Facilitating visuospatial attention for the contralateral hemifield by repetitive TMS on the posterior parietal cortex

Similar documents
Event-related TMS over the right posterior parietal cortex induces ipsilateral visuospatial

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rtms), unlike

Dominant Limb Motor Impersistence Associated with Anterior Callosal Disconnection

Naoyuki Takeuchi, MD, PhD 1, Takeo Tada, MD, PhD 2, Masahiko Toshima, MD 3, Yuichiro Matsuo, MD 1 and Katsunori Ikoma, MD, PhD 1 ORIGINAL REPORT

NeuroImage 44 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

HEMIEXTINCTION INDUCED BY TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION OVER THE RIGHT TEMPORO-PARIETAL JUNCTION

Introduction to TMS Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

An exploration of the role of the superior temporal gyrus in visual search and spatial perception using TMS

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Brief communication Contralateral visual search deficits following TMS

Simulating unilateral neglect in normals using prism adaptation: implications for theory

Durham Research Online

The Predictive Nature of Pseudoneglect for Visual Neglect: Evidence from Parietal Theta Burst Stimulation

Motor intentional disorders in right hemisphere stroke

Effects of Sub-Motor-Threshold Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation on. Event-Related Potentials and Motor-Evoked Potentials

Effect of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation(TMS) on Visual Search Task

TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab

Improving neglect by TMS

Effects Of Attention And Perceptual Uncertainty On Cerebellar Activity During Visual Motion Perception

Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology. Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Disorders of Object and Spatial perception. Dr John Maasch Brain Injury Rehabilitation Service Burwood Hospital.

Categorisation of perceptual and premotor neglect patients across different tasks: is there strong evidence for a dichotomy?

Brain and Cognition. Visual field asymmetry in attentional capture. Feng Du *, Richard A. Abrams. abstract. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Magnetic stimulation and movement-related cortical activity for acute stroke with hemiparesis

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Supplemental Data. Brain Oscillatory Substrates. of Visual Short-Term Memory Capacity

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

Two eyes make a pair: facial organization and perceptual learning reduce visual extinction

Hyperexcitability of parietal-motor functional connections in the intact left-hemisphere of patients with neglect

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

Coding of Far and Near Space in Neglect Patients

Line versus representational bisections in unilateral spatial neglect

The Contribution of TMS-EEG Coregistration in the Exploration of the Human Connectome

Practical. Paired-pulse on two brain regions

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization

Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published August 21, 2006

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Restor Neurol Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 March 05.

Pain is more than an unpleasant feeling associated with a somatosensory sensation.

Study of Imagery. Banned by behaviorists

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1

Supplementary figure: Kantak, Sullivan, Fisher, Knowlton and Winstein

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping

Can brain stimulation help with relearning movement after stroke?

Left unilateral neglect or right hyperattention?

A Return to the Gorilla What Effects What

Rapid Review. Non-invasive brain stimulation: a new strategy to improve neurorehabilitation after stroke?

Neurophysiological Basis of TMS Workshop

Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex. 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations. 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage

Applications of neuromodulation to explore vestibular cortical. processing; new insights into the effects of direct current cortical

Space amputation in neglect?

Neurosoft TMS. Transcranial Magnetic Stimulator DIAGNOSTICS REHABILITATION TREATMENT STIMULATION. of motor disorders after the stroke

Repetition-priming effect: a cognitive task for the definition of a clinical assessment

Water immersion modulates sensory and motor cortical excitability

Report. Brain Plasticity in the Adult: Modulation of Function in Amblyopia with rtms

Neuro-MS/D DIAGNOSTICS REHABILITATION TREATMENT STIMULATION. Transcranial Magnetic Stimulator. of motor disorders after the stroke

Efficacy of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation/ transcranial direct current stimulation in cognitive neurorehabilitation

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg

Making the blindsighted see

Effects of monocular viewing and eye dominance on spatial attention

Contribution of the human superior parietal lobule to spatial selection process: an MEG study

Activity of Daily Living and Motor Evoked Potentials in the Subacute Stroke Patients Kil Byung Lim, MD, Jeong-Ah Kim, MD

Unilateral Spatial Neglect USN

Cédric Lenoir, Maxime Algoet, Camille Vanderclausen, André Peeters, Susana Ferrao Santos, André Mouraux

