Two cases of mother infant cannibalism in orangutans

Similar documents
ARE YOU SMARTER THAN AN ORANGUTAN? Take the Quiz...

Lecture 9: Primate Behavior - Ecology

, 24 Affiliation/Position Name

Supplementary Table 1. Relationships between physiological measures and proxies of nutritional status across primate taxa

Top 25 Most Endangered Primates: the Most Current List

Stasiun pengamatan orangutan semi liar dan tantanganya

Index 297. Vervet monkey (Cercopithecus aethiops) , See also Monkey; Old World monkey Virunga National Park 159

The Function of Peaceful Post-Conflict Interactions: An Alternate View. Joan B. Silk. Department of Anthropology

Primates share several behavioral and biological characteristics, which indicates that they evolved from a common ancestor.

Orangutan Key Messages

PROBLEM-SOLVING WITH ORANGUTANS (PONGO PYGMAEUS AND PONGO ABELII) AND CHIMPANZEES (PAN TROGLODYTES):

Behavioral studies and veterinary management of orangutans at Bukit Merah Orang Utan Island, Perak, Malaysia

Author(s) Sakamaki, Tetsuya; Mbangi Mulavwa; Citation Pan Africa News (2009), 16(1):

Raiders of the Lost Bark: Orangutan Foraging Strategies in a Degraded Landscape

RAPID COMMUNICATION One-Trial Long-Lasting Food-Aversion Learning in Wild Japanese Monkeys (Macaca fuscata)

arboreal arboreal crepuscular ( nocturnal

Lester E Fisher Center for the Study and Conservation of Apes Lincoln Park Zoo, Chicago

Variable Grooming Behaviours in Wild Chimpanzees

Orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) Spatial Memory and Behavior in a Foraging Task

Charlotte Carne 1 *, Stuart Semple 1, Helen Morrogh-Bernard 2,3, Klaus Zuberbühler 4,5, Julia Lehmann 1. Abstract. Introduction

ALL ABOUT PRIMATES! Gorilla World and Jungle Trails

Grooming Partners of Immature Blue Monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis) in the Kakamega Forest, Kenya

Accelerating Academic Achievement. chimpanzee

Intraspecific killing among non-human primates

ALL ABOUT PRIMATES! Gorilla World and Jungle Trails

Lab 6 Primate Behavior

INTRODUCTION TO ORANGUTAN CULTURES. One of the implicit goals in the study of primate behavior (and indeed many

INDEX. California State University, Fullerton, v-vi, 263, 280 Cannibalism, 47 Celebes black macaque (Macaca nigra), see primates,

Chapter 7. Primate Sociality, Social Behavior, and Culture

Daily Skill Builders:

Jurmain et al. Chapter 7. Primate Behavior

TWO MAJOR GROUPS: PROSIMIANS NOT MANY SPECIES, LESS ADVANCED ANTHROPOIDS A TON OF SPECIES, MORE ADVANCED

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 15 Primate sociality: Predators and living in groups Copyright Bruce Owen 2010 Most haplorrine, and

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2008 As we have seen before, the bottom line

MATERNAL DOMINANCE RANK AND AGE AFFECT OFFSPRING SEX RATIO IN PIGTAIL MACAQUES

SEXUAL SWELLINGS OF FEMALE GIBBONS. Kalaweit Gibbon Rehabilitation Project, Kalaweit Care Centre, Jalan Punis No. 14,

Primate Films Database

THE UNREGULATED AND INFORMAL TRADE IN WILDLIFE: IMPLICATIONS FOR BIODIVERSITY AND HEALTH

TRAINING A WHITE RHINO FOR ARTIFICIAL INSEMINATION Amber Berndt and Susanne Wyatt, Indianapolis Zoo,

Unusual behaviour of captive-raised gibbons: implications for welfare

Primate Behavioral Ecology Anthro 381. Nicole Hess, PhD

Human Developmental Research 2006.Vol

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 12 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2009 We want to understand the reasons

The World of Primates

Suborder Haplorrhini

Australasian Primate Society Conference. Program & Abstracts

Grade 5 English Language Arts/Literacy Research Simulation Task 2018 Released Items

A n t h r o p o l o g y

Wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) distinguish between different scream types: evidence from a playback study

Thore Jon Bergman. Department of Psychology Office: University of Michigan Fax:

Behavioral, Ecological, and Evolutionary Aspects of Meat-Eating by Sumatran Orangutans (Pongo abelii)

