PER3 Polymorphism Predicts Sleep Structure and Waking Performance

Similar documents
Association between specific diurnal preference questionnaire items and PER3

Circadian variation of EEG power spectra in NREM and REM sleep in humans: Dissociation from body temperature

Circadian period and the timing of melatonin onset in men and women: predictors of sleep during the weekend and in the laboratory

Homeostatic and Circadian Regulation of the Sleep-Wake Cycle

Circadian Period and the Melatonin Rhythm in Men and Women: Predictors of Sleep during the Weekend and in the Laboratory

Interindividual differences in circadian rhythmicity and sleep homeostasis in older people: effect of a PER3 polymorphism

Chronotype and Sex Effects on Sleep Architecture and Quantitative Sleep EEG in Healthy Young Adults

Effects of light on human circadian rhythms, sleep and cognition

Original Article Chronotype and a PERIOD3 variable number tandem repeat polymorphism in Han Chinese pilots

Interindividual differences in the dynamics of the homeostatic process are trait-like and distinct for sleep versus wakefulness

Deletion of the Mammalian Circadian Clock Gene BMAL1/Mop3 Alters Baseline Sleep Architecture and the Response to Sleep Deprivation

The Use of Bright Light in the Treatment of Insomnia

BIO333 Comparative Physiology and Pharmacology of Sleep. Genetics of Sleep December 3, Raphaelle Winsky-Sommerer, PhD, PD

Sleep, Circadian Rhythms, and Psychomotor Vigilance

Ageing and the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep during forced desynchrony of rest, melatonin and temperature rhythms

Light treatment for sleep disorders: consensus report. IV. Sleep phase and duration disturbances.

Glasgow, 1055 Great Western Road, Glasgow G12 0XH, UK

Getting Through to Circadian Oscillators: Why Use Constant Routines?

Index. sleep.theclinics.com. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type.

Circadian and wake-dependent modulation of fastest and slowest reaction times during the psychomotor vigilance task

Original Article Influence of Per3 genotypes on circadian rhythmicity in flight cadets after militarized management

This brief animation illustrates the EEG patterns of the different stages of sleep, including NREM and REM sleep.

Shantha M. W. Rajaratnam 1,3,Benita Middleton 1,Barbara M. Stone 2,Josephine Arendt 1 and Derk-Jan Dijk 1

SLEEP, SLEEP DEPRIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE

Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11:

The Role of Sleep and Arousal in the Development of Obesity"

Biological Clocks. Lu Chen, Ph.D. MCB, UC Berkeley. Why Does Melatonin Now Outsell Vitamin C??

Sleep Homeostasis and Models of Sleep Regulation

Interactions of polymorphisms in different clock genes associated with circadian phenotypes in humans

Understanding Sleep Regulatory Processes to Improve Waking Performance

Novel Insights from Genetic and Molecular Characterization of the Human Clock

Advance in circadian rhythm genetics in mammals

Power Density in Theta/Alpha Frequencies of the Waking EEG Progressively Increases During Sustained Wakefulness

Definition 1: A fixed point iteration scheme to approximate the fixed point, p, of a function g, = for all n 1 given a starting approximation, p.

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle

The winter recurrence of depressive episodes in patients

SLEEP DISORDERS IN HUNTINGTON S DISEASE. Gary L. Dunbar, Ph.D.

Aging and Circadian Disruption: Causes and Effects

The Transcription Factor DBP Affects Circadian Sleep Consolidation and Rhythmic EEG Activity

Overview. Surviving shift work. What is the circadian rhythm? Components of a Generic Biological Timing System 31/10/2017

Circadian rhythm and Sleep. Radwan Banimustafa MD

Biological Clocks. Lu Chen, Ph.D. MCB, UC Berkeley. What is biological clock?

Sex differences in the circadian regulation of sleep and waking cognition in humans

Overview of the Biology of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

A Novel Approach to Eliminating Jetlag Using Natural Ingredients

Circadian photoreception in humans: More than meets the eye

Phenotypic effects of genetic variability in human clock genes on circadian and sleep parameters

Effects of light exposure and sleep displacement on dim light melatonin onset Gordijn, Margaretha; Beersma, DGM; Korte, HJ; Van den Hoofdakker, RH

Daylight Saving Time (DST) is used worldwide and affects millions of people annually. In the

Sleep, Rhythms and Performance

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Scheduling of sleep/darkness affects the circadian phase of night shift workers

Modules 7. Consciousness and Attention. sleep/hypnosis 1

Diurnal variations in the waking EEG: comparisons with sleep latencies and subjective alertness

Thomas W. O Reilly, MS, PCC in cooperation with Lakeshore Educational and Counseling Services

Piecewise smooth maps for the circadian modulation of sleep-wake dynamics

Dr Alex Bartle. Medical Director Sleep Well Clinic Christchurch

What is Consciousness?

