UCLA UCLA Previously Published Works

Similar documents
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Responses Relate to Differences in Real-World Social Experience

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression

The Neural Correlates of Moral Decision-Making in Psychopathy

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Supplementary Online Content

What do you think of the following research? I m interested in whether a low glycemic index diet gives better control of diabetes than a high

Authors: Paul A. Frewen, PhD 1,2,*, David J. A. Dozois, PhD 1,2, Richard W. J. Neufeld 1,2,3, &Ruth A. Lanius, MD, PhD 2,3 Departments of Psychology

Complicated Grief. Sidney Zisook, M.D*.

smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy

Acetaminophen Reduces Social Pain: Behavioral and Neural Evidence

A 1.5-Year Follow-Up of an Internet-Based Intervention for Complicated Grief

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition

Our Health, Our Thoughts and Our Feelings: How Can We Best Adapt Resiliently During Grief?

The Neural Basis of Economic Decision- Making in The Ultimatum Game

Moving the brain: Neuroimaging motivational changes of deep brain stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder Figee, M.

Neuroimaging vs. other methods

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M.

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Supplementary Online Content

Validation of the Integration of Stressful Life Experiences Scale Short Form in a Bereaved Sample

Distinguishing informational from value-related encoding of rewarding and punishing outcomes in the human brain

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety

Supporting Information. Demonstration of effort-discounting in dlpfc

MARY-FRANCES O CONNOR CURRICULUM VITAE

2/28/2017. Attachment in Substance Use. Objectives. Social Attachments

Grief, Depression, & The DSM-5

Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16

Baseline Vagal Tone Predicts BOLD Response during Elicitation of Grief

The Importance of the Mind for Understanding How Emotions Are

Opinion This Is Your Brain on Politics

Love, Emotions, and the Highly Sensitive Brain

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neuroimage. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 July 15.

Yearning for connection? Loneliness is associated with increased ventral striatum activity to close others

Supplementary Information

Neurobiological Foundations of Reward and Risk

Role of the ventral striatum in developing anorexia nervosa

Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Journal of Affective Disorders

Neurobiology of Addiction

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Eur J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 August 10.

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

The rewarding nature of provocationfocused rumination in women with borderline personality disorder: a preliminary fmri investigation

Identification of Neuroimaging Biomarkers

Personal Space Regulation by the Human Amygdala. California Institute of Technology

Sun, D; Chan, CCH; Hu, Y; Wang, Z; Lee, TMC

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation

Distinct valuation subsystems in the human brain for effort and delay

Supporting Online Material for

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group

VIII. 10. Right Temporal-Lobe Contribution to the Retrieval of Family Relationships in Person Identification

Neural correlates of social exclusion during adolescence: understanding the distress of peer rejection

Nearly 2.5 million persons die each year in the United

Supplementary Online Content

For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Task timeline for Solo and Info trials.

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

PTSD and the brain: What clinicians need to know

An fmri Study of Risky Decision Making: The Role of Mental Preparation and Conflict

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety

Putting Feelings Into Words

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders 2012, 2:11

Supplementary materials. Appendix A;

Embargoed until Oct. 16, 3:30 p.m. CST Contacts: Kat Snodgrass, (202) Press Room, Oct : (504) Todd Bentsen, (202)

NeuroImage 47 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage

Why gamblers fail to win: evidence from neuroscience

Supporting Information

9.01 Introduction to Neuroscience Fall 2007

Part IV: Slipping Up: Neuroscience Basis of Relapse & Recovery July 31, am 12:30 pm with Break 10-10:15am

Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way

Individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) often

What are Substance Use Disorders?

Reproducibility of Visual Activation During Checkerboard Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging at 4 Tesla

Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex

Exploring the brain and behavioral mechanisms of MBIs for depression and anxiety

Neurobehavioral Mechanisms of Fear Generalization in PTSD. Lei Zhang. Duke University, Durham Veteran Affairs Medical Center

Practice Question MOTIVATION AND EMOTION. Motivation as Drives. Motivation 10/22/2012

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging with Arterial Spin Labeling: Techniques and Potential Clinical and Research Applications

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri

Clinical Reasoning for Western Acupuncture

Understanding Addiction: Why Can t Those Affected Just Say No?