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

TREATMENT-SPECIFIC ABNORMAL SYNAPTIC PLASTICITY IN EARLY PARKINSON S DISEASE

Parietal rtms distorts the mental number line: Simulating spatial neglect in healthy subjects

The origins of localization

CENTRIPETAL VERSUS CENTRIFUGAL BIAS IN VISUAL LINE BISECTION: FOCUSING ATTENTION ON TWO HYPOTHESES

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition

Gross Organization I The Brain. Reading: BCP Chapter 7

Enhanced visual perception near the hands

Association Cortex, Asymmetries, and Cortical Localization of Affective and Cognitive Functions. Michael E. Goldberg, M.D.

Directional and Spatial Motor Intentional Disorders in Patients With Right Versus Left Hemisphere Strokes

Supplemental Information

B rain damaged patients may report only the stimulus

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems

Magnetic and Electrical Stimulation in the Treatment of Aphasia

Dissociating the Role of Prefrontal and Premotor Cortices in Controlling Inhibitory Mechanisms during Motor. preparation.

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544

Priming Stimulation Enhances the Depressant Effect of Low- Frequency Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Functional neuroplasticity after stroke: clinical implications and future directions

Correlation between changes of contralesional cortical activity and motor function recovery in patients with hemiparetic stroke

Localization of visually evoked cortical activity in humans.

Statement on Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation for Depression. Position statement CERT03/17

Ocular search during line bisection The effects of hemi-neglect and hemianopia

Neuroscience Tutorial

Neuroimaging biomarkers and predictors of motor recovery: implications for PTs

A UNIFIED PERSPECTIVE OF UNILATERAL SPATIAL NEGLECT

MagPro by MagVenture. Versatility in Magnetic Stimulation

Integrating Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation in Physiotherapy for Patients with Stroke

1. Introduction. Keywords: VOR, pursuit, interhemispheric, brain stimulation, vestibular. Received 14 February 2014 Accepted 2 July 2014

Visual activity in the human frontal eye eld

Advances in Clinical Neuroimaging

Speeded right-to-left information transfer: the result of speeded transmission in right-hemisphere axons?

Emotional Faces Modulate Spatial Neglect: Evidence from Line Bisection

Transcription:

Neuroscience Letters 382 (2005) 280 285 Facilitating visuospatial attention for the contralateral hemifield by repetitive TMS on the posterior parietal cortex Yun-Hee Kim a,b,, Soo-Jung Min b,1, Myoung-Hwan Ko c,2, Ji-Won Park d,3, Sung Ho Jang e,4, Peter K.W. Lee a,5 a Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Stroke and Cerebrovascular Center, Samsung Medical Center, Sungkyunkwan University School of Medicine, 50Ilwon-dong, Kangnam-ku, Seoul 135-710, Republic of Korea b Center for Clinical Medicine, Samsung Medical Center, 50Ilwon-dong, Kangnam-ku, Seoul 135-710, Republic of Korea c Department of Rehabilitation Medicine Chonbuk National University Medical School, 634-18 Keumam-dong, Dukjin-ku, Jeonju, Jeonbuk 561-712, Republic of Korea d Department of Physical Therapy, Graduate School of Public Health, Eulji University, 143-5 Yongdu-dong, Jung-gu, Daejeon 301-832, Republic of Korea e Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, School of Medicine, Yeungnam University, 317-1 Daemyngdong, Namku, Taegu 705-717, Republic of Korea Received 13 December 2004; received in revised form 15 March 2005; accepted 16 March 2005 Abstract Previous studies have demonstrated that repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rtms) could modulate the visuospatial functions. In this study, we investigated the effect of off-line high frequency subthreshold rtms, when applied over the right or left posterior parietal cortex (PPC), on the visuospatial attention of the bilateral hemispaces. The subjects underwent visuospatial tasks before and immediately after receiving 1000 pulses of 10 Hz rtms for a period of 20 min, and their responses were recorded. Our results demonstrated that the high frequency rtms applied over the PPC produced facilitative effects on the visuospatial attention to the contraleteral hemispace. The inhibitory effect to the ipsilateral hemispace was noticeable only in the left PPC. 2005 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation; Visuospatial attention; Posterior parietal cortex Hemi-spatial neglect refers the failure to attend to one side of one s own space. The significance of this neglect as a major source of long-term disability after such brain disease as stroke justifies further research efforts for developing an appropriate therapeutic intervention for this complex and multi- Corresponding author. Tel.: +82 2 3410 2824; fax: +82 2 3410 0388. E-mail addresses: yunkim@smc.samsung.co.kr, yunkim1225@empal.com (Y.-H. Kim), soojung.min@samsung.com (S.- J. Min), mhko@chonbuk.ac.kr (M.-H. Ko), mylovept@eulji.ac.kr (J.- W. Park), belado@med.yu.ac.kr (S.H. Jang), kwlee@smc.samsung.co.kr (P.K.W. Lee). 1 Tel.: +82 2 3410 3762; fax: +82 2 3410 0388. 2 Tel.: +82 63 250 1795. 3 Tel.: +82 42 259 1771. 4 Tel.: +82 53 620 3269; fax: +82 53 625 3508. 5 Tel.: +82 2 3410 2810/2818; fax: +82 2 3410 0388. factorial syndrome. Previous studies have demonstrated that repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rtms) could modulate the visuospatial functions; however, the methods of application and the effects seen in these studies were diverse. Synchronous application of high frequency (25 Hz) rtms applied over the right posterior parietal cortex (PPC) has been reported to induce a perceptual bias on the contralateral hemifield and it produced a transitory contralateral neglect in healthy subjects [6,7,14]. For patients suffering from hemispatial neglect as a consequence of brain damage, the high frequency or 1 Hz rtms ameliorated contralateral visuospatial neglect when they were applied to the unaffected parietal cortex [4,10,13]. These findings were interpreted in the light of the right hemispheric dominance in the visuospatial attention model and the interhemispheric competition model [12,13]. The methods and parameters of applying 0304-3940/$ see front matter 2005 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2005.03.043

