the observations. the cloaca that led to a regulating instrument.

Similar documents
Outline. 4. The Ear and the Perception of Sound (Psychoacoustics) A.1 Outer Ear Amplifies Sound. Introduction

B.,tY, 1 ) TrIt p, ow, Temporary Dea(ness in Birds 379 TEMPORARY DEAFNESS IN BIRDS BY CHARLES W. BRAY AND W. R. THURLOW

SUBJECT: Physics TEACHER: Mr. S. Campbell DATE: 15/1/2017 GRADE: DURATION: 1 wk GENERAL TOPIC: The Physics Of Hearing

College of Medicine Dept. of Medical physics Physics of ear and hearing /CH

Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception

Chapter 13 Physics of the Ear and Hearing

INTRODUCTION TO PURE (AUDIOMETER & TESTING ENVIRONMENT) TONE AUDIOMETERY. By Mrs. Wedad Alhudaib with many thanks to Mrs.

COM3502/4502/6502 SPEECH PROCESSING

SOLUTIONS Homework #3. Introduction to Engineering in Medicine and Biology ECEN 1001 Due Tues. 9/30/03

Hearing Sound. The Human Auditory System. The Outer Ear. Music 170: The Ear

Music 170: The Ear. Tamara Smyth, Department of Music, University of California, San Diego (UCSD) November 17, 2016

Auditory System. Barb Rohrer (SEI )

Ear Exam and Hearing Tests

General about Calibration and Service on Audiometers and Impedance Instruments

Auditory Physiology Richard M. Costanzo, Ph.D.

Outline. The ear and perception of sound (Psychoacoustics) A.1 Outer Ear Amplifies Sound. Introduction

Technical Discussion HUSHCORE Acoustical Products & Systems

The Ear. The ear can be divided into three major parts: the outer ear, the middle ear and the inner ear.

Week 2 Systems (& a bit more about db)

AND9020/D. Adaptive Feedback Cancellation 3 from ON Semiconductor APPLICATION NOTE INTRODUCTION

Basic Audiogram Interpretation

Sound and its characteristics. The decibel scale. Structure and function of the ear. Békésy s theory. Molecular basis of hair cell function.

Glossary For Parents. Atresia: closure of the ear canal or absence of an ear opening.

Hearing. istockphoto/thinkstock

3M Center for Hearing Conservation

Discrete Signal Processing

Ganglion Cells Blind Spot Cornea Pupil Visual Area of the Bipolar Cells Thalamus Rods and Cones Lens Visual cortex of the occipital lobe

Auditory Physiology PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 29. Hearing

PSY 310: Sensory and Perceptual Processes 1

Before taking field measurements, it is important to determine the type of information required. The person making the measurement must understand:

Hearing Aids. Bernycia Askew

Receptors / physiology

IN THE UNITED STATES PATENT AND TRADEMARK OFFICE UTILITY APPLICATION FOR UNITED STATES PATENT FOR

A surgical approach for a cochlear implant: An anatomical study

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Hearing. PSYCHOLOGY (8th Edition, in Modules) David Myers. Module 14. Hearing. Hearing

Hearing. and other senses

PERIPHERAL AND CENTRAL AUDITORY ASSESSMENT

Anatomy of the ear: Lymphatics

Unit VIII Problem 9 Physiology: Hearing

Health, Safety, Security and Environment

to vibrate the fluid. The ossicles amplify the pressure. The surface area of the oval window is

FM SYSTEMS. with the FONIX 6500-CX Hearing Aid Analyzer. (Requires software version 4.20 or above) FRYE ELECTRONICS, INC.

Noise has been defined as a sound without agreeable quality or as unwanted or undesirable sound

Pediatric Hearing Screening Training for the PCA. Gouverneur Healthcare Services 227 Madison Street New York, NY 10002

Music and Hearing in the Older Population: an Audiologist's Perspective

Two Modified IEC Ear Simulators for Extended Dynamic Range

i5)i / /. / AN ANALYTICAL STUDY OF ELECTRIC RESPONSES AT THE PERIPHERY OF THE AUDITORY SYSTEM MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY

HEARING CONSERVATION PROGRAM

Systems Neuroscience Oct. 16, Auditory system. http:

Suitcase Lab Measurement of Digital Cellphone Interference Levels on Hearing Aids

Work Design and Industrial Ergonomics - Ear and Noise

Implementation and analysis of international standard for electroacoustic performance evaluation of hearing aids