CURRICULUM VITAE TANYA M. MUELLER

ESRM 350 Reproduction and Mating Systems

Supplemental Table 1. List of cell lines used

Reintroduction of Orangutans: A New Approach. Indonesia. Verslagen en technische gegevens

FEEDING COMPETITION AND PATCH SIZE IN THE CHIMPANZEE SPECIES PAN PANISCUS AND PAN TROGLODYTES. (Acc. 14-XII-1987)

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2010 We want to understand the reasons

Measuring temperament in rhesus macaques: consistency and change in emotionality over time

Mangabey (Cercocebus albigena) Ranging Patterns in Relation to Fruit Availability and the Risk of Parasite Infection in Kibale National Park, Uganda

Unit 4 continued Primates and primate behavior

Peaceful Primates: The History and Function of Reconciliation in Non-Human Primate Societies

A comparison of buttress drumming by male chimpanzees from two populations

Orang utans. Understanding Forced Copulations

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Wild Orangutan Males Plan and Communicate Their Travel Direction One Day in Advance

Great Apes Do Not Learn Novel Tool Use Easily: Conservatism, Functional Fixedness, or Cultural Influence? GRUBER, Thibaud

Mother-Offspring Conflict in Orangutans

EDUCATION PROFESSIONAL EXPERIENCE PUBLICATIONS. Journal Articles

REPRODUCTION AND INFANT PELAGE COLOURATION OF THE BANDED LEAF MONKEY (MAMMALIA: PRIMATES: CERCOPITHECIDAE) IN SINGAPORE

Animal Behavior. Thur March 24: Animal Behavior: outline. Questions: What, How, and Why. Behavior is shaped partly by inheritance

Erratum to: Functional response to fruiting seasonality by a primate seed predator,

.%e. Orangutans in Perspective. Forced Copulations and Female Mating Resistance. Cheryl D, Knott and Sonya M. Kohlenberg. *S;:; -'* ::.

BLY 122 Lecture Notes (O Brien) Chapter 47 Behavior

Groups studied in the comparative analysis of. culture. Source: Cultures in Chimpanzees Nature, 399, (1999)

When the last member of a species dies without any surviving

Bio 1M: The evolution of apes (complete) 1 Example. 2 Patterns of evolution. Similarities and differences. History

When the last member of a species dies without any surviving

First generation microarray-system for identification of primate species subject to bushmeat trade

Spider monkey in Taricaya

IPS - Research Grant Application

We are an example of a biological species that has evolved

QUARTERLY ASSESSMENT

S.No. Chapters Page No. 1. Plants Animals Air Activities on Air Water Our Body...

Journal of Human Evolution

Swadesh wordlist, categorised by semantic field.

The Conservation Implications of Avian Influenza (H5N1) Colin Poole, Asia Program, WCS

Andrea W. Clay a b, Mollie A. Bloomsmith a b, M. Jackson Marr b & Terry L. Maple b a Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory

Left Nipple Preferences in Infant Pan paniscus and P. troglodytes

Challenges Facing Preservation and Conservation of Asian Elephants

An observation of lethal aggression in Bornean whitebearded gibbons Hylobates albibarbis

Feeding ecology of olive baboons in the Kibale forest: Preliminary results on diet and food selection June 5 th July 11 th Caley Johnson

Organism Project. Asian Elephant. Abby-Rose Mannes

5. Which word describes the tone of

EBOLA INFORMATION KIT 20 to 26 October 2014 MEMBERS NEWS HUNTING SERVICES FUND LEGISLATION CALENDAR GENERAL SHOP POPULAR ARTICLES

Forage 27.5% Rest 9% Travel 8% Socialize 4% Other 1.5% Sleep (nest) 50%

Ch. 5 - Primate Behavioral Ecology

HIST 110 HISTORY OF CIVILIZATIONS. Week 1 & 2 October 2, Animals and Humans. Human Dynamism & Invasiveness

Types of Mating Systems

Sexual selection and the evolution of sex differences

Coprophagy in Captive Brown Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella)

Transcription:

DOI 10.1007/s10329-009-0142-5 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Two cases of mother infant cannibalism in orangutans David Fenwick Dellatore Æ Corri D. Waitt Æ Ivona Foitova Received: 9 February 2009 / Accepted: 28 February 2009 Ó Japan Monkey Centre and Springer 2009 Abstract Observations of ape cannibalism have to this point been limited to chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) where it is associated with infanticide and consumption by unrelated individuals (Watts and Mitani, Primates 41(4):357 365, 2000). Here we report for the first time observations of two unrelated female Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii) cannibalizing the remains of their infants on different occasion, a behavior never before reported in any ape species. The two orangutans were wild-born rehabilitated individuals, and had been reintroduced to an area hosting a largely unregulated primate tourism industry and experienced restricted ranging conditions. Though it is possible that this is a strategy to regain energy and nutrients or a result of individual history, comparative data suggest that this is an aberrant behavior which may be linked to environmental stressors within the area. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s10329-009-0142-5) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users. D. F. Dellatore (&) School of Social Sciences and Law, Oxford Brookes University, Oxford OX3 0BP, UK e-mail: dave@orangutans-sos.org D. F. Dellatore Sumatran Orangutan Society, Jl. Sei Bohorok Baru No. 79, Medan, North Sumatra 20154, Indonesia C. D. Waitt Department of Zoology, Oxford University, Oxford OX2 8QJ, UK I. Foitova Department of Botany and Zoology, Masaryk University, 61137 Brno, Czech Republic Keywords Cannibalism Orangutan Reintroduction Stress Primate tourism Introduction Cannibalism is a rare event in primates, with observed cases documented in only a few species, including: chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) (Palombit et al. 2000), common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) (Bezerra et al. 2007), snubnosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus bieti) (Xiang and Grueter 2007), blue monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni) (Fairgrieve 1995), and thick-tailed bushbabies (Galago crassicaudatus umbrosus) (Tartabini 1991). Of the great apes, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) alone had thus far been observed engaging in cannibalism (Bygott 1972; Watts and Mitani 2000), with cannibalism inferred in gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei) through infant remains found in the feces of two unrelated individuals (Fossey 1976, cited in Goodall 1977). Mother infant, or filial, cannibalism is more common in both nonprimate species and primate species living under stressful laboratory conditions, such as galagoes (Rohwer 1978; Tartabini 1991). Methods Field studies took place within Gunung Leuser National Park, Sumatra, Indonesia. The site, Bukit Lawang (03 32.770 0 N, 098 07.000 0 E), hosted a rehabilitation project from 1972 to 2001, until alternative facilities were developed in Sumatra. Throughout the time the rehabilitation project was running, Bukit Lawang was and still remains host to a wildlife tourism industry centered on the orangutans. Although it is forbidden to touch, feed or

