nogo-P3 is related to motor inhibition [20]. There are few published reports concering response inhibition of substance addicts by ERPs. Previous research did find that chronic abuse of alcohol [34] and the drug ecstasy [35] changed the late process (Nogo-P3) in response to the inhibition task. No significant change of the N2 component was found compared with the control group [34]. However, there are no ERP research reports available for the neuralpsychological mechanism of response inhibition in heroin addicts. To this end, ERPs in the visual Go/Nogo task were compared between heroin addicts and healthy controls to explore the response inhibition time course changes and provide neurophysiological evidence for inhibitory control dysfunction in heroin addicts. An equal probability Go/Nogo task had been used in several clinical studies to elicit robust Nogo ERPs [34]. Because the amplitudes of N2 and P3 in the response inhibition task are sensitive to the Nogo probability [28], the equal probability task was used to eliminate the bias caused by low-probability stimuli. We hypothesized that heroin addicts would exhibit attenuated Nogo ERP effects in the fronto-central areas compared with healthy controls. 1 Methods 1.1 Participants Heroin addicts were recruited from the Health Center of YANG B et al. Sci China Ser C-Life Sci Aug. 2009 vol. 52 no. 8 780-788 781

the Beijing Reeducation School. All patients met the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) [2] criteria for drug abuse/ dependence, exclusively with regard to heroin abuse or dependence. Patients with previous consumption of other drugs, use of psychotropic agents, or excessive alcohol intake were excluded. There were fourteen male heroin addicts in the patient group, with a mean duration of heroin abuse of (13.54±5.71) (Mean±SD) years, a mean dosage of (1.46±1.27) g per day, and a mean abstinence duration of (4.67±6.44) months. The twentyeight items Opioid Addiction Severity Inventory (OASI- 28) [36] was used to rate heroin addiction severity. Three of the patients were classified as moderate addicts, and the others were classified as severe addicts. Fourteen healthy controls, matched in age and education level (age: patients= (41±7.11) years, controls= (41±10.50) years; education: patients= (8.92±1.89) years, controls= (9.69±2.18) years, all t-test P>0.05), were recruited from the local community. The Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) [37] was used to rate the anxiety level of addicts and controls, and a significant difference was found between the groups (BAI: addicts= (12.21±14.39), controls=(4.57±5.69), t (BB)=1.85, P<0.05 ). All participants were right-handed with normal or corrected-to-normal vision. These participants had no known history of head trauma or other medical/psychiatric conditions that could cause cognitive impairment. All participants were paid volunteers. The purpose of the study was explained to each participant, and written informed consent was obtained. 1.2 Stimuli and procedure Visual stimuli were two equilateral triangles (upright and inverted) of the same size (wide=7.00 cm, high=6.06 cm) in grey background. Each stimulus was presented in the center of a computer screen(light degree= 60 cd/m 2 )by the Neuroscan STIM-2 system. Participants were seated in a sound-attenuated semi-dark room, facing a monitor placed 100 cm from their eyes, with a visual angle of 4 4. In the task, 100 Go stimuli and 100 Nogo stimuli were pseude-randomly displayed one by one. The participants were instructed to respond by pressing a button using their thumbs as quickly as possible after the Go stimuli appeared and to withhold the response when the Nogo stimuli appeared. The number of times each stimulus was used as Go and No-go stimulus was counterbalanced across subjects in each group. Each stimulus was presented for 50 ms, with the mean inter-trial intervals (ITI) being 1500 ms (randomly between 1000 2000 ms). The hand to press button was counterbalanced across the participants. Before EEG recording, participants performed two practice blocks consisting of 20 Go and Nogo trials, to ascertain that their operation was correct. During the experiment, participants were instructed to watch the center of the screen, relax, and minimize eye blinks or body movements. 