Contact Search Sitemap. Gallery Products Staff. Press. Scientific Background. Collaborators

The Neurobiology of Addiction

Functional Movement Disorders

Voxel-based morphometry in clinical neurosciences

Title of file for HTML: Peer Review File Description:

Supplementary Digital Content

Background Paper: Obsessive Compulsive Disorder. Kristen Thomas. University of Pittsburgh

The Effect of Regular Naltrexone Dosing on Disordered Gamblers: An Examination of Neural Activation, Gambling Urges, and Gambling Behaviour

Amy Garrett, Ph.D., Victor Carrion, M.D., and Allan Reiss, M.D. Stanford University School of Medicine Department of Psychiatry

Altered effective connectivity network of the thalamus in post traumatic stress disorder: a resting state FMRI study with granger causality method

Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain

Transcription:

UCLA UCLA Previously Published Works Title Craving love? Enduring grief activates brain's reward center. Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/4w5644dq Journal NeuroImage, 42(2) ISSN 1095-9572 Authors O'Connor, Mary-Frances Wellisch, David K Stanton, Annette L et al. Publication Date 2008-08-15 DOI doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.256 Peer reviewed escholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California

Text: 1,774 words Abstract: 150 Figures: 3 Tables: 1 Craving love? Enduring grief activates brain s reward center Mary-Frances O Connor a, David K. Wellisch b, Annette L. Stanton abc, Naomi I. Eisenberger c, Michael R. Irwin a, Matthew D. Lieberman c a Cousins Center for Psychoneuroimmunology, Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, University of California at Los Angeles; b Department of Psychiatry, Semel Institute for Neuroscience, University of California at Los Angeles; c Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles. 1

ABSTRACT (150 words) Complicated grief (CG) occurs when an individual experiences prolonged, unabated grief. The neural mechanisms distinguishing CG from noncomplicated grief (NCG) are unclear, but hypothesized mechanisms include both pain-related activity (related to the social pain of loss) and reward-related activity (related to attachment behavior). Bereaved women (11 CG, 12 NCG) participated in an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging scan, during grief elicitation with idiographic stimuli. Analyses revealed that whereas both CG and NCG participants showed pain-related neural activity in response to reminders of the deceased, only those with CG showed reward-related activity in the nucleus accumbens (NA). This NA cluster was positively correlated with self-reported yearning, but not with time since death, participant age, or positive/negative affect. This study supports the hypothesis that attachment activates reward pathways. For those with CG, reminders of the deceased still activate neural reward activity, which may interfere with adapting to the loss in the present. 2

There is no pain so great as the memory of joy in present grief. ~Aeschylus, founder of Greek tragedy Introduction Grief is one of life s most painful experiences. When suffering the loss of a loved one, one can feel as if the attendant sadness and longing will last indefinitely. Although successful adaptation to the loss is the most frequent response (Bonnano et al., 2002), grief does not abate in a substantial minority; rather, it develops into Complicated Grief (CG) (Ott, Lueger, Kelber, & Prigerson, 2007). CG, previously known as chronic, pathological or traumatic grief, includes debilitating recurrent pangs of painful emotions, with intense yearning, longing and searching for the deceased, and preoccupation with thoughts of the loved one. This syndrome has now been defined by an empirically-derived set of criteria (Boelen & van den Bout, 2005) and is being considered for inclusion in the DSM-V. The neurocognitive mechanisms involved in CG are currently unknown. Some have hypothesized that attachment may activate reward pathways and that this neural response may have addiction-like properties (Insel, 2003; Panksepp, Knutson, & Burgdorf, 2002). We hypothesize that a major neurocognitive difference between CG and noncomplicated grief (NCG) is that reminders of the deceased may still activate neural rewards for those with CG. This reward activation may interfere with adapting to the loss in the present. Supporting this idea are subjective reports from CG patients indicating pleasurable reveries about the lost love (during which the reality of the loss is ignored) in addition to painful yearning (Shear, Frank, Houck, & Reynolds, 2005). These self-report findings suggest the involvement of both reward and pain networks. 3

