BEHAVIOR CHANGE: AN OUTLINE SUMMARY

Similar documents
Influencing Mountain Biker behaviour

Behavior Change Theories

BEHAVIOR CHANGE THEORY

2015 NADTA Conference Pre-Education Committee Book Club Everyday Bias, Howard J. Ross, Suggested Group Discussion Questions

Designing interventions to change behaviour. Susan Michie

Radon: A Behavioural as well as a Technical & Health Challenge.

SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY. Social Influences on the Self. Self Concept. How do we see ourselves? How do we see others?

Chapter 8: Consumer Attitude Formation and Change

Novel research methods used to explore behaviour change maintenance focusing on weight loss and physical activity

THE HEALTH BELIEF MODEL. Health Behavior CHAPTER 6

Chapter 8: Consumer Attitude Formation and Change

Littlejohn, S. W. (2001). Theories of human communication. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. ["Theories of Message Reception and Processing" pp.

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior

A Behavioural Insights Approach to Population Level Behaviour Change

Chapter 13. Social Psychology

Review of public information programmes to enhance home radon screening uptake and home remediation

Behavioral change theories : explain the reasons behind alterations in individuals' behavioral patterns. serve to understand better those factors

Chapter 3-Attitude Change - Objectives. Chapter 3 Outline -Attitude Change

individual differences strong situation interactional psychology locus of control personality general self-efficacy trait theory self-esteem

Psychology. January 11, 2019.

Breaking the Bias Habit. Jennifer Sheridan, Ph.D. Executive & Research Director Women in Science & Engineering Leadership Institute

Behavioural Insights: Changing Behaviours, Not Minds

Evaluating behavior change interventions in terms of their component techniques. Susan Michie

Users. Perception and Cognition

BEHAVIOUR WORKSHOP. Changing behaviour for good. 0020_Hamell_Workshop_Workbook_FAW.indd 1 14/09/ :43

Lesson 12. Understanding and Managing Individual Behavior

33 Multiple choice questions

Assessing Readiness To Change

Lumus360 Psychometric Profile Pat Sample

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

NEGOTIATION SEVENTH EDITION

Motivating children in and out of school: Research findings and practical implications

Person Perception. Forming Impressions of Others. Mar 5, 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu

reward based power have ability to give you what you want. coercive have power to punish

Revision notes 7.5. Section 7.5 Health promotion

SOCI 323 Social Psychology

Study Guide for Why We Overeat and How to Stop Copyright 2017, Elizabeth Babcock, LCSW

PSK409-Health Psychology

Client Care Counseling Critique Assignment Osteoporosis

The Psychology of Rare Events: Challenges to Managing Tail Risks

Factors that affect interpersonal attraction:

(SAT). d) inhibiting automatized responses.

Behavioural insights for intervention design

THE DYNAMICS OF MOTIVATION

Trauma Informed Care. Rick Edwards, LPC

Unconscious Social Behavior. John F. Kihlstrom. University of California, Berkeley

VIRGINIA LAW REVIEW IN BRIEF

Is there any way you might be better off if you quit? What happens when you think about it? What do you imagine will happen if you don t change?

Estimated Distribution of Items for the Exams

8/17/2012. Self-Concept Video. Cultural Differences in Defining the Self. Chapter 5. The Self: Understanding Ourselves in a Social Context

PUBLIC HEALTH GUIDANCE SCOPE

Communication for Change A S H O R T G U I D E T O S O C I A L A N D B E H A V I O R C H A N G E ( S B C C ) T H E O R Y A N D M O D E L S

Groups, norms, and conformity. What s it about?

Introduction to Psychology Social Psychology Quiz

HOW TO IDENTIFY A RESEARCH QUESTION? How to Extract a Question from a Topic that Interests You?

Education. ignored good behavior and scolded misbehavior and offered threats.

Motivation, Conflict, Emotion. Abdul-Monaf Al-Jadiry, MD; FRCPsych Professor of Psychiatry

The Attribute Index - Leadership

The Power Of Self-Belief

Tobacco dependence: Implications for service provision

HANDOUTS UNITED REPUBLIC OF TANZANIA MINISTRY OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL WELFARE. HIV and AIDS Voluntary Counselling and Testing MODULE 1.