1.3 Data recording and off-line analysis Scalp voltages were recorded using a 32-channel Ag/ AgCl electrodes cap (10 20 International System). The acquisition software was NeuroScan SynAmps (El Paso, TX). The electrodes were referenced to the linked mastoids and the ground electrode was on the forehead (frontal midline). Eye movements were monitored with a supraorbital vertical lead and a horizontal lead placed on the external canthus of the eyes. Electrode impedance was maintained below 5 kv. The EEG signals were recorded continuously with a bandpass at 0.05 100 Hz and digitized with a 1000 Hz sampling rate. The software used for analyzing the recorded signals was NeuroScan SCAN4.3. Ocular artifacts were corrected with an eye-movement correction algorithm described by Semlitsch and his colleagues [38]. ERP epochs with a 1000 ms duration were extracted (including 200 ms before and 800 ms after stimulus onset) and corrected by the 200 ms baseline. EEG exceeding ±100 μv were automatically rejected as artifacts. Individual ERP averages were derived for correct Go and Nogo trials, and digitally low-pass filtered with zero phase shift (0 17 Hz, 24 db/octave). 1.4 Statistical analysis Behavioral indexes, including Go response time, hit rate, and false rate, were compared by t-test to explore the group difference between the heroin addicts and the controls. The N2 component was quantified as the most negative amplitude within a 200 to 300 ms window following stimulus onset. The P3 component was quantified as the most positive amplitude within 300 to 500 ms following the N2 peak. In order to highlight the Nogo effect, difference waves (Nogo minus Go) were computed for N2 and P3, respectively designated as N2d and P3d [22]. The 782 YANG B et al. Sci China Ser C-Life Sci Aug. 2009 vol. 52 no. 8 780-788

peak latency was respectively computed at the site Fz and Fcz, for N2 and P3. To assess the different effects of response inhibition between addicts and healthy controls, a Response (Go, Nogo) Site (5 midline sites: Fz, Fcz, Cz, Cpz, Pz) Group (addicts, controls) repeated measures ANOVA was performed for peak N2 and P3 amplitudes. A Site (5 midline sites: Fz, Fcz, Cz, Cpz, Pz) Group (addicts, controls) repeated measures ANOVA was performed for peak N2d and P3d amplitudes. A Response (Go, Nogo) Group (addicts, controls) repeated measures ANOVA was performed for the latency of N2 and P3. The Geisser-Greenhouse correction was used for any repeated measures containing more than one degree of freedom in the numerator [39]. The statistical significance was set at the P<0.05 level. All estimates of effect sizes were reported using partial Eta squared values (η 2 ), which described the proportion of certain effect variance accounted for in the total variances of the effect and errors. Statistical analysis was performed with the statistical package SPSS 13.0. 2 Results 2.1 Behavioral results No significant group differences were found between addicts and control groups in all behavioral indices: the mean Go-response time (addicts=(359.42±59.59) ms, controls=(360.72±.57) ms, t (BB)= 0.07; P>0.05), the mean ratio of hit (addicts =(0.98±0.03), controls =(0.96± 0.06), t (BB)=0.85; P>0.05), and the mean ratio of false alarm (addicts =(0. 04±0.04), controls =(0. 05±0.07), t (BB)= 0.34; P>0.05) 2.2 ERP results The waveforms and topography of ERPs in heroin addicts and controls during the Nogo as well as the Go condition are respectively illustrated in Figure 1 and Figure 2. (1) N2. With regard to N2 amplitudes, both the response main effect (Nogo-N2=(3.86±0.66) μv, Go-N2 =(5.85±0.66) μv, F (1,BB)=20.05, P<0.05, η 2 =0.44) and the site main effect (F (4,104) =4.35, P<0.05, η 2 =0.14) were significant, but the Group main effect was not (F (1,BB)= 1.04, P>0.05). The interaction of the Response and Group was significant (F (1,BB)=13.71, P<0.05, η 2 =0.35). A simple effects analysis further revealed that a larger Nogo-N2 amplitude was observed in the controls but not in the addicts (Controls: Nogo-N2 controls =(3.67±0.93) μv, Go-N2 controls =(7.31±0.94) μv, F (1,BB)=33.46, P<0.05; Addicts: Nogo-N2 addicts =(4.05±0.92) μv, Go-N2 addicts =(4.39±0.94) μv, F (1,BB)=0.30, P>0.05). Addicts exhibited enhanced N2 amplitude compared with controls among Go trials (Go-N2 controls =(7.31±0.94) μv, Go N2 addicts =(4.39±0.94) μv, F (1,BB)=4.84, P<0.05). Among Nogo trails, no significant group difference in N2 amplitude was observed (Nogo-N2 controls =(3.67±0.93) μv, Nogo-N2 addicts =(4.05±0.92) μv, F (1,BB)=0.01, P>0.05). There was no significant interaction between the Site and Group (F (4,104) =0.52, P>0.05). The repeated-measures ANOVA on N2 amplitudes of the Nogo-Go difference wave revealed a significant main effect of the Group (N2d controls=( 4.98±0.57) μv, N2d addicts =( 2.72±0.57) μv, F (1,BB)=7.81, P<0.01, η 2 = 0.23). There was no effect observed in the latency of N2 (all P>0.05). (2) P3. Neither the Group effect (F (1,BB)=0.15, P>0.05) nor a Response by Group interaction (F (1,BB)=0.031, P>0.05) or a Site by Group interaction (F (4,104) =0.78, P>0.05) on P3 amplitudes was found. Consistent with previous studies, the Site main effect (F (4,104) =31.85, P<0.05, η 2 =0.55) and a Response by Site interaction (F (4,104) =45.00, P<0.05, η 2 =0.63) were observed. Post-hoc tests revealed that Nogo-P3 was significantly larger than Go-P3 in anterior sites (Fz, Fcz) (P<0.05) while the Go-P3 was larger than Nogo-P3 in posterior sites (Cpz, Cz)(P<0.05), which led to the Site effect of P3. There was a significant Site effect in P3d (F (4,104) = 63.84, P<0.05, η 2 =0.71) and the largest P3d was shown in Fcz. There was no effect observed in the latency of P3 (all P>0.05). 3 Discussion In the present study, we used a visual Go/Nogo paradigm for investigating N2 and P3 in heroin addicts and healthy controls. No performance differences were found between the two groups [9,14]. This finding was probably due to the fact that relatively little effort is required for participants to perform the simple Go/Nogo task. Consistent with previous research, the equal probability Go/Nogo task elicited a strong Nogo effect, a larger Nogo-N2 amplitude and anterior Nogo-P3, especially in healthy controls [19,30]. The Nogo effect was attenuated in the fronto-central area of heroin addicts, YANG B et al. Sci China Ser C-Life Sci Aug. 2009 vol. 52 no. 8 780-788 783

Figure 1 Grand mean ERP for the Go and Nogo stimuli for heroin addicts and controls. Figure 2 Topographic maps of N2 and P3 amplitude in heroin addicts and controls. 784 YANG B et al. Sci China Ser C-Life Sci Aug. 2009 vol. 52 no. 8 780-788

which confirmed our hypothesis. The ERP results showed the diminished N2 Go/Nogo effect and decreased N2d amplitudes in heroin addicts compared with the controls. The most significant finding of this study was that heroin addicts had larger Go-N2 amplitudes than controls, which resulted in reduced N2d amplitudes. We also observed a Nogo P3 anteriorization which probably reflected the response inhibition in Nogo trails [24,30]. No significant group effect of P3 on either the amplitudes or the distribution on the scalp was found. Those results indicated that the heroin addicts had an abnormal early process (200 350 ms) in the response inhibition task compared with the healthy controls. The abnormal ERP pattern was first reported in heroin addiction and other substance abuse studies. Possible implications and causative factors concerning the special ERP characteristics of heroin addicts will be discussed. 3.1 Response inhibitory process of heroin addicts As noted earlier, Nogo N2 and P3 are two primary components related to inhibitory control in the Go/Nogo task. However, no consensus on the psychological significance of Nogo-N2 and Nogo-P3 has been reached. The Nogo-N2 or N2 Nogo effect was presumed to reflect a top-down inhibition [22] or conflict monitoring P,27]. However, the inhibition hypothesis could not explain the enhanced Go-N2 amplitudes, such as the N2 likeness negative component in the rare frequency Go condition P]. Neither could the inhibition hypothesis explain the enhanced Go-N2 amplitudes in heroin addicts. Otherwise, inhibition would not occur in a Go response which involves no control inhibition, and the change of N2d amplitude would