The nucleus accumbens (NA) is the region most commonly associated with reward (Knutson, Adams, & Fong, 2001). NA has also been shown to play a role in social attachment, such as sibling and maternal affiliation, via neurotransmitters and peptides (e.g., dopamine and oxytocin) (Young, Lim, Gingrich, & Insel, 2001). The pain network, by contrast, including the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dacc), insula, and periaqueductal gray (PAG) has been previously implicated in both physical (Rainville, 2002) and social pain (Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams, 2003; Rainville, 2002). The present analysis specifically investigates whether the CG group had greater activity occurred in the brain s reward or pain networks than the NCG group. Methods and Materials Participants Women (11 CG, 12 NCG) who had experienced the death of a mother or sister to breast cancer in the past 5 years were recruited to participate in an event-related fmri study. Participants were recruited from a clinic for women at familial risk of breast cancer and from the community. Sixty-four persons were screened by telephone, and 35 had an initial interview. Participants were excluded for current Axis I disorder (including major depression), as evidenced by a structured clinical interview (Spitzer, Williams, Gibbons, & First, 1994). CG was diagnosed in a structured clinical interview (Prigerson & Jacobs, 2001) using empirically-derived criteria. The mean age of participants was 43.70 (SD = 10.00), they had an average of 16.57 (SD = 2.35) years of school and 78.26% were white. The two groups did not differ on any demographic characteristics as assessed by ANOVA and chi-square analyses. The Institutional Review Board at UCLA approved the study and all participants gave written informed consent. Procedure 4

Each participant provided a photograph of their deceased loved one and these photos were matched with photos of a stranger. Participants also shared an autobiographical narrative of the death event and 15 grief-related idiographic words were chosen from it. These words were matched for part of speech, number of letters and frequency of usage in the English language with 15 neutral words. These photos and words were made into 60 composites (Figure 1), each consisting of one picture (deceased or stranger) and one word (grief-related or neutral). Empirical support for the grief-eliciting task has been shown previously using both skin conductance methodology and participants subjective reports (Gündel, O'Connor, Littrell, Fort, & Lane, 2003). Participants viewed composites through goggles in randomized order. Each participant was given the same set of instructions: Focus on any thoughts, feelings or memories you have to the combination of the picture and the word on the slide. Let yourself respond emotionally to each slide by being aware of your feelings without trying to alter them. fmri Methodology Data were acquired on a Siemens Allegra 3T scanner. For each participant, a highresolution structural T2-weighted echo-planar image (spin-echo; TR = 5000 ms; TE=33 ms; matrix size 128 x 128; 36 axial slices; FOV = 20cm; 3-mm thick, skip 1-mm) was acquired coplanar with the functional scan. A functional scan, lasting 5 minutes and 30 seconds, was acquired (echo planar T2*-weighted gradient-echo, TR = 2500 ms, TE = 25 ms, flip angle = 90, matrix size 64 x 64, 36 axial slices, FOV = 20cm; 3-mm thick, skip 1-mm). The imaging data were analyzed using statistical parametric mapping (SPM 99; Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, Institute of Neurology, London, UK). Images were realigned, normalized, and smoothed with an 8mm Gaussian kernel, full width at half maximum. Analysis used the general linear model in an event-related analysis. Effects at each 5

voxel were estimated using linear contrasts to compare regionally specific effects. The contrasts for the individual subjects were aggregated for single-group analysis and for between-group analysis according to the random effect model in SPM. All group comparison analyses (e.g., CG > NCG) were thresholded using an uncorrected p-value of.005 combined with a cluster size threshold of 10 voxels. Where indicated, parameter estimates were extracted from group comparisons and reported for each group separately. All coordinates are reported in MNI format. Results Demographics. The mean age of participants was 43.70 (SD = 10.00), they had an average of 16.57 (SD = 2.35) years of school and 78.26% were white. The two groups did not differ on any demographic characteristics as assessed by ANOVA and chi-square analyses. Reward Activation. Greater reward-related activity occurred in the CG relative to the NCG individuals. Notably, the NA (x=10, y=20, z=-6; Figure 2A) was the only region of the brain that was more active in response to grief-related words than neutral words among those with CG compared to NCG (t=3.51, p<0.001, 15 voxels). Parameter estimates were extracted from this region and reported separately for each group as shown in Figure 2B. CG individuals produced increased NA activity in response to grief words (t=3.36, p<.01), whereas the NCG individuals did not (t=-1.82, p<.10). A comparison of the deceased and stranger photos did not produce NA activity. This may have been due to habituation effects as 15 different grief words were used, but only a single deceased photograph was shown repeatedly. To examine this possibility, we compared deceased and stranger photos from the first third of the trials only in order to isolate pre-habituation activity. Here, as in the grief words comparison, there was 6