Y.-H. Kim et al. / Neuroscience Letters 382 (2005) 280 285 281 rtms need to be considered in order to obtain the desired effect. High frequency rtms (5 25 Hz) has been shown to increase cortical excitability transiently beyond the period of train of stimulation [16]. Therefore, it can be postulated that the high frequency rtms can modulate visuospatial attention, which is influenced by the increased excitability of the focal cortical area if it is applied in an appropriate manner. To the best of our knowledge, there has been no report on the effect of off-line high frequency rtms on the visuospatial functions. In this study, we aimed to assess the effect of high frequency rtms delivered over the right or left PPC on the visuospatial perception of the contralateral and ipsilateral hemispace by using the computerized line bisection task in normal healthy subjects. Twenty healthy right-handed subjects (5 males and 15 females; the mean age was 22.9 years) having normal or corrected-to-normal vision participated in our study. We obtained an informed consent from all the subjects for their participation in the experiment, and the study was approved by the Institutional Research Board at Samsung Medical Center. The exclusion criteria used for the selection of subjects conformed to the current guidelines for rtms research [18]. The subjects were informed that they could end the experiment at any moment if they found the stimulation to be uncomfortable. The line bisection task was designed as follows: all the stimuli were presented on a 15-in. monitor driven by a Pentium-3 PC programmed with SuperLabPro 2.0 software. The stimulus consisted of a black, horizontal transected or bisected line on a white background. The horizontal lines were of five different lengths ranging from 36 to 40. A short vertical line (2.2 long) transected the horizontal lines. All the lines were 0.1 thick. When the line was asymmetrical about the transaction, the elongated line segment was 1 longer than the shorter line segment. The stimuli were always presented with the transection mark in front of the subjects head and body midline. A central fixation cross appeared for 1000 ms, and this was followed by the appearance of the stimulus for 180 ms (to limit the scanning eye movement); this was immediately followed by the mask for 1000 ms. The mask consisted of a thick horizontal line that was thicker than the horizontal line of the stimulus, and also a vertical line that had the same width as the transection mark. The mask was symmetrical around the head midline, and it covered the entire area of the previously displayed stimulus and extended to the edges of the screen (Fig. 1A). The subjects were comfortably seated 47 cm away from the screen. All the subjects used their right index and middle fingers to make their responses to the stimulus by using a mouse. In half of the task blocks, the subject was asked to press the left or right mouse button according to whether the left or right side of the line appeared longer. In the remaining half of the task blocks, the subject pressed the mouse button that corresponded to the shorter side of the line. The subjects were asked to respond as quickly as they could, but they were Fig. 1. (A) Example of one trial for the line bisection task. A central fixation was presented for 1000 ms and then the stimulus (left-elongated, rightelongated or bisected lines) appeared for 180 ms, and this was followed by the mask stimulus for 1000 ms. (B) Location of rtms site in the left and right parietal lobe. The anatomical site of stimulation is indicated by the red dots and white arrows in the axial (left), the sagittal (middle), and the coronal (right) sections of T1-weighted MRI. instructed not to sacrifice their response accuracy for the sake of speed. An experimental session consisted of four blocks of a task and each block contained 40 trials including 10 bisected lines, 15 left-elongated lines and 15 right-elongated lines. The subjects were informed that each side of the line was elongated in 50% of the trials; therefore, they were unaware of the presentation of an exactly bisected line in 25% of the trials. The stimuli were presented in a random order within each block and the four blocks were counter-balanced. The subjects performed a session having a total of 160 trials (four blocks) over a period of 7 min as a baseline assessment, and then 1000 trains of 10 Hz rtms were applied for a period of 20 min as is described below. Immediately after the rtms, the subjects again performed a session of 160 trials. The experiment session for one subject lasted a total of 44 min. The resting motor threshold (RMT) was determined on the left first dorsal interosseus muscle using a Medelec Synergy system (Medelec, UK). TMS was performed using a 70 mm figure-of-eight coil by moving the coil in 1 cm steps around the presumed hand motor area of the right hemisphere to determine the optimal position for activation. The RMT intensity was approached from the supra-threshold levels by reducing the stimulus intensity initially in 5% steps, and then in 1% steps. The RMT was defined as the first stimulus intensity that failed to produce a MEP in three of six subsequent trials. The rtms stimulation site was individually defined for each subject within the 10 20 electroencephalogram (EEG) coordinate system, and it corresponded to the position P4 that