UNDERSTANDING HEARING LOSS

UNDERSTANDING HEARING LOSS

THE EAR AND HEARING Be sure you have read and understand Chapter 16 before beginning this lab. INTRODUCTION: hair cells outer ear tympanic membrane

Advanced otoacoustic emission detection techniques and clinical diagnostics applications

Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Week 1. Laboratory session: Measuring thresholds

Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology. Psychoacoustics of hearing impairment

Noise at Work Regulations. Mick Gray MRSC, LFOH, ROH. MWG Associates Ltd

Auditory System & Hearing

Auditory System Feedback

Educational Module Tympanometry. Germany D Germering

Auditory System & Hearing

A Novel Software Solution to Diagnose the Hearing Disabilities In Human Beings

Hearing and Balance 1

Hearing: Physiology and Psychoacoustics

PSY 214 Lecture # (11/9/2011) (Sound, Auditory & Speech Perception) Dr. Achtman PSY 214

PC BASED AUDIOMETER GENERATING AUDIOGRAM TO ASSESS ACOUSTIC THRESHOLD

au/images/conductive-loss-new.jpg

Organisation Internationale de Métrologie Légale

Contents. Exercises and Questions. Answers

INTRODUCTION TO AUDIOLOGY Hearing Balance Tinnitus - Treatment

Loudness. Loudness is not simply sound intensity!

THE MECHANICS OF HEARING

Cochlear Implant The only hope for severely Deaf

OIML R 122 Annex C RECOMMENDATION. Edition 1999 (E) ORGANISATION INTERNATIONALE INTERNATIONAL ORGANIZATION

Anatomy and Physiology of Hearing

MEDICAL RESEARCH LABORATORY

Importance of a Good Start. Topics. Rationale. In-Situ Audiometry. In-Situ Audiometry. Enhancing the Initial

MECHANISM OF HEARING

Adaptive Feedback Cancellation for the RHYTHM R3920 from ON Semiconductor

NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL CENTER NAVAL SUBMARINE BASE NEW LONDON GROTON, CONNECTICUT MEMORANDUM REPORT 70-2

Audiological Bulletin no. 46

The following describes general use of ELIOS device

Brad May, PhD Johns Hopkins University

Testing FM Systems on the 7000 Hearing Aid Test System

Underwater hearing in California sea lions (Zalophus californianus): Expansion and interpretation of existing data

Impact of the ambient sound level on the system's measurements CAPA

Testing FM Systems with FONIX FP40 Analyzers

HEARING IMPAIRMENT LEARNING OBJECTIVES: Divisions of the Ear. Inner Ear. The inner ear consists of: Cochlea Vestibular

Hearing Evaluation: Diagnostic Approach

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1

Technical Report: Distortion Product Otoacoustic Emissions That Are Not Outer Hair Cell Emissions DOI: /jaaa

Sound Workshop. What is sound Longitudinal Waves Frequency and pitch Hearing ranges Sounds in solids, liquids and gases Sound in a vacuum

ACTIVITY USING RATS A METHOD FOR THE EVALUATION OF ANALGESIC. subject and a variety of stimuli employed. In the examination of new compounds

Hearing. Figure 1. The human ear (from Kessel and Kardon, 1979)

HEARING GUIDE PREPARED FOR CLINICAL PROFESSIONALS HEARING.HEALTH.MIL. HCE_ClinicalProvider-Flip_FINAL01.indb 1

NIDCD NATIONAL TEMPORAL BONE, HEARING AND BALANCE PATHOLOGY RESOURCE REGISTRY

Learning Targets. Module 20. Hearing Explain how the ear transforms sound energy into neural messages.

Transcription:

HEARING IN THE BLACKFOOTED PENGUIN, SPHENISCUS DEMERSUS, AS REPRESENTED BY THE COCHLEAR POTENTIALS* BY ERNEST GLEN WEVER, PAUL N. HERMAN, JAMES A. SIMMONS, AND DAVID R. HERTZLER AUDITORY RESEARCH LABORATORIES, PRINCETON UNIVERSITY Communicated May 14, 1969 Abstract.-A study of the hearing of the penguin Sphenikcus demersus, in terms of the cochlear potentials, showed sensitivity over a range at least of 1 to 15, Hz, with the best sensitivity in the region of 6 to 4 Hz. The form of the sensitivity function is consistent with the vocalizations of these animals. In general, this species of penguin shows good agreement with other birds in both the form and range of auditory sensitivity. The auditory capabilities of one species of penguin, Spheniscus demermus, commonly called the Blackfooted or Jackass Penguin, were investigated by means of the cochlear potentials. This is one of the smaller penguin species, breeding on islands off the western coast of South Africa. Three animals were used for the observations. The animals were anesthetized with ethyl carbamate, administered intraperitoneally in a 2 per cent solution at a dosage of.1 cc per gram of body weight. During the experiments the animal rested on a heating blanket, and a body temperature of 38C was maintained by means of a temperature probe in the cloaca that led to a regulating instrument. The middle ear cavity was exposed by a lateral approach, as indicated in Figure 1. The right side of the head is shown, with the region of the skin incision indicated. The removal of skin and muscle tissue in this region exposed the posterv -, X 7 External auditory meatus Line of incision Drilled opening bon '-ngular bone FIG. 1.-Outline drawing of the head of Spheniscus demersus, seen from' the right side. The region of the incision is indicated. Inset: the area exposed by removal of skin and muscle tissues. 676