Gold and Hohwy, Rationality and Schizophrenic Delusion

IMPLICIT BIAS IN A PROFESSIONAL SETTING

Howard Sheth Model. The model claims that a person s purchase decision is often influenced by more than one individuals.

Chapter Seven Attitude Change

The Ecological Perspective: A Multilevel, Interactive Approach

Motivation: Internalized Motivation in the Classroom 155

Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS) / Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/Encephalopathy (ME)

They are updated regularly as new NICE guidance is published. To view the latest version of this NICE Pathway see:

Chapter 3: Perception and the Self in IPC 01/24/2012

From theory-inspired to theory-based interventions: Linking behaviour change techniques to their mechanisms of action.

Implicit Attitude. Brian A. Nosek. University of Virginia. Mahzarin R. Banaji. Harvard University

ASSESSMENT OF INTRAPERSONAL SKILLS. Rick Hoyle Duke University

Role of Health Education and Communication Part 3

Resource: Focal Conflict Model

UBC Social Ecological Economic Development Studies (SEEDS) Student Report

WAKING UP TO UNCONSCIOUS BIAS JOY WARMINGTON, CEO

Health Behaviour Change: Applying Prochaska and DiClemente s Stages of Change Model

Defining Social Psychology. informal definition: the study of how people think about, influence, and relate to other people

MOTIVATIONAL INTERVIEWING

Psychological Barriers and Unintended Consequences in Outreach Campaigns Promoting Natural Shorelines with Property Owners

REACHING THE HARD TO REACH

My Notebook. A space for your private thoughts.

Perception and Processing of Safe Driving Messages: A Multi-Method Approach

2. Which of the following is not an element of McGuire s chain of cognitive responses model? a. Attention b. Comprehension c. Yielding d.

Motivation CURRENT MOTIVATION CONSTRUCTS

BY FORCE OF HABIT USING HABIT TO UNDERSTAND WEIGHT MANAGEMENT BEHAVIOURS

Defining Psychology Behaviorism: Social Psychology: Milgram s Obedience Studies Bystander Non-intervention Cognitive Psychology:

Black Butterfly: A Statement on Counseling Minority Youth. Kimberly McLeod. Texas Southern University

Postgraduate Diploma in Marketing June 2017 Examination Exploring Consumer Behaviour (ECB)

When Your Partner s Actions Seem Selfish, Inconsiderate, Immature, Inappropriate, or Bad in Some Other Way

MOPIP Meeting Nov. 1, Social Norms. Dan Reilly

Internalized Motivation in the Classroom

Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain

Motivation & Emotion. Outline Intrinsic & extrinsic motivation. Intrinsic-extrinsic motivations & goal-setting. Intrinsic motivation

HUMANITIES 001: CREATIVE MINDS W E E K 3

Users. Ergonomics vs. Cognetics

PERSON PERCEPTION September 25th, 2009 : Lecture 5

Pain Self-Management Strategies Wheel

The Knowing-Doing Gap

Transcription:

BEHAVIOR CHANGE: AN OUTLINE SUMMARY Created for Faunalytics by Alison Lenton, PhD, and Kathleen Patton November 2017 1. Definition A. Behavior: A physical action taken in response to either internal or external stimuli. The action may be observable or relatively covert/subtle, but in either case should be measureable. 2 2. A hypothetical depiction of behavior change and maintenance over time. 5 Changes in behavior potential (likelihood of engaging in a particular behavior) over time following initial behavior change. 3. Inputs to the initiation of a new behavior: There are 80+ theories of behavior change describing a multitude of potential inputs to that change. 2, 5 Below we outline some of the key inputs identified by theories relevant to health behavior, which is a fairly complex behavior (also see Appendices). 8 A. Cognitions (beliefs, perceptions) about the health threat : Higher perception of threat for NOT changing (e.g., cancer) Increased likelihood of initiating new behavior (e.g., smoking cessation). 1. The perceived likelihood of the risk (i.e., a person s judgment of the probability of some negative outcome if they don t change). a. Perceptions of probability are imprecise: People overestimate low risk and underestimate high risk. 2. The perceived severity of the risk (i.e., just how bad would that outcome be?). 3. Fear/anxiety about the risk (i.e., how much does the person fear the negative outcome?). 1