282 Y.-H. Kim et al. / Neuroscience Letters 382 (2005) 280 285 was localized over the right posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and to the position P3 that was localized over the left PPC. Magnetic resonance images were taken on two subjects before the experiment, and the anatomical site of stimulation was confirmed as pointing to the posterior parietal lobe by using optical tracking via a minibird tracker (NexGen Ergonomics Inc., USA) (Fig. 1B). RTMS was delivered on the scalp in accordance with the generally established safety recommendations by using a Magstim Rapid stimulator with two Booster Modules (Magstim Co. Ltd., UK). The 70 mm figure-of-eight coil was held tangentially to the skull with the handle pointing about 45 posterolaterally in order to stimulate the parietal cortex. Fifty pulses of 10 Hz rtms were applied over a period of 5 s. with an RMT of 80% and an intertrain interval of 55 s. A total of 1000 pulses of stimulation were given during 20 min. Sham stimulation was performed using the one-wing sham method at an angle of 90 from the tangential plane to the scalp at the Pz area. Fifteen out of 20 subjects participated in the experiment two times and five subjects participated three times with an interval of 1 week between each experimental period, and the P3, P4 or sham stimulations were administered in a pseudorandomized order. For each of the stimulation conditions, data was obtained from 15 persons. The rates of correct or incorrect responses to the left- or rightelongated lines were obtained and these were used to define the accuracy and error rate for the transected lines. The correct response for the left (or right)-elongated line included the responses of both left (or right)-longer and right (or left)-shorter. The response rates of the left- or right-longer were obtained for the bisected line as well. Response bias was calculated following the methods previously described [5]. Three-way repeated measures ANOVA was conducted using the side of the response (left and right) and the sessions (baseline and post-rtms) as the within subject factors, and the type of stimulation (P3, P4 and sham) as the between subject factor. Differences were regarded as significant when the P values were less than 0.05. When comparing the performances of the baseline sessions, there was no significant difference between the three rtms conditions, the P3, P4, and the sham stimulation, in the rates of accuracy or the error responses for the transected lines, and also for the response rates for the bisected lines. Overall, the subjects responded more accurately for the left-elongated lines than the right-elongated lines (P = 0.002) during the baseline session. The response rate for the leftlonger was significant higher than for the right-longer response to bisected lines (P < 0.001). These phenomena were consistent with the findings on so-called pseudo-neglect that have been demonstrated in normal persons [2,3]. After the application of rtms over the left posterior parietal region (P3), the accuracy for detection of the rightelongated lines was increased (P = 0.013) and the error rate was decreased (P = 0.050). However, the error rate for the leftelongated lines was increased (P = 0.046). For the bisected lines, the number of choices of the left-longer decreased (P = 0.019) and the number of choices of the right-longer increased (P = 0.030) after rtms application (Table 1 and Fig. 2). After the application of rtms over the right posterior parietal region (P4), the accuracy for the left-elongated lines increased (P = 0.005) and the error rate decreased (P = 0.013) as compared with the baseline session. The accuracy and error rate for the right-elongated lines showed no significant changes. For the bisected line, the subjects number of choices of the left-longer increased (P = 0.028) and subjects number of choices of the right-longer decreased (P = 0.045) after rtms application (Table 1 and Fig. 2). There were no significant changes in the accuracy and the error rate for the transected lines, or in the response rate for the bisected lines after sham stimulation. The three-way repeated measures ANOVA revealed the main effect of the side (left-longer and right-longer) on the accuracy [F(1, 42) = 11.789, P = 0.001] and the error rate [F(1, 42) = 11.714, P = 0.001] of the transected lines, and main effect of the side (left-longer and right-longer) on the response rate of the bisected line [F(1, 42) = 17.697, P = 0.000]. In addition, there was an interaction between the type of stimulation (P3, P4 and sham), the session (baseline and post-rtms), and the side on the accuracy [F(2, 42) = 8.026, P = 0.001] and the error rate [F(2, 42) = 6.862, P = 0.003] of the transected lines and on the response rate of the bisected lines [F(2, Table 1 Accuracy and error rate for the transected lines and the response rate for the bisected lines in the three stimulation conditions rtms Session Accuracy (%) Error rate (%) Response of bisected line (%) Left-elongated Right-elongated Left-elongated Right-elongated Left-longer Right-longer Lt. parietal Baseline 71.78 ± 16.04 58.89 ± 24.06 22.22 ± 10.83 34.00 ± 18.62 57.0 ± 11.19 37.0 ± 13.20 Post-rTMS 69.56 ± 16.38 66.67 ± 20.88 * 26.11 ± 12.95 * 28.67 ± 17.07 * 50.5 ± 12.82 * 44.3 ± 13.21 * Rt. parietal Baseline 72.78 ± 16.14 65.33 ± 18.68 23.89 ± 13.33 32.00 ± 16.72 54.7 ± 12.02 42.5 ± 11.92 Post-rTMS 80.11 ± 11.71 ** 64.67 ± 16.38 18.11 ± 10.44 * 33.56 ± 15.69 61.8 ± 14.98 * 35.3 ± 16.03 * Sham Baseline 72.22 ± 16.14 63.45 ± 17.59 25.22 ± 10.25 33.33 ± 16.71 54.7 ± 12.78 41.0 ± 12.67 Post-rTMS 70.44 ± 16.79 59.78 ± 22.15 26.44 ± 14.92 37.44 ± 19.39 53.8 ± 16.31 52.5 ± 14.94 Values are mean ± S.D. p 0.05; comparison between the baseline and post-rtms sessions. p < 0.01; comparison between the baseline and post-rtms sessions.