disturb the orangutans, such practices have (Rijksen and Meijaard 1999) and still do occur in the forest for the enjoyment of tourists. Observations reported here constituted part of a larger study on the behavioral health of these orangutans. The data reported are from observations collected from 22 May to 31 July 2007. Focal animal sampling with continuous recording (Altmann 1974) was conducted from dawn to dusk (sleeping nest to sleeping nest). The cannibalistic mothers were two adult reintroduced, or semiwild females, Edita and Ratna, who were both confiscated from the pet trade. Approximate ages at rescue and at the time of the observed events are included in Table 1. Results Edita, at the time of 23 years of age, had her 12-month-old infant die in the forest of Bukit Lawang on 21 May 2007. She carried and protected the corpse with her for 7 days, and came into contact with two conspecifics, both of whom showed no interest in the corpse. Both before and after consumption began, Edita would remove and inspect the corpse which she carried slung around the back of her neck, occasionally emitting a whimper vocalization. On the eighth day Edita began consuming the remains of the infant. By this point most of the body had decomposed in the rainforest climate, with only the backbone and limbs remaining. She was observed eating in nine bouts, ranging from 1 to 13 min. At one point she dipped a portion of the corpse into a tree hole filled with water and then ate from it. Edita encountered no other orangutans beyond her own accompanying juvenile offspring, 8-year-old Sepi, who was on two occasions observed begging, and eventually allowed to consume a portion both times through passive food sharing. The corpse was consumed in its entirety over three consecutive days. The second observation took place less than a month later in an unrelated 20-year-old female, Ratna, whose 7-monthold infant died in the forest on 9 June 2007. Neither mother was within any observable proximity of the other since the first instance of cannibalism occurred, with consumption beginning with Ratna on 13 June 2007. Similarly, little of the body remained, with the head and most of the body missing. Three observed bouts, ranging from 7 to 9 min occurred on a single day (Fig. 1 and also see video clip available as online supplementary material). Ratna was not heard to emit any vocalizations, and on four occasions was seen swinging the corpse from her foot whilst sitting atop a branch. She chewed and consumed the soft tissue, spitting out the bone material (with a small portion, believed to be the clavicle, recovered). During the final bout, she was observed taking the corpse into her mouth, and then mixing with insects sucked out of a termite nest taken from the ground. She continued to carry the remains for the remainder of the day until building and entering a night nest 2 h 50 min later. Ratna no longer had the corpse when she exited her nest the following morning, thus she either discarded or consumed all that remained. Causes of death remain unknown in both cases, but Ratna s infant was observed to have appeared unwell and unresponsive a few days before death. There was no evidence that either of the mothers harmed or killed their own infants. Discussion Cannibalism of infants in primates is generally associated with infanticide by unrelated individuals, where it appears that the primary intent is only to kill from infanticide and not cannibalism (Hrdy 1974). Instances of killing competitors infants have been reported for both adult males (e.g., chacma baboons: Palombit et al. 2000) and, though to a lesser extent, adult females (e.g., chimpanzees: Table 1 Information regarding the cannibalistic reintroduced orangutans Orangutan Date confiscated Age at confiscation Age at birth of offspring Offspring Sex Date of birth Interbirth interval Alive Date of death Age at death Edita 5 Sep 1988 5 10.24 Heppy Male 1 Dec 1993 N 12 Mar 1998 4.28 15.72 Sepi Female 28 May 1999 5.49 Y 22.72 Saprol Male 27 May 2006 7 N 21 May 2007 0.98 Ratna 2 Feb 1991 4 10.88 Moneter Male 20 Dec 1997 N 12 Mar 1998 0.22 12.83 Desmel Male 1 Dec 1999 1.95 N 16 Jun 2000 0.54 14.37 Silvia Female 15 Jun 2001 1.54 N 21 Dec 2002 1.52 16.25 Sukajulu Male 6 May 2003 1.89 N Unknown ±1 18.50 Agustin Male 3 Aug 2005 2.25 N 9 Dec 2005 0.35 19.80 Hatta Male 21 Nov 2006 1.3 N 9 Jun 2007 0.55

Fig. 1 Ratna engaging in mother infant cannibalism Townsend et al. 2007; marmosets: Bezerra et al. 2007) ina variety of primate species. Individuals may benefit by cannibalizing their victims to gain nutritional and energetic benefits (Fox 1975). However it makes little evolutionary sense for orangutan females to kill their infants, nor is there any evidence that this happened here. It is possible that cannibalism may be a strategy to allow females to regain energy by consuming infants that have died, as cannibalism is more likely to occur under such conditions of low availability of alternative foods, crowding of conspecifics, or fear of starvation (Nishimura and Isoda 2004). However, none of these predicted conditions for cannibalism to occur were taking place, and there was an abundance of fruiting trees at the time of these events. The entire population also has access to two supplementary provisioning sessions per day (generally milk and bananas), hosted by the national park office, in addition to any unscheduled illegal feedings through the tourism industry, with extra food often given to females with infants. It may be possible that females were consuming the carcasses to obtain trace nutrients that would not be available from plant material, though such cannibalistic behavior has never before been seen in any other orangutan population. Meat consumption has been reported among wild orangutans, though infrequently, and is considered to be opportunistic feeding rather than active hunting, upon relatively easy targets (e.g., the greater slow loris, Nycticebus coucang; Utami and Van Hooff 1997). Although this hypothesis cannot be refuted with the data here, this also seems unlikely due to the absence of data of female primates consuming their own infants under natural conditions. Another possibility may be that the females became accustomed to meat eating while in captivity, which resulted in them viewing their infants corpses as a part of their diet. Diet prior to rescue cannot be confirmed, so it cannot be refuted that the animals had consumed meat regularly. However, the typical diet given by those holding orangutans in captivity consists of simple rice and fruit (Utami and Van Hooff 1997). Further, given that wild orangutans and many other primate species consume meat (e.g., chimpanzees, baboons), yet do not commit filial cannibalism, it seems unlikely that the inclusion of meat in the captive diet would impact this. The lack of any previous observations of mother infant cannibalism in any great ape species suggests it unlikely that this is a normal part of their behavioral repertoire. Orangutans have the longest interbirth interval of any primate, and the mother infant bond is very strong (Galdikas and Wood 1990); here, both mothers carried and protected their dead infants, a behavior not atypical for many species of primates (Shopland and Altmann 1987). However for them to have engaged in filial cannibalism, in our opinion, the most plausible explanation is that this is aberrant behavior. Ex-captive orangutans are exposed to considerable traumas and periods of social isolation (Rijksen and Meijaard 1999). Early social deprivation could impair cognitive development (Stoinski and Whiten 2003), thus it is possible the cannibalism is an extension of this. Reintroduced orangutans must substantially alter their behaviors and withdraw from any dependence on humans to be able to return to natural life in the forest (Russon 2002). This can be compromised by maintained contact with humans, which unfortunately is still a regular occurrence in the study site as a result of uncontrolled tourism developments. There is instead accumulating evidence of higher than normal infant (\3 years) mortality: 56% from 1988 to January 2009, amongst offspring born to rehabilitant mothers (Dellatore 2009). This is an extremely high infant mortality rate as compared with that of wild Sumatran orangutans, which has been estimated at a mean range of 0.5 6% occurring over 30 years in the Ketambe forest research station (Singleton et al. 2004). However, in the Sepilok Orangutan Rehabilitation Centre there is also a high rate of infant mortality reported: 57% (Kuze et al. 2008). Potential causes to consider include poor mothering skills that could have resulted in poor nutrition and inadequate care for offspring. Two out of three of Edita s, and