Sleep Timing and Circadian Phase in Delayed Sleep Phase Syndrome

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm

University of Groningen. Circadian control of the sleep-wake cycle Beersma, Domien G. M.; Gordijn, Margaretha. Published in: Physiology & Behavior

Daytime Exposure to Bright Light, as Compared to Dim Light, Decreases Sleepiness and Improves Psychomotor Vigilance Performance

Modelling the relation of body temperature and sleep: importance of the circadian rhythm in skin temperature

Sleep Biology: Circadian Rhythms and Mother Nature

Defining and determining the properties of the human sleep homeostat Zavada, Andrei

Quantitative measurements of sleepiness

Recent Advances in Understanding Sleep and Sleep Disturbances in Older Adults Growing Older Does Not Mean Sleeping Poorly

Polymorphisms in the circadian expressed genes PER3 and ARNTL2 are associated with diurnal preference and GNb3 with sleep measures

Chronobiologic Aspects of Heart Rate Variability

Manuscript EEG power spectra

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Circadian-Based New Technologies for Night Workers

Sleep and Body Temperature in "Morning" and "Evening" People

Theta waves in children s waking electroencephalogram resemble local aspects of sleep during wakefulness

Short-term homeostasis of REM sleep assessed in an intermittent REM sleep deprivation protocol in the rat

LESSON 4.5 WORKBOOK How do circuits regulate their output?

Fatigue and Circadian Rhythms

EEG Electrode Placement

September 26 28, 2013 Westin Tampa Harbour Island. Co-sponsored by

Acute exposure to evening blue-enriched light impacts on human sleep

Facts about Sleep. Circadian rhythms are important in determining human sleep patterns/ sleep-waking cycle

Chronobiology and Sleep. Prolonged Interval From Body Temperature Nadir to Sleep Offset in Patients With Delayed Sleep Phase Syndrome

Age-Related Changes in the Circadian Modulation of Sleep-Spindle Frequency During Nap Sleep

EEG Sleep Circadian rhythms Learning Objectives: 121, 122

Daily circadian misalignment impairs human cognitive performance task-dependently

CHAPTER 7. Can light make us bright? Effects of light on cognition sleep

Artificial organisms that sleep

Exclusion criteria and outlier detection

Make sure you remember the Key Concepts

Circadian and ultradian NREM-REM sleep modulation of Dream Recall: Effects of age and spectral activity

Biological Rhythms, Sleep, and Dreaming. Elaine M. Hull

Sleep-Wake Cycle I Brain Rhythms. Reading: BCP Chapter 19

Chapter Five. Sleep McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved.

Sleep and Melatonin in Diurnal Species

March 14. Table of Contents: 91. March 14 & Unit 5 Graphic Organizer part Dream Journal Assignment

Teenagers: Sleep Patterns and School Performance

Dr Alex Bartle. Director Sleep Well Clinic

Relationship of chronotype to sleep pattern in a cohort of college students during work days and vacation days

Transcription:

Current Biology 17, 613 618, April 3, 2007 ª2007 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved DOI 10.1016/j.cub.2007.01.073 PER3 Polymorphism Predicts Sleep Structure and Waking Performance Report Antoine U. Viola, 1 Simon N. Archer, 1,2 Lynette M. James, 1 John A. Groeger, 3 June C.Y. Lo, 3 Debra J. Skene, 2 Malcolm von Schantz, 1,2 and Derk-Jan Dijk 1,2, * 1 Surrey Sleep Research Centre 2 Centre for Chronobiology School of Biomedical and Molecular Sciences 3 Department of Psychology School of Human Sciences University of Surrey Guildford GU2 7XH United Kingdom Summary Circadian rhythmicity and sleep homeostasis interact to regulate sleep-wake cycles [1 4], but the genetic basis of individual differences in sleep-wake regulation remains largely unknown [5]. PERIOD genes are thought to contribute to individual differences in sleep timing by affecting circadian rhythmicity [6], but not sleep homeostasis [7, 8]. We quantified the contribution of a variable-number tandem-repeat polymorphism in the coding region of the circadian clock gene PERIOD3 (PER3) [9, 10] to sleep-wake regulation in a prospective study, in which 24 healthy participants were selected only on the basis of their PER3 genotype. Homozygosity for the longer allele (PER3 5/5 ) had a considerable effect on sleep structure, including several markers of sleep homeostasis: slow-wave sleep (SWS) and electroencephalogram (EEG) slowwave activity in non-rapid eye movement (non-rem) sleep and theta and alpha activity during wakefulness and REM sleep were all increased in PER3 5/5 compared to PER3 4/4 individuals. In addition, the decrement of cognitive performance in response to sleep loss was significantly greater in the PER3 5/5 individuals. By contrast, the circadian rhythms of melatonin, cortisol, and peripheral PER3 mrna expression were not affected. The data show that this polymorphism in PER3 predicts individual differences in the sleeploss-induced decrement in performance and that this differential susceptibility may be mediated by its effects on sleep homeostasis. Results and Discussion The coding region of the PERIOD3 (PER3) gene contains a variable-number tandem-repeat (VNTR) polymorphism (insertion of nucleotides 3031 3084) in which a motif encoding 18 amino acids is repeated either four (PER3 4 ) or five times (PER3 5 ) [9]. These repeat units contain a cluster of putative phosphorylation motifs [10]. Retrospective studies have shown that this *Correspondence: d.j.dijk@surrey.ac.uk polymorphism is associated with diurnal preference and delayed-sleep-phase syndrome (DSPS) [10 12]. Here we present the results of a prospective study in which we investigated the functional consequences of this polymorphism for sleep and circadian physiology as well as waking performance. Subjects were selected only on the basis of their genotype. Four hundred and four men and women were genotyped in order to select matched pairs of individuals who were homozygous for the PER3 4 and PER3 5 alleles. These individuals then participated in a field study in which we characterized the timing of their sleep-wake cycle. This field study was followed by an intensive physiological monitoring study, in which circadian rhythms and homeostatic aspects of sleep regulation and performance were quantified under baseline conditions and subsequently during sleep loss. Ten PER3 5/5 (4 women, 6 men; age 6 standard error of the mean = 25.2 6 1.1 yr) and 14 PER3 4/4 (6 women, 8 men; 24.8 6 1.0 yr) healthy participants, closely matched for age, sex, and ethnicity and without sleep complaints, completed both the field and laboratory studies (see Table S1 in the Supplemental Data available online). Assessment of sleep timing by sleep diary and actigraphy during the field study revealed no significant difference in bed time (PER3 5/5, 01:03 6 0:32; PER3 4/4, 01:03 6 0:21; p > 0.05) or wake time (PER3 5/5, 07:57 6 0:28; PER3 4/4, 08:41 6 0:21; p > 0.05) (Figure 1A). No major difference was observed in reported sleep duration (PER3 5/5, 06:55 6 0:19; PER3 4/4, 07:38 6 0:15; p > 0.05) or actigraphically assessed sleep duration (PER3 5/5, 06:51 6 0:14; PER3 4/4, 07:19 6 0:12; p > 0.05). For assessment of the endogenous phase and amplitude of circadian rhythms in the absence of the confounding effects of light-dark and behavioral cycles, participants underwent a constant-routine protocol, in which they were kept awake in dim light (<5 lux) for approximately 40 hr in a semirecumbent posture [13, 14]. During this period, motor-activity levels did not differ between the genotypes and were near stable (Figure 1A), indicating that the constant-routine protocol was successful in eliminating the 24 hr activity cycle, which masks endogenous circadian rhythms. PER3 mrna levels in total RNA extracted from peripheral blood mononuclear cells exhibited a robust endogenous circadian rhythm with a peak value occurring at 06:42 6 01:04 in PER3 5/5 and 06:26 6 0:28 in PER3 4/4 (p > 0.05) (Figure 1B). This circadian rhythm is very similar to a previously reported profile [15]. The amplitude and mean levels of PER3 mrna were also not significantly different between the genotypes. The circadian rhythms of plasma cortisol (Figure 1C) and melatonin (Figure 1D), two hormonal rhythms driven by the circadian pacemaker in the suprachiasmatic nuclei of the hypothalamus, were also remarkably similar in the two genotypes. The average peak value of cortisol occurred at 09:48 6 1:02 in PER3 5/5 and 9:00 6 00:27 in PER3 4/4. No difference was observed in cortisol amplitude. For the melatonin profile, the onset (PER3 5/5, 23:27 6 0:28; PER3 4/4,