Y.-H. Kim et al. / Neuroscience Letters 382 (2005) 280 285 283 Fig. 2. Effects of rtms on the subjects responses in the three stimulation conditions (left parietal cortex, P3 rtms; right parietal cortex, P4 rtms; and sham stimulation). Three-way repeated measures ANOVA revealed that there was a significant interaction between the sides, the sessions and the type of stimulation on the accuracy for correctly detecting the transected lines (A) and on the error rate (B). There was also a significant interaction between the sides, the sessions and the type of stimulation on the response rate for the bisected lines (C). 42) = 6.217, P = 0.004] (Fig. 2). The response bias showed no significant difference between the three conditions or the sessions [5]. Our results demonstrated that the high frequency rtms applied over the right or left PPC produced the hemispatial modulation of the visuospatial attention. The effect on the visuospatial function of the contraleteral hemispace was facilitative for both sides; stimulation of the right PPC increased visuospatial attention to the left hemispace and it also increased the leftward bias in healthy subjects. Stimulation of the left PPC increased the attention to the right hemispace and it reduced the leftward bias. The inhibitory effect to the ipsilateral hemispace was noticeable only for the left PPC. There was no detectable adverse effect of rtms in all the subjects. We would like to discuss two issues that are in line with previous researches to help interpreting our results; one is the effect of high frequency rtms on cortical function and the other is the asymmetry of the two hemispheres and their interaction on the visuospatial attention. In the previous studies, the synchronous application of high frequency rtms (25 Hz) produced inhibitory effects on the processing of visuospatial stimuli and it also induced contralateral neglect [5 7]. Bjoertomt et al. have reported that stimulation of the right PPC, when delivered 150 ms after a visual stimulus, produced a relative transitory rightward bias in the perceived midpoint of the bisected lines [3]. In contrast, we implemented the visuospatial task after applying high frequency rtms. This off-line application may have produced a residual facilitative effect on the cortical excitation, and then this resulted in improvement of the visuospatial function for the contralateral hemispace. The effect of high frequency rtms for increasing the cortical excitability and the size of the MEP was well demonstrated by previous studies when it was applied on the motor cortex [1,17]. This facilitative