all six of Ratna s offspring have died (Table 1), with average age at death being 1.1 years. Thus it is possible that these rehabilitant orangutans are incompetent in raising offspring. However both mothers exhibited feeding proficiency, with Edita being observed consuming 72 different forest species, and Ratna 42 different species, over the course of this study. Also both were seen to carry and protect their infants before and after death, thus it does not appear they were impassive to their own progeny. Another possibility is disease, though whether naturally occurring or due to human/tourist presence is unknown. As is often the case, cause of death in free-ranging populations such as these remains unknown, as any health problems are often not noted until the effect becomes apparent, so that the cause could be one of any number of factors (Wallis and Lee 1999), all of which is further complicated with difficulties in acquiring the corpse so that a post mortem examination can be conducted. Though it is difficult to determine the absolute cause, in many documented cases there is a strong probable disease transmission link between humans and primates (Quammen 2007). Previous studies have shown that primate populations in close proximity to humans have higher prevalence levels of parasitic infection than those not exposed (reviewed in Wallis and Lee 1999); it is thus possible that these semiwild orangutans, with regular contact with humans and time spent on the ground (Dellatore 2009), are contracting illnesses that prove fatal to their infants. Other populations of reintroduced orangutans have been subject to long-term study, and none have yet reported cannibalism. However, in Bukit Lawang, not only cannibalism, but filial cannibalism, was observed to have occurred twice in a 1-month period, in separate individuals who were not seen to have interacted in the interim period. Thus it seems there are outside causes to consider in this population of orangutans. Mothers cannibalizing their deceased infants has been linked to stressful and artificial environmental conditions in other primates [e.g., galagoes (Tartabini 1991) with only one published case in the wild of filial cannibalism reported in a population of baboons (Goodall 1977)]. The orangutans are experiencing disturbed environmental conditions as the population s ranging behavior is artificially restricted and there is a heavy tourist presence in the area, which has been associated with disruptions in behavior and poor reproductive success (Dellatore 2009). Previous studies in nonhuman primates have shown that infant mortality can be an indicator of the impact of tourism on the population (Berman et al. 2007), thus this may be seen as a serious issue to consider for Bukit Lawang. Orangutans may be particularly sensitive to disturbances during development or within the environment, which may lead to abnormal maternal behavior such as treating offspring as consumable resources. To conclude, we cannot reject the hypotheses that these cases of mother infant cannibalism stemmed from attempts at nutritional gain or past history. Three of the four species of great apes have now been said to engage in cannibalism. Though when considered in the context of comparative behavioral data from other orangutan populations and primate species, environmental disturbance seems the most likely causal factor in these cases of filial cannibalism. However, it is acknowledged that this may be a complex behavior with the underlying causes being multifactorial. With tropical forests shrinking by the day (FWI/GFW 2002), and Sumatran orangutans listed as critically endangered (IUCN 2008), this can be taken as yet further call to retain as many undisturbed, free-living populations in the wild as possible. Acknowledgments We thank LIPI, the Indonesian Department of Science, for permitting this research to take place, Orangutan Health Project/MU 0021622416 the Orang Utan Republik Education Initiative LP Jenkins Memorial Fellowship for providing financial support, and Ian Singleton and an anonymous reviewer for critical discussion and reading of the manuscript. This research was conducted in part fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Primate Conservation at Oxford Brookes University. References Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour: sampling methods. Behaviour 49(3):227 267 Berman CM, Li J, Ogawa H, Ionica C, Yin H (2007) Primate tourism, range restriction and infant risk among Macaca thibetana at Mt Huangshan, China. Int J Primat 28:1 1141 Bezerra BM, Souto ADS, Schiel N (2007) Infanticide and cannibalism in a free-ranging plurally breeding group of common marmosets (Callithrix Jacchus). Am J Primatol 69:945 952 Bygott JD (1972) Cannibalism among wild chimpanzees. Nature 238:410 411 Dellatore DF (2009) Behavioural health of reintroduced orangutans at Bukit Lawang, Sumatra, Indonesia. In: Russell C, Russon A, Wallis J (ed) Primate-focused tourism. Am Soc Primatol (in press) Fairgrieve C (1995) Infanticide and infant eating in the blue monkey (Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni) in the Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda. Folia Primatol 64:69 72 Fossey D (1976) Transcript of the great apes. LSB Leakey Foundation News 6:4 5 Fox LR (1975) Cannibalism in natural populations. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 6:87 106 FWI/GFW (2002) The state of the forest: Indonesia. Forest Watch Indonesia, Washington Galdikas BMF, Wood JW (1990) Birth spacing patterns in humans and apes. Am J Phys Anthropol 83(2):185 191 Goodall J (1977) Infant killing and cannibalism in free-living chimpanzees. Folia Primatol 28:259 282 Hrdy SB (1974) Male male competition and infanticide among the langurs (Presbytis entellus) of Abu, Rajasthan. Folia Primatol 22:19 58 IUCN (2008) 2008 IUCN red list of threatened species Kuze N, Sipangkui S, Malim TP, Bernard H, Ambu LN, Kohshima S (2008) Reproductive parameters over a 37-year period of free-