greater activation in the NA (x=-14, y=4, z=-16) for the CG group than the NCG group (t=3.68, p<.001, 18 voxels). Yearning. To clarify the functional role of NA activity in grief, correlational analyses were performed with parameter estimates of activity from the observed cluster (10, 20, -6). Greater NA activation in the grief words comparison correlated significantly with more selfreported yearning for the deceased (r=.42, p<.05) (Figure 3). No correlation was observed between NA activation and length of time since the death, participant age, or general positive or negative affect. Similarly, the NA activation observed during the early trials of the deceased photos comparison was also associated with yearning (r=.36, p<.05), and was not correlated with length of time since the death, participant age, or general positive or negative affect. Pain Network Activation. To examine pain-related neural activity, we examined: a) whether each group showed neural activity in three regions previously linked to pain processes (anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), insula, periaqueductal gray (PAG) (Peyron, Laurent & Garcia- Larrea, 2000); and b) whether there were significant differences between the groups in painrelated neural activity in those regions (p <.005, 10 voxels). If one group showed significant activation in a region (p<.005, 10 voxels), we reported the activation from that same coordinate in the other group (Table 1). Each group showed significant activity in pain-related regions (dacc, insula, PAG) in the comparisons of the deceased vs. stranger pictures and grief vs. neutral words (Table 1), as has been previously shown in NCG (Gündel et al., 2003). The only activity in pain-related regions that differed between groups was in the left insula (x=-34, y=14, z=18), which was more active in the NCG group during the viewing of grief-related vs. neutral words (t=4.20, p<0.001, 87 voxels). Discussion 7

Two models of grief have been hypothesized: a detachment model and a reunion model (Bowlby, 1980). In the detachment model, the grief emotion is believed to play a role in the acceptance of the reality of the death and therefore assist in recovery from the loss. In the reunion model, the grief is a form of protest against the separation from the deceased, and serves to promote reunion with the lost person, not detachment. After the death, cues of the deceased (such as memories, photos, etc.) persist, triggering yearning and grief. Coping with these salient cues is a major task in adjusting to the death (Freed & Mann, 2007). Freed and Mann hypothesize that if the detachment model is correct, the pangs of grief would occur with reduced NA activity over time, as the salience of the cues decreases and acceptance of the reality leads to detachment. If the reunion model is correct, that the pangs of grief would continue to occur with NA activity, with reward activity in response to the cues motivating reunion with the deceased. This study demonstrates that each of these models may be accurate for distinct subgroups of bereaved individuals: relative to non-grief eliciting stimuli, those with CG produced significant NA activations, whereas those with NCG produced NA reductions during grief elicitation. Findings suggest that both CG and NCG groups may have felt pain upon presentation of grief-related stimuli, but only those with CG also activated an area important for reward processing when viewing cues of the deceased. The association between NA activity and yearning, but not time since death, participant age, or positive/negative affect provides convergent and discriminative validity. The addiction-relevant aspect of this neural response (Knutson et al., 2001) may help to explain why it is hard to resist engaging in pleasurable reveries about the deceased even though engaging in these reveries may prevent those with CG from adjusting the to the realities of the present. Many who suffer from addiction-like disorders experience them as afflictions; similarly we are not suggesting that reveries about the deceased 8

are emotionally satisfying, but rather may serve as craving responses that may make adapting to the reality of the loss more difficult. Prior work has demonstrated a relationship between autonomic arousal and posterior cingulate (PCC) activity during grief-elicitation (O Connor et al., 2007). O Connor and colleagues hypothesized a role for PCC in those with CG, given that those with CG may have greater arousal. However, there were no significant differences in PCC activity between the NCG and CG groups in the present study. Future work is needed to more carefully explore whether the PCC plays a role in CG in a larger sample. In addition, connectivity analyses in the prior study showed that PCC activation was associated with subgenual ACC activation. Similarly, future studies should investigate connectivity between NA and prefrontal or limbic regions important in emotion regulation. Understanding the reward processes activated in those with CG could substantially change treatment of this disorder. Therapies such as behavioral interventions that target reward processes may confer benefit and preferentially aid in adapting to the loss (Shear et al., 2005). Likewise, dopaminergic interventions that alter reward sensitivity could theoretically be more effective in treating CG than serotonergic interventions, which have failed to alter grief intensity (Zygmont et al., 1998). Addressing the continued craving of past relationships may assist those with CG in adapting to the loss. 9

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We would like to thank the UCLA Brain Mapping Center for their assistance. This research was supported by funds from the California Breast Cancer Research Program of the University of California, Grant Number 10IB-0048. This work was also supported in part by grant T32- MH19925, the Cousins Center for Psychoneuroimmunology and the Friends of the Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior. 10