B. Cognitions about the new behavior (i.e., the behavior they are supposed to adopt): More positive beliefs Increased likelihood of initiating behavior change. 1. Intentions to implement the behavior (to what degree does the person develop when/where/how-plans to implement the change). 2. Willingness to implement the behavior (i.e., the person s inclination to perform the new behavior when in conducive circumstances). a. Intentions vs. Willingness: Research indicates that intentions better predict change in some behaviors (e.g., more familiar, low-risk behaviors), whereas willingness better predicts change in others (e.g., unfamiliar, high-risk behaviors). 3. Attitude (overall evaluation: positive and/or negative) toward the new behavior. a. Attitude may also encompass beliefs about whether the new behavior is actually effective in preventing or promoting some outcome and beliefs about whether there are any costs associated with learning or enacting the new behavior. 4. Social norms regarding the behavior (i.e., Injunctive norm: Is the behavior viewed positively by important others? Descriptive norm: Do other important people already do it?). 5. One s perceived ability to implement the behavior (i.e., self-efficacy; e.g., a person may want to implement the behavior, but feel unable to). 6. Social prototypes relevant to the behavior (i.e., stereotypes about the type of person who does or does not engage in the new behavior). a. Prototypes are a function of judgments of similarity (how similar are you to the type of person who does X?) and evaluation (how likeable or dislikable is the type of person who does X?). C. Volitional (goal- and motivation-related) factors: Greater goal-planning, -relevance and ability to/for the new behavior Increased likelihood of behavior change. 1. If-then plans (see 2.B.1) a. These plans will also include identification of obstacles to enacting the new behavior and the formulation of additional if-then plans for overcoming those obstacles. 2. Behavioral skills (i.e., does or will the person have the knowledge/skills required to implement the change?) a. Both actual and perceived skills are relevant to whether behavior will change 3. Processes of change (i.e., strategies a person can use to change their own behavior, e.g., developing self-awareness, re-organizing one s environment, engaging in substitute activities) 4. Progress monitoring (i.e., where the person reviews and evaluates their progress toward the goal perhaps recording their progress in a diary and, in so doing, may also make readjustments to their if-then plans) 2

a. Progress monitoring is often done via comparing oneself to one s own goal, as well as to someone else s progress with respect to that same goal. b. Comparisons relate to both features of the new behavior itself (e.g., frequency, intensity, duration of behavior X ) and to outcomes of the behavior (e.g., weight loss) 5. Executive function (i.e., the ability to self-regulate or exert control over oneself) a. Three components: i. Response inhibition (i.e., the ability to override the dominant/unwanted response); ii. Mental flexibility (i.e., being able to switch between rules, perhaps depending on context); iii. Working memory (i.e., being able to hold relevant information in mind and use it effectively) D. Implicit (unconscious, automatic) cognition: More positive implicit beliefs regarding the new behavior Increased likelihood of behavior change. At the same time, more positive implicit beliefs regarding the old behavior Lesser likelihood of behavior change. 1. Attentional bias (i.e., the extent to which behavior-related cues capture and hold the person s attention; e.g., tobacco, mock meat) 2. Implicit attitudes (i.e., the person s automatic affective/emotional reaction to a relevant stimulus). 3. Approach bias (i.e., the person s automatic action tendency toward the stimulus; measured in terms of relative approach versus avoidance tendencies). 4. Implicit goals or norms (standards for behavior that are unconsciously primed/cued by features of the context; e.g., in one study, a poster advertising a low-fat recipe primed participants to eat fewer snacks) 5. Habits (i.e., a memory-to-behavior process that is automatically and repeatedly performed in response to a particular cue/stimulus) 11, 12 a. Habitual behaviors are enacted quickly, efficiently, rigidly, thoughtlessly, and in a well-worn sequence. b. Triggering cues include time of day, location, setting, people, or preceding behaviors. c. Replacing an old habit with a new one is extremely challenging, because old habits get activated automatically in the presence of relevant cues. Even when new habits are formed, the old memory traces remain. Interventions work best when they interfere with the automatic cueing of old habits (via practicing response inhibition and managing exposure to the triggering cues) and, simultaneously, promote the repetition of the desired habit via: i. Frequent repetition; ii. The establishment of clear and stable cues for behavioral initiation, which can be facilitated by formulating concrete if-then plans and piggybacking the new habit onto an existing habit, such as teethbrushing; 3