Current Biology 614 Figure 1. Endogenous Circadian Rhythms of Hormones and PER3 mrna Levels Do Not Differ between PER3 5/5 and PER3 4/4 Homozygotes (A) Shows the activity during the constant routine. Time courses of (B) PER3 mrna levels in total RNA extracted from blood mononuclear cells, (C) plasma cortisol concentration, and (D) plasma melatonin concentration are shown. All data are averaged across ten PER3 5/5 (open symbols) and 14 PER3 4/4 (filled symbols) participants. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean, and time represents local time. 23:45 6 0:21), offset (PER3 5/5, 08:44 6 0:24; PER3 4/4, 09:18 6 0:29), midpoint (PER3 5/5, 04:06 6 0:26; PER3 4/4, 04:31 6 0:24), and amplitude (PER3 5/5, 71.37 6 9.51 pg/ml; PER3 4/4, 72.78 6 10.82 pg/ml) did not differ between the genotypes (p > 0.05 in all cases). Despite the absence of significant differences in the timing and amplitude of these markers of central and peripheral circadian oscillators, the two genotypes differed strikingly with respect to sleep propensity, electroencephalogram (EEG) during wakefulness and sleep, and waking performance. Whereas during baseline, no differences were observed in rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, stage 1 or 2 sleep, or total sleep time (see Table S2), PER3 5/5 subjects fell asleep more readily than PER3 4/4 subjects (sleep latency: PER3 5/5, 8.6 6 1.3 min; PER3 4/4, 18.1 6 2.6 min; p < 0.005), indicating a greater sleep propensity. Accordingly, these participants also spent more time in slow-wave sleep (SWS) (PER3 5/5, 22.7% 6 1.6% of total sleep time; PER3 4/4, 15.7% 6 1.6% of total sleep time; p = 0.006), a well-established marker of the homeostatic oscillator [16]. SWS is characterized by low-frequency, high-amplitude oscillations in the EEG. Detailed analysis of the spectral composition of the EEG during non-rem sleep (stages 1, 2, 3, and 4), REM sleep, and wakefulness revealed that the effects of the PER3 genotype on SWS were not caused by a general effect on the amplitude of the EEG. Instead, we found that the effects of the PER3 genotype on the EEG were specific to frequency and vigilance state. Whereas during non-rem sleep (Figures 2A and 2B) the largest differences were observed in the slowwave range, during REM sleep (Figures 2C and 2D) and wakefulness (Figures 2E and 2F) the genotypedependent differences were located primarily in the theta and alpha range. It is well established that slowwave activity (SWA) during non-rem sleep [17] and EEG activity in the theta and alpha frequency range during wakefulness [18] and REM sleep [17, 19] track homeostatic sleep pressure and sleepiness. Therefore, the data imply that the PER3 polymorphism affects sleep homeostasis in all three vigilance states. This interpretation is strengthened by an analysis of the effects of the PER3 polymorphism on the time course of EEG activity during sleep. Baseline sleep of PER3 5/5 participants was characterized by higher initial values of SWA followed by a steeper decline (Figure 3A). Alpha activity in REM sleep remained higher in PER3 5/5 throughout the sleep episode (Figure 3D). When the sleep homeostatic system was challenged by sleep deprivation, genotype-dependent differences in non- REM and REM sleep persisted. During recovery sleep, the initial values of SWA during non-rem sleep (Figure 3B) and alpha activity in REM sleep throughout the recovery episode (Figure 3E) were higher in PER3 5/5 (p < 0.05). As indexed by the duration of SWS and REM, the response to sleep loss also differed between the genotypes. In both genotypes, sleep deprivation led to an increase in SWS (Figure 3C), but the associated inhibition of REM sleep in the initial part of recovery sleep was stronger in PER3 5/5 (Figure 3F). As assessed in the constant-routine protocol, the increase in theta activity in the EEG during sustained wakefulness differed noticeably between the genotypes. In PER3 5/5 individuals, it increased substantially with time awake. By contrast, in the PER3 4/4 subjects, no noticeable change was observed over time (Figure 4A). We also monitored the occurrence of slow rolling eye movements (SEMs), an EEG-independent marker of inattention and drowsiness [14]. Over the course of the 40 hr of wakefulness, SEMs increased more rapidly in PER3 5/5 than in PER3 4/4 homozygotes. The largest differences between the genotypes were observed in the morning hours of the second day of sleep deprivation (Figure 4B). The genotype-dependent difference in the response to sleep deprivation during the biological night of these two markers of sleepiness and inattention raises the possibility of a genetically determined variability in susceptibility to the negative effects of sleep deprivation on performance. We assessed various domains of waking performance (including working memory, attention, and psychomotor