be attributed to the Nogo-N2. According to the inhibition hypothesis, enhanced N2d was related to more efficient inhibition [22]. In our study, a significantly larger N2d was observed in the controls, but no significant behavioral differences, such as false alarm rates, were found between two groups. The ceiling effect of the simple task could also lead to such behavioral results. Furthermore, Dipole source analysis located the Nogo-N2 as well as the Error related negative (ERN) in ACC. N2 was supposed to reflect a general conflict monitoring process P]. Substantial evidence indicates that the early ERP components reflect attentional processes triggered by task demands [40], and the enhanced N2 to Go stimuli may reflect enhanced cognitive resources allocated to the Go stimuli. According to the conflict monitoring hypothesis, we speculated that the Go stimuli would be a strong conflict signal to heroin addicts, which led to the increased Go-N2 and decreased N2 Nogo effect (N2d). fmri results suggested that ACC activities decreased in heroin addicts compared with controls, consistent with the attenuated N2d amplitudes in the fronto-central sites. As the ACC function was assessed by subtracting the Go condition from the Nogo condition, we supposed that the attenuation of activity in the anterior cingulate might be due to hyperactivity in the Go condition, based on the increased Go-N2 amplitudes in heroin addicts. Nogo-P3 or anterior P3-No Go was supposed to reflect a general inhibition process [28,29]. No significant differences in the amplitude and the latency of P3 (P3d, Go-P3, Nogo-P3) were found between the addicts and the controls. We supposed that the general inhibition process of the addicts was normal. In support of this idea, the behavioral data demonstrated that the addicts performed as well as the healthy controls, consistent with previous research [9,10]. We supposed that (i) the diminished N2 Nogo effect (Nogo vs. Go) and attenuated N2d amplitudes of the heroin addicts reflected an abnormal early process. (ii) the increased Go-N2 amplitudes indicated that more attentional resources were engaged or required for heroin addicts to monitor the response signal. In addition, the group difference was observed using the difference wave (N2d) but not the original ERP waves in the Nogo condition (Nogo-N2) as the index of the N2 Go/Nogo effect, which indicated that the difference wave might be a more sensitive index for detecting neuropsychological changes in heroin addition. 3.2 Potential reasons for the abnormal process The unique ERPs pattern identified in this study may be caused by two salient factors. One is the structural damage in brains of chronic heroin users. At the gross structure level, opiate-dependent addicts exhibited decreased gray matter density in both the prefrontal and the temporal cortex [41]. At the cellular level, neuron damage in the bilateral frontal cortex have been reported [42,43]. fmri studies have found that heroin addicts had a hypoactivation of PFC which involves response inhibition [14,15], whereby addicts exhibited decreased N2d amplitudes on fronto-central scalp sites compared with the controls. These results might have been caused by structural damage in the frontal lobe. YANG B et al. Sci China Ser C-Life Sci Aug. 2009 vol. 52 no. 8 780-788 785

However, the neuropathological changes in opiate users have been far less investigated to date [5]. The exact mechanism of damage to brain structure and function caused by opioids remains uncertain with further neuropharmacological research is indicated. Another possible reason is personality trait or heredity. One of the primary personality dimensions that have been consistently linked to addicts is impulsivity [44]. People who had higher scores on Eysenck s Impulsiveness Questionnaire (EIQ) [45] or decreased response times in the Go/Nogo task [46] display decreased Nogo-P3 but no change of N2 in response to inhibition tasks. By contrast, heroin addicts in the present study and impulsive-violent offenders [47] exhibited significantly lower amplitudes of N2 difference waves. Because the Nogo-P3 was thought to be motor or response inhibition itself [28,29], we suspected