284 Y.-H. Kim et al. / Neuroscience Letters 382 (2005) 280 285 effect of off-line high frequency rtms on the visuospatial function, however, has not been described previously. The temporal relationship between the magnetic stimulation and behavioral task seems to be important in predicting the effect of high frequency stimulation. In our study, the effect of high frequency rtms to the ipsilateral hemifield was different for the bilateral PPCs. Stimulation of the left PPC increased the error rate of the ipsilateral hemispace, however, stimulation of the right PPC did not produce ipsilateral spatial error. These findings can be interpreted in line with the two best-known neural models of spatial attention; inter-hemispheric inhibition and right hemispheric specialization. Experimental studies and models of spatial attention have investigated the representation of external attentional space across the two cortical hemispheres, and these studies and models have suggested the involvement of reciprocal inter-hemispheric inhibition [4,10,15]. According to this model, the inhibition of one side of the parietal cortex may suppress the reciprocal inhibition to the opposite hemisphere, and then this increases the spatial attention for the ipsilateral hemispace. These phenomena were previously well reported on healthy subjects and for patients with brain damage [4,10,13,15]. According to this view, facilitation of one side of the PPC may decrease the spatial attention to the ipsilateral hemispace. However, our results have shown evidence of ipsilateral dysfunction (an increased error rate) not in both hemispheres, but in only the left PPC. Interpretation of these findings is somewhat puzzling; however, including consideration of the right hemispheric specialization model can solve this question. The right hemispheric specialization model is based on the assumption that the left hemisphere directs attention predominantly to the contralateral right side, whereas the right hemisphere directs attention to both hemispaces [12]. This model also postulates that the right hemisphere devotes more neuronal resources to spatial attention, and this has been confirmed in the course of previous clinical observations and functional imaging experiments that used a variety of behavioral tasks [8,9,11,12]. If these ideas are all true, increasing the activity of the right PPC not only increases the reciprocal inhibition to the left PPC, but it also enhances the neural apparatus to both hemispaces; therefore, the effect for the right hemispace can be cancelled out. These findings are congruent with the clinical observation of the infrequent and less severe right-side neglect seen for patients with brain damage [12]. Further studies using different parameters of rtms may give us more information on this issue. Our present study has contributed crucial information concerning the effect of high frequency off-line rtms applied over the parietal cortex on the facilitation of visuospatial attention for the contralateral hemispace. These findings may well help to promote the further methodological trials for studying the effect of high frequency rtms, and the findings may spur therapeutic trials for those patients having unilateral visuospatial neglect following brain injury. Acknowledgements This research was supported by a grant to Dr. Y.