PER3 Predicts Sleep and Waking Performance 615 Figure 2. EEG Power Spectra during non- REM Sleep, REM Sleep, and Wakefulness Differ between PER3 5/5 and PER3 4/4 Homozygotes in a Vigilance-State and Frequency- Specific Manner (A) Absolute EEG power-density spectra in non-rem sleep (stages 1 to 4; frontal derivation) during baseline sleep. (B) EEG power spectra in non-rem sleep of PER3 5/5 participants during the first quarter of the baseline sleep episode are expressed relative the corresponding EEG power spectra in PER3 4/4 participants. (C) Absolute EEG power-density spectra in REM sleep (central derivation) during baseline sleep. (D) EEG power spectra in REM sleep of PER3 5/5 participants during baseline sleep are expressed relative to the corresponding EEG power spectra in PER3 4/4 participants. (E) Absolute power spectra of EEG during wakefulness (central derivation) in PER3 5/5 and PER3 4/4 homozygotes. EEG was recorded during dedicated 2 min sessions, scheduled to occur every 2 hr during the constant routine. (F) EEG power spectra during wakefulness in PER3 5/5 participants are expressed relative to the corresponding EEG power spectra in PER3 4/4 participants. In (A), (C), and (E), the error bars face upward for PER3 5/5 and downward for PER3 4/4 participants. All values are plotted at the upper limit of the 1 Hz frequency bands. * indicates a significant difference between genotypes, p < 0.05 (PER3 5/5 [open symbols]: n = 10 [all panels], PER3 4/4 [filled symbols]: n = 13 [A, B, C, and D] or n = 14 [E and F]). performance) during the extended period of wakefulness in a performance test battery consisting of verbal and spatial 1-, 2-, and 3-back tests; a sustained-attention-to-response task; a paced-visual-serial-addition task; a self-paced digit-symbol-substitution test; simple-reaction-time and serial-reaction-time tests; and a motor-tracking task. The performance data were summarized by computation of a composite performance score. Whereas during the first biological day of the constant-routine protocol performance was near stable in both genotypes, the patterns dissociated when wakefulness was extended into the biological night (Figure 4C). Most strikingly, PER3 5/5 homozygotes performed very poorly during the hours after the melatonin midpoint. The decrement in waking performance in the PER3 4/4 homozygotes was far less. These major differences in performance between the two genotypes occurred during the late-night and early-morning hours, a time known from both laboratory and field studies as the nadir of the circadian timing system and during which performance is poorest and sleep propensity at its peak [2]. This is also the time at which many sleepiness-related accidents occur [20] and the greatest impairment is seen in shift-work sleep disorders [21]. The PER3 5-repeat allele, which is the less frequent one in most ethnic groups [22], has been associated with extreme morning preference [10, 11], while the 4-repeat allele has been linked with DSPS in our previous study [10]. The primate-specific expansion of the polymorphic region [23], its association with variation in diurnal preference and DSPS [9 11], its potential impact on phosphorylation of the PER3 protein [10], and its expression in hypothalamic and other brain areas implicated in sleep regulation [24, 25] led us to consider it as a candidate for mediating some of the marked individual differences in sleep-wake regulation. These individual differences include the preferred timing of sleep-wake cycles, the structure of sleep, EEG patterns during sleep and wakefulness, and their response to sleep loss and circadian-phase misalignment [20, 26 29]. Our results indicate that the PER3 polymorphism may contribute to the marked individual differences in performance decrement during sleep loss [29]. The phenotypes that we found to be predicted by homozygosity for the PER3 alleles are also reminiscent of some of the classical phenotypes in human sleep research morning and evening types [30, 31], and short and long sleepers [32]. Sleep in both morning types and PER3 5/5 individuals is characterized by high initial values of SWA, in particular when compared to evening types who sleep at a similar circadian phase [30]. This is in accordance with the previously reported association between this PER3 polymorphism and diurnal preference in subjects selected from a larger population sample [10]. Thus, this PER3 polymorphism appears to contribute to the differences in sleep structure, but not the differences in circadian timing [33], between morning and evening types.

