Supplemental Information: Adaptation can explain evidence for encoding of probabilistic. information in macaque inferior temporal cortex

Similar documents
Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Ch.20 Dynamic Cue Combination in Distributional Population Code Networks. Ka Yeon Kim Biopsychology

Encoding of stimulus probability in macaque inferior temporal cortex

Neuron, Volume 63 Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in Macaque area V4.

Neural response time integration subserves. perceptual decisions - K-F Wong and X-J Wang s. reduced model

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Neural Coding. Computing and the Brain. How Is Information Coded in Networks of Spiking Neurons?

Reading Neuronal Synchrony with Depressing Synapses

A Neural Model of Context Dependent Decision Making in the Prefrontal Cortex

Deep Neural Networks Rival the Representation of Primate IT Cortex for Core Visual Object Recognition

Precise Spike Timing and Reliability in Neural Encoding of Low-Level Sensory Stimuli and Sequences

Correlation at the neuron and population levels. Correlation at the neuron and population levels. Recording from LGN/V1.

Processing and Plasticity in Rodent Somatosensory Cortex

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System

Visual Categorization: How the Monkey Brain Does It

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15

Spontaneous Cortical Activity Reveals Hallmarks of an Optimal Internal Model of the Environment. Berkes, Orban, Lengyel, Fiser.

Over-representation of speech in older adults originates from early response in higher order auditory cortex

Oscillatory Brain States and Variability in Visual Short-Term Memory

Information processing at single neuron level*

Rolls,E.T. (2016) Cerebral Cortex: Principles of Operation. Oxford University Press.

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings

Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd

Beyond bumps: Spiking networks that store sets of functions

Hebbian Plasticity for Improving Perceptual Decisions

A model of the interaction between mood and memory

Noise in attractor networks in the brain produced by graded firing rate representations

Self-Organization and Segmentation with Laterally Connected Spiking Neurons

The perirhinal cortex and long-term familiarity memory

Encoding of categories by non-category specific neurons in the inferior temporal cortex

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Supplementary material

Temporally asymmetric Hebbian learning and neuronal response variability

Supplementary Figure 1. Recording sites.

Electrophysiological and firing properties of neurons: categorizing soloists and choristers in primary visual cortex

Neural Encoding. Naureen Ghani. February 10, 2018

Key questions about attention

Supplementary Figure 1. Localization of face patches (a) Sagittal slice showing the location of fmri-identified face patches in one monkey targeted

Models of visual neuron function. Quantitative Biology Course Lecture Dan Butts

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Title: Dynamic Population Coding of Category Information in ITC and PFC

The Time Course of Negative Priming

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex

Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons

Synaptic Plasticity and Connectivity Requirements to Produce Stimulus-Pair Specific Responses in Recurrent Networks of Spiking Neurons

Sensory Adaptation within a Bayesian Framework for Perception

Neurobiology of Hearing (Salamanca, 2012) Auditory Cortex (2) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang

Repetition Suppression Accompanying Behavioral Priming. in Macaque Inferotemporal Cortex. David B.T. McMahon* and Carl R. Olson

Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings. Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014

A Model of Dopamine and Uncertainty Using Temporal Difference

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

arxiv: v1 [q-bio.nc] 25 Apr 2017

Neuronal Dynamics: Computational Neuroscience of Single Neurons

Supplemental Material

Structure and Function of the Auditory and Vestibular Systems (Fall 2014) Auditory Cortex (3) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral training.

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Neural correlates of the perception of sound source separation

Biological Information Neuroscience

Spiking Neuron Simulations of the Relationships Among Repetition Suppression, Neural Synchronization, and Behavioral Priming

Commentary on Moran and Desimone's 'spotlight in V4

Same or Different? A Neural Circuit Mechanism of Similarity-Based Pattern Match Decision Making

The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neural Netw. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2007 November 1.

Bursting dynamics in the brain. Jaeseung Jeong, Department of Biosystems, KAIST

Input-speci"c adaptation in complex cells through synaptic depression

How is the stimulus represented in the nervous system?

Lecture overview. What hypothesis to test in the fly? Quantitative data collection Visual physiology conventions ( Methods )

Nonlinear processing in LGN neurons

Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making

Computing with Spikes in Recurrent Neural Networks

In: Connectionist Models in Cognitive Neuroscience, Proc. of the 5th Neural Computation and Psychology Workshop (NCPW'98). Hrsg. von D. Heinke, G. W.

Choice-related activity and correlated noise in subcortical vestibular neurons

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest

An Account of Interference in Associative Memory: Learning the Fan Effect

How does the ventral pathway contribute to spatial attention and the planning of eye movements?

Modeling Individual and Group Behavior in Complex Environments. Modeling Individual and Group Behavior in Complex Environments

Perceptual Decisions between Multiple Directions of Visual Motion

An Auditory-Model-Based Electrical Stimulation Strategy Incorporating Tonal Information for Cochlear Implant

Word Length Processing via Region-to-Region Connectivity

Different inhibitory effects by dopaminergic modulation and global suppression of activity

A general error-based spike-timing dependent learning rule for the Neural Engineering Framework

Time Course and Stimulus Dependence of Repetition-Induced Response Suppression in Inferotemporal Cortex

A computational account for the ontogeny of mirror neurons via Hebbian learning

A Computational Model of Prefrontal Cortex Function

Learning and Adaptive Behavior, Part II

Supporting Online Material for

arxiv: v3 [q-bio.nc] 7 Jul 2017

Physiological and Physical Basis of Functional Brain Imaging 6. EEG/MEG. Kâmil Uludağ, 20. November 2007