ranging female Borneo orangutans at Sepilok Orangutan Rehabilitation Centre. Primates 49:126 134 Nishimura K, Isoda Y (2004) Evolution of cannibalism: referring to costs of cannibalism. J Theor Biol 226:291 300 Palombit RA, Cheney DL, Fischer J, Johnson S, Rendall D, Seyfarth RM, Silk JB (2000) Male infanticide and defense of infants in chacma baboons. Infanticide by males and its implications. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 152 Quammen D (2007) Deadly contact: how animals and humans exchange disease, October edn. Nat Geogr Rijksen HD, Meijaard E (1999) Our vanishing relative: the status of wild orangutans at the close of the twentieth century. Kluwer, Dordrecht Rohwer S (1978) Parent cannibalism of offspring and egg raiding as a courtship strategy. Am Nat 112(984):429 440 Russon AE (2002) Return of the native: cognition and site-specific expertise in orangutan rehabilitation. Int J Primatol 23(3):461 478 Shopland JM, Altmann J (1987) Fatal intragroup kidnapping in yellow baboons. Am J Primatol 13:61 65 Singleton I, Wich S, Husson S, Stephens S, Utami Atmoko S, Leighton M, Rosen N, Traylor-Holzer K, Lacy R, Byers O (2004) Orangutan population and habitat viability assessment: final report. IUCN/SSC Conservation Breeding Specialist Group, Apple Valley Stoinski TS, Whiten A (2003) Social learning by orangutans (Pongo abelii and Pongo pygmaeus) in a simulated food-processing task. J Comp Psychol 117(3):272 282 Tartabini A (1991) Mother infant cannibalism in thick-tailed bushbabies (Galago crassicaudatus umbrosus). Primates 32(3):379 383 Townsend SW, Slocombe KE, Emery Thompson M, Zuberbühler K (2007) Female-led infanticide in wild chimpanzees. Curr Biol 17:355 356 Utami SS, Van Hooff J (1997) Meat-Eating by adult female Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii). Am J Primatol 43:159 165 Wallis J, Lee DR (1999) Primate conservation: the prevention of disease transmission. Int J Primatol 20(6):803 826 Watts DP, Mitani JC (2000) Infanticide and cannibalism by male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Primates 41(4):357 365 Xiang ZF, Grueter CC (2007) First direct evidence of infanticide and cannibalism in wild snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus bieti). Am J Primatol 69(3):249 254