Current Biology 616 Figure 3. The Dynamics of EEG Slow-Wave Activity in non-rem Sleep, EEG Alpha Activity in REM Sleep, and the Sleep-Stage Response to Sleep Loss Differ between PER3 5/5 and PER3 4/4 Homozygotes (A, B, D, and E) Time courses of mean (6 standard error of the mean) slow-wave activity (SWA; 0.75 4.5 Hz) in non-rem sleep (stages 1 4) and alpha activity (8 12 Hz) in REM sleep during the baseline and recovery sleep episodes for ten PER3 5/5 (open symbols) and 13 PER3 4/4 (filled symbols) participants. (* indicates a significant difference between genotypes, p < 0.05.) (C) Slow-wave sleep (SWS) time and (F) REM sleep time expressed as a percentage of total sleep time (6 standard error of the mean) for baseline and recovery sleep in PER3 5/5 (white bar) and PER3 4/4 (black bar) participants. For each participant, sleep-stage percentages during the baseline sleep period were compared to the sleep-stage percentages for the same time period of recovery sleep. (* indicates a significant difference between genotypes or between baseline and recovery sleep, p < 0.05). The sleep of habitual short sleepers is also characterized by high initial values of SWA and high levels of theta activity in the EEG during wakefulness, very similar to those reported here for PER3 5/5 individuals. The effect of the PER3 polymorphism on SWA is of a comparable magnitude to the effects of genetic variation in an already well-known key component of the homeostatic regulation of sleep, the adenosine 2A receptor and adenosine deaminase system [34]. In summary, this prospective study, in which subjects were selected by genotype alone, demonstrates that the PER3 VNTR polymorphism affects key markers of sleep homeostasis, including sleep latency, SWS, theta activity in the waking EEG, and the decrement in waking performance in response to sleep loss. However, no significant effect on objective measures of sleep-timing and circadian-rhythm parameters was observed. Within the framework of the homeostatic and circadian regulation of sleep and performance, these data imply that the PER3 VNTR polymorphism affects the homeostatic aspect of sleep regulation. This is the first time that such an effect has been demonstrated in humans and contrasts the limited animal data available for Per1 and Per2 [7, 8]. However, in animal studies, other clock genes, such as Clock, Cry1 and Cry2, NPAS2, DBP, and BMAL1 have been shown to influence aspects of the sleep and waking EEG as well as the response to sleep loss [35 39], although effects on performance have not previously been reported for any of these genes. The independence of circadian and homeostatic aspects of sleep regulation has been a mainstay of our current models of sleep regulation [1, 40]. Nevertheless, our data collected from humans, together with data from mice, imply that this concept does not extend to the molecular level, and that some genes previously described as clock genes perform noncircadian roles. Conclusions The effects of the PER3 polymorphism on SWS, SWA, and the decrements of waking performance during the biological night, as observed in this study, are significant and substantial. This implies that this polymorphism may be an important marker for individual differences in sleep and susceptibility to sleep loss and circadianphase misalignment, which are major causes of health problems and accidents in our society. Supplemental Data Supplemental Data include Experimental Procedures and two tables and are available with this article online at: http://www.currentbiology.com/cgi/content/full/17/7/613/dc1/. Acknowledgments This research was supported by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC) (BSS/B/08523). We thank the clinical and research team of the Clinical Research Centre and Vanessa Kyriakopoulou, Alexander Garvin, Jayshan Carpen, Matt Cooper, and Dr. Benita Middleton for their help. Received: November 16, 2006 Revised: January 29, 2007 Accepted: January 29, 2007 Published online: March 8, 2007