REFERENCES Boelen, P. A., & van den Bout, J. (2005). Complicated Grief, Depression, and Anxiety as Distinct Postloss Syndromes: A Confirmatory Factor Analysis Study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(11), 2175-2177. Bonnano, G. A., Wortman, C. B., Lehman, D. R., Tweed, R. G., Haring, M., Sonnega, J., et al. (2002). Resilience to loss and chronic grief: A prospective study from preloss to 18-months postloss. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 83(5), 1150-1164. Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol 3. Loss, sadness and depression. New York: Basic Books. Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fmri study of social exclusion. Science, 302(5643), 290-292. Gündel, H., O'Connor, M.-F., Littrell, L., Fort, C., & Lane, R. D. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of grief: An fmri study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(11), 1946-1953. Insel, T. R. (2003). Is social attachment an addictive disorder? Physiology & Behavior, 79(3), 351. Knutson, B., Adams, C., & Fong, G. H., D. (2001). Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. Journal of Neuroscience. O Connor, M.-F., Gündel, H., McRae, K., & Lane, R.D. (2007). Functional neuroanatomical correlates of emotional arousal during grief. Neuropsychopharmacology 32, 2184-2189. Ott, C. H., Lueger, R. J., Kelber, S. T., & Prigerson, H. G. (2007). Spousal bereavement in older adults: Common, resilient, and chronic grief with defining characteristics. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders, 195(4), 332-341. Panksepp, J., Knutson, B., & Burgdorf, J. (2002). The role of brain emotional systems in addictions: a neuro-evolutionary perspective and new "self-report" animal model. Addiction, 97(4), 459-469. Peyron, R., Laurent, B., & Garcia-Larrea, L. (2000). Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis. Clinical Neurophysiology, 30(5), 263-288. Prigerson, H. G., & Jacobs, S. C. (2001). Traumatic grief as a distinct disorder: A rationale, consensus criteria, and preliminary empirical test. In M. S. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, W. Stroebe & H. Schut (Eds.), Handbook of Bereavement Research: Consequences, Coping and Care (pp. 613-645). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association. Rainville, P. (2002). Brain mechanisms of pain affect and pain modulation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12(2), 195. Shear, K., Frank, E., Houck, P. R., & Reynolds, C. F., III. (2005). Treatment of Complicated Grief: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 293(21), 2601-2608. Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., Gibbons, M., & First, M. D. (1994). Structured Clinical Interview of the DSM-IV. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Young, L., Lim, M., Gingrich, B., & Insel, T. (2001). Cellular mechanisms of social attachment. Hormones and Behavior. Zygmont, M., Prigerson, H., Houck, P., Miller, M., Shear, M., Jacobs, S., et al. (1998). A post hoc comparison of paroxetine and nortriptyline for symptoms of traumatic grief. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(5), 241-245. 11

Region Group MNI Coordinates (x,y,z) T p-value (voxels) Grief-related > Neutral (words): ACC NCG -2 34 8 5.12 0.001 (140) CG -2 34 8 2.74 0.02 Insula NCG -36 18 18 4.73 0.001 (44) CG ----- ----- ----- PAG NCG ----- ----- ----- CG ----- ----- ----- Deceased > Stranger (pictures): dacc NCG -2 24 24 4.17 0.001 (309) CG -2 24 24 1.87 0.05 Insula NCG -40 8-2 4.76 0.001 (235) 48 10 0 4.47 0.001 (277) CG -40 8-2 1.70 0.02 48 10 0 2.31 0.03 PAG NCG -10-18 -16 9.74 0.0001 (55) CG -10-18 -16 2.37 0.02 Table 1. Effects in three regions of the pain network for both CG (N=11) and NCG (N=12) in response to pictures of the deceased and grief-related words. Significant activations in one group (p <.005, 10 voxels) were examined and reported for the other group. T and p values are listed for each group separately. Cluster sizes were determined at the threshold of.005, 10 voxels (if the significance did not reach this threshold, cluster sizes were not comparable and therefore not listed). dacc = dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, PAG = periaqueductal gray 12

Figure 1. Example of picture/word composites presented to subject. 13

Figure 2. A) Nucleus accumbens activity (10, 20, -6) in response to grief-related vs. neutral words that was significantly greater in the Complicated Grief group compared to the Noncomplicated Grief group (pictured at p <.05). B) Bar graph showing nucleus accumbens activity (10, 20, -6) in response to grief-related vs. neutral words for those with Complicated and Noncomplicated Grief. 14

Figure 3. Positive correlation between self-reported yearning and BOLD activity in the NA (10, 20, -6; N=23) for each subject. 15