that the sort criteria as EIQ or response time might reflect only one component of impulsivity, i.e., decreased motor inhibition. The attenuated N2 effect in behavioraly abnormal individuals might indicate another component of impulsivity, such as excessive conflict monitoring due to an earlier inhibition control process dysfunction P,27]. In twin studies, genetic model-fitting analysis showed that about 60% of the variance in frontal N2 (N2d) and P3 (Go-P3, Nogo-P3) amplitudes in the Go/Nogo task could be attributed to genetic factors [48]. This finding indicates that the abnormal ERP patterns in heroin addicts may be inherited from their parents and cause them to be more impulsive. It may explain why individuals who manifest impulsive behaviors are more likely to engage in substance misuse [49]. As most addicts have been involved in the cycle of addiction, rehabilitation, and relapse, the abnormal ERP pattern might be caused by the interaction of drugs and heredity. 3.3 Limitations and future directions As our study was the first ERP research of inhibitory control in heroin addiction, our data leaves many questions unanswered concerning neuropsychological process of response inhibition in heroin addicts. First, in this study heroin abstinent addicts (of more than one month) were compared with normal controls but not with current users. Using a short memory task, Papageorgiou and his colleagues found that the P3 amplitude was smaller in opioid-abstinent addicts than in current users and no significant difference existed between current users and healthy controls [17]. Whether or not the N2 component of current users in the response inhibition task might show normal amplitudes should be confirmed in future studies. Second, a simple Go/Nogo task was used to compare the neural activation in the response inhibition process between the heroin addicts and healthy controls. Although the response inhibition process revealed a general mechanism of inhibitory control or a possible neural basis such as the frontal-hypothalamic loop, whether or not other inhibition processes, such as inhibition of emotion or memory, share the same inhibitory control mechanism or not, remains uncertain [6]. We will extend our studies of heroin addiction to other inhibitory control fields, such as emotional inhibition. Finally, impulsive behavior, especially drug seeking and relapse in heroin addicts, generally occur in socialogically complex environments. Emotional stress is a well known context factor contributing to drug use and relapse [50]. The N2 component was sensitive to time pressure [51] and emotional valence [52] in Go/Nogo task. Manipulating the emotional stress variables in the Go/Nogo task may help to illustrate the relationship between emotional stress, inhibitory control, and drug abuse [53]. 4 Conclusions We found that heroin addicts exhibited a diminished Go/Nogo effect in the response inhibition process (200 300 ms), and the attenuated effect was due to the overactivation towards the target stimuli. The results reflected the impaired conflict monitoring process of the response inhibition in chronic heroin addicts. The ERP data provided neurophysiological evidence for impulsivity or impaired inhibition control in heroin addicts. 1 Garavan H, Ross T J, Stein E A. Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: An event-related functional MRI study. HProc Natl Acad Sci USAH, 1999, 96: 8301 8306[DOI] 2 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association. 1994 3 Jentsch J D, Taylor J R. Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology, 1999, 146: 373 390[DOI] 4 Goldstein R Z, Volkow N D. Drug addiction and it s underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of 786 YANG B et al. Sci China Ser C-Life Sci Aug. 2009 vol. 52 no. 8 780-788