PER3 Predicts Sleep and Waking Performance 617 Figure 4. Deterioration of Waking Performance and Increase of Theta EEG Activity and Slow Eye Movements during Sleep Deprivation Is Greater in PER3 5/5 Than PER3 4/4 Participants Time course of (A) central EEG theta (5 8 Hz) activity during wakefulness, (B) incidence of slow eye movement (SEMs) (percentage of 30 s epochs containing at least one SEM), and (C) waking performance (composite performance score) are plotted relative to the timing of the plasma melatonin rhythm (D) in ten PER3 5/5 (open symbols) and 14 PER3 4/4 (filled symbols) homozygotes. EEG theta activity, SEMs, and waking performance data were averaged per 2 hr intervals, relative to the midpoint of the melatonin rhythm. (* indicates a significant difference between genotypes, p < 0.05; upper abscissa indicates approximate wake duration.) Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. References 1. Borbely, A.A., Dijk, D.J., Achermann, P., and Tobler, I. (2001). Processes underlying the regulation of the sleep-wake cycle. In Handbook of Behavioural Neurobiology, Volume 12: Circadian Clocks, J.S. Takahashi and R.Y. Moore, eds. (New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers), pp. 458 473. 2. Czeisler, C.A., and Dijk, D.J. (2001). Human circadian physiology and sleep-wake regulation. In Handbook of Behavioural Neurobiology, Volume 12: Circadian Clocks, J.S. Takahashi and R.Y. Moore, eds. (New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers), pp. 531 569. 3. Dijk, D.J., and von Schantz, M. (2005). Timing and consolidation of human sleep, wakefulness, and performance by a symphony of oscillators. J. Biol. Rhythms 20, 279 290. 4. Saper, C.B., Scammell, T.E., and Lu, J. (2005). Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and circadian rhythms. Nature 437, 1257 1263. 5. Tafti, M., Maret, S., and Dauvilliers, Y. (2005). Genes for normal sleep and sleep disorders. Ann. Med. 37, 580 589. 6. Toh, K.L., Jones, C.R., He, Y., Eide, E.J., Hinz, W.A., Virshup, D.M., Ptacek, L.J., and Fu, Y.H. (2001). An hper2 phosphorylation site mutation in familial advanced sleep phase syndrome. Science 291, 1040 1043. 7. Kopp, C., Albrecht, U., Zheng, B., and Tobler, I. (2002). Homeostatic sleep regulation is preserved in mper1 and mper2 mutant mice. Eur. J. Neurosci. 16, 1099 1106. 8. Shiromani, P.J., Xu, M., Winston, E.M., Shiromani, S.N., Gerashchenko, D., and Weaver, D.R. (2004). Sleep rhythmicity and homeostasis in mice with targeted disruption of mperiod genes. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 287, 47 57. 9. Ebisawa, T., Uchiyama, M., Kajimura, N., Mishima, K., Kamei, Y., Katoh, M., Watanabe, T., Sekimoto, M., Shibui, K., Kim, K., et al. (2001). Association of structural polymorphisms in the human period3 gene with delayed sleep phase syndrome. EMBO Rep. 2, 342 346. 10. Archer, S.N., Robilliard, D.L., Skene, D.J., Smits, M., Williams, A., Arendt, J., and von Schantz, M. (2003). A length polymorphism in the circadian clock gene PER3 is linked to delayed sleep phase syndrome and extreme diurnal preference. Sleep 26, 413 415. 11. Pereira, D.S., Tufik, S., Louzada, F.M., Benedito-Silva, A.A., Lopez, A.R., Lemos, N.A., Korczak, A.L., D Almeida, V., and Pedrazzoli, M. (2005). Association of the length polymorphism in the human PER3 gene with the delayed sleep-phase syndrome: Does latitude have an influence upon it? Sleep 28, 29 32. 12. Jones, K.H., Ellis, J., von Schantz, M., Skene, D.J., Dijk, D.J., and Archer, S.N. (2007). Age-related change in the association between a polymorphism in the PER3 gene and preferred timing of sleep and waking activities. J. Sleep Res. 16, 12 16. 13. Duffy, J.F., and Dijk, D.J. (2002). Getting through to circadian oscillators: Why use constant routines? J. Biol. Rhythms 17, 4 13. 14. Cajochen, C., Khalsa, S.B., Wyatt, J.K., Czeisler, C.A., and Dijk, D.J. (1999). EEG and ocular correlates of circadian melatonin phase and human performance decrements during sleep loss. Am. J. Physiol. 277, 640 649. 15. Takimoto, M., Hamada, A., Tomoda, A., Ohdo, S., Ohmura, T., Sakato, H., Kawatani, J., Jodoi, T., Nakagawa, H., Terazono, H., et al. (2005). Daily expression of clock genes in whole blood cells in healthy subjects and a patient with circadian rhythm sleep disorder. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 289, R1273 R1279. 16. Dijk, D.J., and Czeisler, C.A. (1995). Contribution of the circadian pacemaker and the sleep homeostat to sleep propensity, sleep structure, electroencephalographic slow waves, and sleep spindle activity in humans. J. Neurosci. 15, 3526 3538. 17. Dijk, D.J., Shanahan, T.L., Duffy, J.F., Ronda, J.M., and Czeisler, C.A. (1997). Variation of electroencephalographic activity during non-rapid eye movement and rapid eye movement sleep with phase of circadian melatonin rhythm in humans. J. Physiol. 505, 851 858. 18. Cajochen, C., Brunner, D.P., Krauchi, K., Graw, P., and Wirz- Justice, A. (1995). Power density in theta/alpha frequencies of the waking EEG progressively increases during sustained wakefulness. Sleep 18, 890 894. 19. Roth, C., Achermann, P., and Borbely, A.A. (1999). Alpha activity in the human REM sleep EEG: Topography and effect of REM sleep deprivation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110, 632 635. 20. Rajaratnam, S.M., and Arendt, J. (2001). Health in a 24-h society. Lancet 358, 999 1005. 21. Czeisler, C.A., Walsh, J.K., Roth, T., Hughes, R.J., Wright, K.P., Kingsbury, L., Arora, S., Schwartz, J.R., Niebler, G.E., and Dinges, D.F. (2005). Modafinil for excessive sleepiness