Decision-making mechanisms in the brain

Master s Thesis. Presented to. The Faculty of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences. Brandeis University. Department of Psychology

Lecture 1: Neurons. Lecture 2: Coding with spikes. To gain a basic understanding of spike based neural codes

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

Supplementary Materials

A coincidence detector neural network model of selective attention

dacc and the adaptive regulation of reinforcement learning parameters: neurophysiology, computational model and some robotic implementations

Basics of Computational Neuroscience: Neurons and Synapses to Networks

Transcription:

Supplemental Information: Adaptation can explain evidence for encoding of probabilistic information in macaque inferior temporal cortex Kasper Vinken and Rufin Vogels Supplemental Experimental Procedures We simulated responses as a Poisson process of which the rate for a particular stimulus presentation r t (i.e. a cue or choice stimulus) is determined by a simple model incorporating firing rate dependent fatigue, firing rate dependent recovery, and stimulus-specific adaptation. Specifically, r t is a combination of a fixed rate for the baseline r base and the unadapted rate associated with the cue stimulus r stim (referring to either a low noise face r face, a low noise fruit r fruit, or a high noise cue response r noise ), which can be suppressed according to a stimulus-specific adaptation variable A t that captures the state of adaptation for that stimulus at that time (0 = complete suppression, 1 = no adaptation). In addition, a fatigue variable F t captures the state of neural fatigue at a particular trial. Note that we also enforced a stronger response to choice stimuli by multiplying r stim by a factor of 1.2. = +. A t is determined by stimulus-specific resource variables R t face parameter δ. For example, in case of a face stimulus: or R t fruit and a lower bound = + 1

Both R face fruit t and R t start at a value of 1 and are updated after every stimulus presentation following a simple resource decay model [S1]. That is, after a presentation of a low noise face cue R face decreases and R fruit increases (or vice versa after the presentation of a fruit cue): = = + 1 Similarly, R face t and R fruit t are updated after the presentation of a choice stimulus. Since there were no stimulus specific effects for high noise cue stimuli in the data reported by Bell et al., we considered them as neither face, nor fruit and let both R face and R fruit increase. The interstimulus interval between a choice stimulus and the next cue (mean = 1350 ms, not counting time to initiate a new trial) is longer than that between the cue and choice within a trial (mean = 350 ms, not counting response time). We account for this difference by attenuating the decay of the resource variable after a choice stimulus by a factor of 3. For example, for a face choice that would be: = 1 1 3 To update the fatigue variable F after every stimulus presentation t, we used the net rate for that stimulus, normalized by the maximum unadapted net rate for the face and fruit choice stimuli (r fruit and r face multiplied by a factor of 1.2): = max,. F is then updated with the fatigue parameter α and recovery parameter β:

= + 1+. The value of F is constrained between 0 and 1. If the net firing rate to the previous choice stimulus decreases, the amount of recovery will eventually become higher than the amount of response fatigue (if it is 0, F will increase with β). We accounted for the difference in interstimulus interval mentioned earlier by attenuating the amount of recovery after a cue stimulus by a factor of 3 (i.e. using β/3). Populations of neurons were simulated by generating firing rate values r base, r face, r fruit, and r noise from lognormal distributions. The average baseline firing rate was 10 Hz (SD = 5) and the average net response was 15 Hz (SD = 15) to faces, 6 Hz (SD = 6) to fruit, and.5 Hz (SD =.5) to high noise stimuli. This produced for simulation C response levels similar to those shown in Figure 3A of [S2]. Fatigue and recovery parameters α (mean =.12, SD =.15) and β (mean =.08, SD =.1) and stimulus specific adaptation parameters γ (mean γ =.5, SD =.1) and δ (mean δ =.5, SD =.1) were generated using beta distributions. The experimental design was as described by [S2], except that a block had a fixed number of 50 trials (the mean block length in [S2]). For each simulated neuron, each of the five block-types (0%, 25%, 50%, 75%, 100% faces) was repeated three times in random order, resulting in a total of 750 trials. Behavioral responses were randomly generated to approximate the mean behavioral performances per p(face) as reported in Figure 1B of [S2]. Supplemental Results Contributions of cue and choice stimuli

To examine whether the effect is driven by the probability of a face cue or that of a face choice, one could use the following regression model (C. Summerfield, personal communication): = + + + + h + h + h +, where p(face cue ) is identical to the p(face) of Model 1, and p(face choice ) is based on the history of choices instead of cues. For our simulated data, the effect for the cue is clearly much stronger than for the choice (β 2 = -0.05, versus β 3 = -0.01). This could lead one to conclude that it is indeed the expectation of a face cue that drives the effect (which is false because we did not simulate expectation). Decoding forthcoming cue identity Bell et al. [S2] could decode the upcoming cue from baseline activity on trials were expectation and the stimulus were congruent: faces in high p(face) blocks versus fruits in low p(face) blocks. While they argued that neural expectation signals carried information about its likely identity, it cannot be excluded that these decoding results arose from temporally correlated slow fluctuations in baseline activity, which were unlikely to be equally spread across high p(face) and low p(face) blocks within a session. We confirmed this possibility by first arbitrarily dividing real monkey IT spiking data in blocks, and then decoding block membership from baseline activity, leading to an above chance accuracy similar in magnitude to theirs (data not shown). Supplemental References S1. Mill, R. (2014). Stimulus-Specific Adaptation, Models. In, Encyclopedia of

Computational Neuroscience. D. Jaeger, and R. Jung, ed. (New York: Springer New York), pp. 1 7. S2. Bell, A.H., Summerfield, C., Morin, E.L., Malecek, N.J., and Ungerleider, L.G. (2016). Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex. Curr. Biol. 26, 2280 2290.