the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiat, 2002, 159: 1642 1652[DOI] 5 Ersche K D, Sahakian B J. The neuropsychology of amphetamine and opiate dependence: Implications for treatment. Neuropsychol Rev, 2007, 17: 317 336[DOI] 6 Aron A R. The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. Neuroscientist, 2007, 13: 214 228[DOI] 7 Pau C W H, Lee T M C, Chan S F. The impact of heroin on frontal executive functions. Arch Clin Neuropsych, 2002, 17: 663 670 8 Lu Y. The Function of Heroin Abstinents Behavior Inhibition and Dynamic Characteristics (in Chinese). Dissertation for the Master degree, Kunming: Yunnan Normal University, 2006: 13 15 9 Verdejo-Garacia A J, Perales J C, Perez-Garcia M. Cognitive impulsivity in cocaine and heroin polysubstance abuses. Addict Behav, 2007, 32: 950 966[DOI] 10 Fishbein D H, Krupitsky E, Flannery B A, et al. Neurocognitive characterizations of Russian heroin addicts without a significant history of other drug use. Drug Alcohol Depen, 2007, 90: 25 38[DOI] 11 Rubia K, Russell T, Overmeyer S, et al. Mapping motor inhibition: conjunctive brain activations across different versions of Go/No-Go and stop tasks. NeuroImage, 2001, 13: 250 1[DOI] 12 Lyoo I K, Pollack M H, Silveri M M, et al. Prefrontal and temporal gray matter density decreases in opiate dependence. Psychopharmacology, 2006, 184: 139 144[DOI] 13 Haselhorst R, Dürsteler MacFarland K M, Scheffler M A K, et al. Frontocortical N-acetylaspartate reduction associated with long-term IV heroin use. Neurology, 2002, 58: 305 307 14 Lee T M C, Zhou W H, Lou X J, et al. Neural activity associated with cognitive regulation in heroin users: a fmri study. Neurosci Lett, 2005, 382: 211 216[DOI] 15 Fu L P, Bi G H, Zou Z T, et al. Impaired response inhibition function in abstinent heroin dependents: An fmri study. Neurosci Lett, 2008, 438: 322 3[DOI] 16 Bauer L O. CNS recovery from cocaine, cocaine and alcohol, or opioid dependence: a P300 study. Clin Neurophysiol, 2001, 112: 1508 1515[DOI] 17 Papageorgiou C C, Liappas I A, Ventouras E M, et al. Long-term abstinence syndrome in heroin addicts: indices of P300 alterations associated with a short memory task. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry, 2004, 28: 1109 1115[DOI] 18 Logan G D, Cowan W B, Davis K A. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 1984, 10: 276 291[DOI] 19 Pfefferbaum A, Ford J M, Weller B J, et al. ERPs to response production and inhibition. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 1985, 60: 423 434[DOI] 20 Ruchsow M, Groen G, Kiefer M. Response inhibition in borderline personality disorder: event-related potentials in a Go/Nogo task. J Neural Transm, 2008, 115: 127 133[DOI] 21 Goldstein M, Brendel G, Tuescher, O, et al. Neural substrates of the interaction of emotional stimulus processing and motor inhibitory control: An emotional linguistic go/no-go fmri study. NeuroImage, 2007, 36: 10 1040[DOI] 22 Falkenstein M, Hoormann J, Hohnsbein J. ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. Acta Psychologica, 1999, 101: 7 291[DOI] 23 Kopp B, Mattler U, Goertz R, et al. N2, P3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task involving selective response priming. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol,, 1996, 99: 19 27 24 Bokura H, Yamaguchi S, Kobayashi S. Electrophsiological correlates for response inhibition in a Go/Nogo task. Clin Neurophysiol, 2001, 113: 2224 2232[DOI] 25 Kim M S, Kim Y Y, Yoo S Y, et al. Electrophysiological correlates of behavioral response inhibition in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depress Anxiety, 2007, 24: 22 31[DOI] Nieuwenhuis S, Yeung N, van den Wildenberg, et al. Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulated function in a go/nogo task: effects of response conflict and trial type frequency. Cognit Affect Behav Neurosci, 2003, 3: 17 [DOI] 27 Donkers F C, van Boxtel G J. The N2 in go/no-go tasks reflects conflict monitoring not response inhibition. Brain Cogn, 2004, 56: 165 176 28 Bruin K J, Wijers A A. Inhibition, response mode, and stimulus probability: a comparative event-related potential study. Clin Neurophysiol, 2002, 113: 1172 1182[DOI] 29 Burle B, Vidal F, Bonnet M. Electroencephalographic nogo potentials in a no-movement context: the case of motor imagery in humans. Neurosci Lett, 2004, 360: 77 80[DOI] 30 Salisbury D F, Griggs C B, Shenton M E, et al. The NoGo P300 anteriorization' effect and response inhibition. Clin Neurophysiol, 2004, 115: 1550 1558[DOI] 31 Dimoska A, Johnstone S J, Barry R J. The auditory-evoked N2 and P3 components in the stop-signal task: indices of inhibition, response-conflict or error-detection? Brain Cogn, 2006, 62: 98 112[DOI] 32 Bekker E M, Kenemans J L, Verbaten M N. Source analysis of the N2 in a cued Go/NoGo task. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res, 2005, 22: 221 231[DOI] 33 Strik W K, Fallgatter A J, Brandeis D, et al. Three-dimensional tomography of event-related potentials during response inhibition: Evidence for phasic frontal lobe activation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 1998, 108: 406 413[DOI] 34 Kamarajan C, Porjesz B, Jones K A, et al. Alcoholism is a disinhibitory disorder: neurophysiological evidence from a Go/No-Go task. Biol Psychol, 2005, 69: 353 373[DOI] 35 Gamma A, Brandeis D, Brandeis R, et al. The P3 in ecstasy polydrug users during response inhibition and execution. J Psychopharmacol, 2005, 19: 504 512[DOI] 36 Lian Z, Liu Z. Reliability and development of opiate addiction severity inventory (in Chinese). Chinese Magazine of Drug Abuse Prevention and Treatment, 2003: 85 88 37 Cheng K S, Wong C, Wong K, et al. A study of psychometric properties, normative scores and factor structure of beck anxiety inventory Chinese version. Chin J Clin Psychol, 2002: 4 6 38 Semlitsch H V, Anderer P, Schuster P, et al. A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology, 1986, 23: 695 703[DOI] 39 Geisser S, Greenhouse S W. An extension of Box s results on the use of the F distribution in multivariate analysis. Ann Math Stat, 1958, 29: 885 891[DOI] 40 Fabiani F, Gratton G, Federmeier K D. Event-related brain potentials: YANG B et al. Sci China Ser C-Life Sci Aug. 2009 vol. 52 no. 8 780-788 787

methods, theory, and applications. In: Cacioppo J T, Tassinary L G, Berntson G G. Eds, Handbook of Psychophysiology (3rd), Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 2007. 85 119 41 Lyoo I K, Pollack M H, Silveri M M, et al. Prefrontal and temporal gray matter density decreases in opiate dependence. Psychopharmacology, 2006, 184: 139 44[DOI] 42 Haselhorst R, Dursteler-MacFarland K M, Scheffler K M, et al. Frontocortical N-acetylaspartate reduction associated with long-term IV heroin use. Neurology, 2002, 58: 305 307 43 Xia J, Zhou Y, Yang B, et al. A clinical MR spectroscopy study of heroin-induced brain damage (in Chinese). HCentral China Medical JournalH, H2004: 227 228 44 Dawe S, Gullo M J, Loxton N J. et al. Reward drive and rash impulsiveness as dimensions of impulsivity: implications for substance misuse. Addict Behav, 2004, 29: 1389 1405[DOI] 45 Dimoska A, Johnstone S J. Neural mechanisms underlying trait impulsivity in non-clinical adults: Stop-signal performance and event-related potentials. Prog Neuro-Psychoph, 2007, 31: 443 454[DOI] 46 Ruchsow M, Groen G, Kiefer M, et al. Impulsiveness and ERP components in a Go/Nogo task. J Neural Transm, 2008, 115: 909 915[DOI] 47 Chen C Y, Tien Y M, Juan C H, et al. Neural correlates of impulsive-violent behavior: an event-related potential study. Neuroreport, 2005, 16: 1213 1216[DOI] 48 Anokhin A P, Heath A C, Myers E. Genetics, prefrontal cortex, and cognitive control: a twin study of event-related brain potentials in a response inhibition task. Neurosci Lett, 2004, 368: 314 318[DOI] 49 Elkins I J, King S M, McGue M, et al. Personality traits and the development of nicotine, alcohol, and illicit drug disorders: prospective links from adolescence to young adulthood. J Abnorm Psychol, 2006, 115: 39[DOI] 50 Sinha R. The role of stress in addiction relapse. Curr Psychiatry Rep, 2007, 9: 388 395[DOI] 51 Jodo E, Kayama Y. Relation of a negative ERP component to response inhibition in a Go/No-go task. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 1992, 82: 477 482[DOI] 52 Chiu P H, Holmes A J, Pizzagalli D A. Dissociable recruitment of rostral anterior cingulate and inferior frontal cortex in emotional response inhibition. NeuroImage, 2008, 42: 988 997[DOI] 53 Li C R, Sinha R. Inhibitory control and emotional stress regulation: Neuroimaging evidence for frontal-limbic dysfunction in psycho-stimulant addiction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2008, 32: 581 597[DOI] 788 YANG B et al. Sci China Ser C-Life Sci Aug. 2009 vol. 52 no. 8 780-788