-H.K. from the Brain Research Center of the 21st Century Frontier Research Program; this program was funded by the Ministry of Science and Technology of Republic of Korea (Grant No. M103-KV010014 03-K2201 01430). References [1] A. Berardelli, M. Inghilleri, J.C. Rothwell, S. Romeo, A. Curra, F. Gilio, N. Modugno, M. Manfredi, Facilitation of muscle evoked responses after repetitive cortical stimulation in man, Exp. Brain Res. 122 (1998) 79 84. [2] E. Bisiach, R. Ricci, M. Lualdi, M.R. Colombo, Perceptual and response bias in unilateral neglect: two modified versions of the milner landmark task, Brain Cogn. 37 (1998) 369 386. [3] O. Bjoertomt, A. Cowey, V. Walsh, Spatial neglect in near and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation, Brain 125 (2002) 2012 2022. [4] F. Brighina, E. Bisiach, M. Oliveri, A. Piazza, V. La Bua, O. Daniele, B. Fierro, 1 Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the unaffected hemisphere ameliorates contralesional visuospatial neglect in humans, Neurosci. Lett. 336 (2003) 131 133. [5] F. Brighina, E. Bisiach, A. Piazza, M. Oliveri, V. La Bua, O. Daniele, B. Fierro, Perceptual and response bias in visuospatial neglect due to frontal and parietal repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in normal subjects, Neuroreport 13 (2002) 2571 2575. [6] B. Fierro, F. Brighina, M. Oliveri, A. Piazza, V. La Bua, D. Buffa, E. Bisiach, Contralateral neglect induced by right posterior parietal rtms in healthy subjects, Neuroreport 11 (2000) 1519 1521. [7] B. Fierro, F. Brighina, A. Piazza, M. Oliveri, E. Bisiach, Timing of right parietal and frontal cortex activity in visuo-spatial perception: a TMS study in normal individuals, Neuroreport 12 (2001) 2605 2607. [8] G.R. Fink, J.C. Marshall, N.J. Shah, P.H. Weiss, P.W. Halligan, M. Grosse-Ruyken, K. Ziemons, K. Zilles, H.J. Freund, Line bisection judgments implicate right parietal cortex and cerebellum as assessed by fmri, Neurology 54 (2000) 1324 1331. [9] K.M. Heilman, T. Van Den Abell, Right hemisphere dominance for attention: the mechanism underlying hemispheric asymmetries of inattention (neglect), Neurology 30 (1980) 327 330. [10] C.C. Hilgetag, H. Theoret, A. Pascual-Leone, Enhanced visual spatial attention ipsilateral to rtms-induced virtual lesions of human parietal cortex, Nat. Neurosci. 4 (2001) 953 957. [11] Y.H. Kim, D.R. Gitelman, A.C. Nobre, T.B. Parrish, K.S. LaBar, M.M. Mesulam, The large-scale neural network for spatial attention displays multifunctional overlap but differential asymmetry, Neuroimage 9 (1999) 269 277. [12] M.M. Mesulam, A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect, Ann. Neurol. 10 (1981) 309 325. [13] M. Oliveri, E. Bisiach, F. Brighina, A. Piazza, V. La Bua, D. Buffa, B. Fierro, rtms of the unaffected hemisphere transiently reduces contralesional visuospatial hemineglect, Neurology 57 (2001) 1338 1340. [14] M. Oliveri, P.M. Rossini, P. Pasqualetti, R. Traversa, P. Cicinelli, M.G. Palmieri, F. Tomaiuolo, C. Caltagirone, Interhemispheric asymmetries in the perception of unimanual and bimanual cutaneous stimuli. A study using transcranial magnetic stimulation, Brain 122 (Pt. 9) (1999) 1721 1729. [15] M. Oliveri, P.M. Rossini, R. Traversa, P. Cicinelli, M.M. Filippi, P. Pasqualetti, F. Tomaiuolo, C. Caltagirone, Left frontal transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces contralesional extinction in patients with unilateral right brain damage, Brain 122 (Pt. 9) (1999) 1731 1739.

Y.-H. Kim et al. / Neuroscience Letters 382 (2005) 280 285 285 [16] A. Pascual-Leone, F. Tarazona, J. Keenan, J.M. Tormos, R. Hamilton, M.D. Catala, Transcranial magnetic stimulation and neuroplasticity, Neuropsychologia 37 (1999) 207 217. [17] A. Pascual-Leone, J.M. Tormos, J. Keenan, F. Tarazona, C. Canete, M.D. Catala, Study and modulation of human cortical excitability with transcranial magnetic stimulation, J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 15 (1998) 333 343. [18] E.M. Wassermann, Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: report and suggested guidelines from the International Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, June 5 7, 1996, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 108 (1998) 1 16.