iii. Rewards received for successful execution of the behavior, especially if the rewards are intermittent, e.g., like a slot-machine. 4. Initiating versus maintaining behavior change: These may require different types of interventions. Above, we have been addressing initial behavior change primarily. Factors that contribute to the maintenance of a new behavior include: 5, 7 A. Converting the behavior into a habit (see 3.D.5-3.D.5.a). B. Permanently changing key beliefs (e.g., beliefs regarding the behavioral outcomes, their enjoyment of the behavior, how the behavior relates to their identity and values, etc.) or changing how related ambiguous information is interpreted (such that ambiguous information is interpreted in a change-promoting direction). C. Successful monitoring and regulation of progress and the development of effective, implementable plans for overcoming obstacles. D. Creating a reinforcing social cycle, such that other people treat the person in a way that supports her/his behavior change and the person sees her/his new behavior have positive impact socially E. Changing the environment to reduce the future psychological and/or physical costs (e.g., self-regulatory, memory demands, financial outlay, discomfort, etc.) of continued engagement in the new behavior. 1. This notion is relevant to the concept of nudging people toward more rational behavior; see the book Nudge by Thaler and Sunstein (2008). F. Identifying rip current pathways, which is where there is an existing current of conditions that facilitate and support the targeted behavior: All the person needs to do is take the first, small step into the current, and then behavior change will happen without further intervention (e.g., getting people to vote in a political election for the first time makes it extremely likely they will thereafter become a regular voter). 5. Additional considerations A. Merely asking people about their behavior (about frequency, intentions, predictions, satisfaction, etc.), can change that behavior. This is called the question-behavior effect, or QBE. 9 1. QBE effects are larger when people are asked to report their cognitions (beliefs, perceptions) especially self-predictions -- than when asked to report their past behavior. 2. QBE effects are larger when people are asked to make predictions about a difficult behavior that they value highly (as cognitive dissonance is more likely if they fail to meet own standard) 3. QBE effects are larger when questions asked face-to-face vs. over the phone, internet, or mail B. Asking people to make multiple simultaneous behavioral changes (e.g., a diabetesintervention that attempts to change both diet and exercise at the same time) is beneficial up to a point: i.e., Making a moderate number of recommendations (2-3) is generally 4

better than making a single recommendation and better than making many (4+) recommendations. 10 1. A moderate number of simultaneous recommendations maximizes the interaction between a person s motivation and her/his capacity to undertake the changes. C. The timing of the behavior-change intervention matters: 7 1. Changing behavior now and changing it in the future require different interventions: a. Now Successful interventions take advantage of short-term memory biases, stereotyped-behavior, and contextual cues b. Future Successful interventions address inattention- and forgettingrelated challenges, e.g., via plan-making, repetition, social pressure. 2. Typically, the benefit of repeated intervention diminishes with each instance unless methods used to counteract habituation (e.g., if long time between interventions, if interventions are not identical). 3. Self-affirmation (i.e., bolstering sources of self-worth, e.g., values, strengths, social relationships, etc.) at each of three key stages of the behavior-change process (i) consideration of message acceptance, (ii) formulation of an intention to adopt the recommended behavior, and (iii) likelihood of enacting the recommended behavior lowers self-defensiveness and increases compliance at all three stages. 3 6. Common errors specific to attempts at changing health behaviors: 4 A. Error 1 Believing that the solution to some human-behavior problem is common sense. This is clearly untrue (consider weight loss and smoking cessation, as well as the abovedescribed array of complex inputs to behavior change) AND ignores the rich and complex psychological, social, and commercial environment in which people live. B. Error 2 Believing that there is some right way to convey a behavior-change message. This is the erroneous belief that some special rhetoric, media campaign, or other advertising tool can lead to fundamental behavior change across a population. Some health campaigns have been effective (HIV and condom-use campaigns), but these didn t rely solely on advertisements: They were part of multi-pronged, long-standing, centrallyorganized, government-supported campaigns. See Appendix 2 for a model of a multipronged approach. C. Error 3 Believing that people act rationally and, as such, that giving them the right information can change behavior. For example, people know they should eat more vegetables and less sugar, but this does not yield change. Many human behaviors, especially habits, do not require any thought (rational or otherwise) whatsoever. D. Error 4 Believing that people inevitably act irrationally: There are many reasons why people behave the way they do and these are often personal to the individual her/himself. One needs to try to understand the way ordinary people make sense of their choices and behaviors to have a better shot at helping them change (if they want to). E.g., people drink alcohol despite knowing the costs to their health. To dismiss the enjoyment people get out 5