Current Biology 618 associated with shift-work sleep disorder. N. Engl. J. Med. 353, 476 486. 22. Nadkarni, N.A., Weale, M.E., von Schantz, M., and Thomas, M.G. (2005). Evolution of a length polymorphism in the human PER3 gene, a component of the circadian system. J. Biol. Rhythms 20, 490 499. 23. Jenkins, A., Archer, S.N., and von Schantz, M. (2005). Expansion during primate radiation of a variable number tandem repeat in the coding region of the circadian clock gene period3. J. Biol. Rhythms 20, 470 472. 24. Takumi, T., Taguchi, K., Miyake, S., Sakakida, Y., Takashima, N., Matsubara, C., Maebayashi, Y., Okumura, K., Takekida, S., Yamamoto, S., et al. (1998). A light-independent oscillatory gene mper3 in mouse SCN and OVLT. EMBO J. 17, 4753 4759. 25. Uschakov, A., Gong, H., McGinty, D., and Szymusiak, R. (2006). Sleep-active neurons in the preoptic area project to the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus and perifornical lateral hypothalamus. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 3284 3296. 26. Van Dongen, H.P., Vitellaro, K.M., and Dinges, D.F. (2005). Individual differences in adult human sleep and wakefulness: Leitmotif for a research agenda. Sleep 28, 479 496. 27. Klerman, E.B., and Dijk, D.J. (2005). Interindividual variation in sleep duration and its association with sleep debt in young adults. Sleep 28, 1253 1259. 28. Leproult, R., Colecchia, E.F., Berardi, A.M., Stickgold, R., Kosslyn, S.M., and Van, C.E. (2003). Individual differences in subjective and objective alertness during sleep deprivation are stable and unrelated. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 284, 280 290. 29. Van Dongen, H.P., Baynard, M.D., Maislin, G., and Dinges, D.F. (2004). Systematic interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: Evidence of trait-like differential vulnerability. Sleep 27, 423 433. 30. Mongrain, V., Carrier, J., and Dumont, M. (2006). Circadian and homeostatic sleep regulation in morningness-eveningness. J. Sleep Res. 15, 162 166. 31. Kerkhof, G.A., and Lancel, M. (1991). EEG slow wave activity, REM sleep, and rectal temperature during night and day sleep in morning-type and evening-type subjects. Psychophysiology 28, 678 688. 32. Aeschbach, D., Postolache, T.T., Sher, L., Matthews, J.R., Jackson, M.A., and Wehr, T.A. (2001). Evidence from the waking electroencephalogram that short sleepers live under higher homeostatic sleep pressure than long sleepers. Neuroscience 102, 493 502. 33. Duffy, J.F., Dijk, D.J., Hall, E.F., and Czeisler, C.A. (1999). Relationship of endogenous circadian melatonin and temperature rhythms to self-reported preference for morning or evening activity in young and older people. J. Investig. Med. 47, 141 150. 34. Retey, J.V., Adam, M., Honegger, E., Khatami, R., Luhmann, U.F., Jung, H.H., Berger, W., and Landolt, H.P. (2005). A functional genetic variation of adenosine deaminase affects the duration and intensity of deep sleep in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102, 15676 15681. 35. Franken, P., Lopez-Molina, L., Marcacci, L., Schibler, U., and Tafti, M. (2000). The transcription factor DBP affects circadian sleep consolidation and rhythmic EEG activity. J. Neurosci. 20, 617 625. 36. Naylor, E., Bergmann, B.M., Krauski, K., Zee, P.C., Takahashi, J.S., Vitaterna, M.H., and Turek, F.W. (2000). The circadian clock mutation alters sleep homeostasis in the mouse. J. Neurosci. 20, 8138 8143. 37. Wisor, J.P., O Hara, B.F., Terao, A., Selby, C.P., Kilduff, T.S., Sancar, A., Edgar, D.M., and Franken, P. (2002). A role for cryptochromes in sleep regulation. BMC Neurosci. 3, 20. 38. Laposky, A., Easton, A., Dugovic, C., Walisser, J., Bradfield, C., and Turek, F. (2005). Deletion of the mammalian circadian clock gene BMAL1/Mop3 alters baseline sleep architecture and the response to sleep deprivation. Sleep 28, 395 409. 39. Franken, P., Dudley, C.A., Estill, S.J., Barakat, M., Thomason, R., O Hara, B.F., and McKnight, S.L. (2006). NPAS2 as a transcriptional regulator of non-rapid eye movement sleep: Genotype and sex interactions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 103, 7118 7123. 40. Dijk, D.J., and Franken, P. (2005). Interaction of sleep homeostasis and circadian rhythmicity: Dependent or independent systems? In Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, Fourth Edition, M.H. Kryger, T. Roth, and W.C. Dement, eds. (Philadelphia: Elsevier), pp. 418 434.