of alcohol is to ignore a logical reason why they continue to do so: The perceived benefits outweigh the risks. E. Error 5 Believing that it is possible to accurately predict human behavior: Science has made great strides in understanding behavior-change mechanisms, but it is impossible to say with any certainty how a given individual will respond to any intervention attempt. Which intervention tool should be used, for which individuals, under which circumstances is, unfortunately, not yet understood. References 1. Abraham, C., & Michie, S. (2008). A taxonomy of behaviour change techniques used in interventions. Health Psychology, 27, 379 387. 2. Davis, R., Campbell, R., Hildon, Z., Hobbs, L., & Michie, S. (2015). Theories of behaviour and behaviour change across the social and behavioural sciences: A scoping review. Health Psychology Review, 9, 323-344. 3. Epton, T., Harris, P. R., Kane, R., van Koningsbruggen, G. M., & Sheeran, P. (2015). The impact of self-affirmation on health-behavior change: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology, 34, 187-196. 4. Kelly, M. P., & Barker, M. (2016). Why is changing health-related behavior so difficult? Public Health, 136, 109-116. 5. Kwasnicka, D., Dombrowski, S. U., White, M., & Sniehotta, F. (2016): Theoretical explanations for maintenance of behaviour change: A systematic review of behaviour theories. Health Psychology Review, 10, 277-296. 6. Michie, S., van Stralen, M. M., & West, W. (2011). The behaviour change wheel: A new method for characterizing and designing behaviour change interventions. Implementation Science, 6, 42. 7. Rogers T., & Frey, E. (2014). Changing behavior beyond the here and now. In Keren G., Wu G. (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (pp. 725 748). Oxford, England: John Wiley. 8. Sheeran, P., Klein, W. M. P., & Rothman, A. J. (2017). Health behavior change: Moving from observation to intervention. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 573-600. 9. Wilding, S., Conner, M., Sandberg, T., Prestwich, A., Lawton, R., Wood, C., Miles, E., Godin, G., & Sheeran, P. (2016). The question-behaviour effect: A theoretical and methodological review and meta-analysis, European Review of Social Psychology, 27, 196-230. 10. Wilson K, Senay I, Durantini M, Sánchez F, Hennessy M, Spring B, & Albarracín D. (2015). When it comes to lifestyle recommendations, more is sometimes less: A meta-analysis of theoretical assumptions underlying the effectiveness of interventions promoting multiple behavior domain change. Psychological Bulletin, 141, 474-509. 11. Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2016). Healthy through habit: Interventions for initiating and maintaining health behavior change. Behavioral Science & Policy, 2, 71 83. 12. Wood, W., & Runger, D. (2016). Psychology of habits. Annual Revieew of Psychology, 37, 289-314. 6

Appendix 1: Abraham and Michie s (2008) Taxonomy of Behavior Change Techniques 7

Appendix 2: Michie et al. s (2011) Behavior Change Wheel: A Method for Characterizing and Designing Health Behavior-Change Interventions 8