VOL. 63, 1969 PSYCHOLOGY: WEVER ET AL. 677 olateral surface of the exoccipital bone, as shown in the inset at the lower right of the figure. A hole drilled in this bone exposed the middle ear cavity. Figure 2 gives the view through this opening from a posterior direction and shows the oval window with the stapedial end of the columella largely covering it and the columellar shaft extending forward and laterally. The round window lies lateral and ventral to the oval window, with an interfenestral process separating the two. An active electrode, consisting of a wire tipped with a silver bead of.3-mm diameter, was placed in contact with the round-window membrane. Two other electrodes, located in inactive tissues in the same region, provided a differential recording of the round window potentials. Oval window \Columnella Interfenestral process Round window Exoccipital bone FIG. 2.-View of the middle ear cavity, obtained by drilling a hole in the lateral surface of the exoccipital bone. The preparation was located in a soundproofed and electrically shielded chamber, and sounds from a loudspeaker were conveyed through the wall of the chamber by a tube with its end sealed over the external ear opening. Running concentrically with the terminal portion of this sound tube was a probe tube that led to a condenser microphone, and the probe tube and microphone system were calibrated to indicate sound pressures at the terminus of the tube and hence at the entrance to the external auditory meatus. All the observations were made on the right ear. Sounds were produced by a dynamic type of loudspeaker (Western Electric type 555) and covered a range of 1 to 15, cycles per second (Hz). Tones below 1 Hz were not employed because of the high level of physiological noise for such tones, and tones above 15, Hz were avoided because the required intensities involved the risk of damage to the ear. A tone synthesizer, used to generate alternating currents over this range, gave precise determination of frequencies (within 1 cycle). These currents were passed through a variable attenuator to a power amplifier and then through a second variable attenuator to the loudspeaker. The two attenuators were controlled with a special switch that included a totalizing mechanism with a numerical display; this device afforded

678 PSYCHOLOGY: WEVER ET AL. PROC. N. A. S. rapid setting of tonal intensity and eliminated the human error of reading and summing a number of dials. Figure 3 gives the sensitivity curves for the three animals. These curves show on the ordinate, for a number of tones, the sound pressures in decibels relative to a level of 1 dyne/sq cm required to produce a standard response of.1 sav at the round window. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 891 2 3 4 5 6 7 891 +2 T TT T 1 - -o i-2-4, - 6 8 I 1 1 1, Frequency FIG. 3.-Sensitivity curves for three specimens of the Blackfooted penguin Spheniscus demersus. Shown is the sound pressure, in decibels relative to 1 dyne/sq cm, necessary to produce a cocblear potential of.1 uv. The three functions are in good agreement over most of the frequency range, but one of them shows significantly keener sensitivity in the region from 15 to 4 Hz. For all three ears the range from 6 to 4 or 5 Hz seems to show the best auditory acuity. Above this range the acuity falls off precipitously. The observations at 15, Hz are not indicated on the graph, but their points fall precisely on the projected curves. The rate of decline of sensitivity beyond 4 Hz is about 6 db per octave. In two of the animals intensity functions were obtained by observing the varying output while increasing the sound level in steps of 5 db. Three of these functions for one of the animals are given in Figure 4. As has been observed in a great variety of animals, the electrical output of the ear is a linear function of sound pressure (except as it may be disturbed by noise) at low and intermediate levels and becomes nonlinear at high levels. We see here a good approximation to linearity in the lower portion of the curves, and then a departure from linearity that is at first slight and increases progressively. In these tests the sound intensity was not carried high enough to produce the flattening and then the decline of the function that are often seen, since we wished to avoid the damage to the ear that may result from a very high level of stimulation. In a single instance, for

VOL. 63, 1969 PSYCHOLOGY: WEVER ET AL. 679 one of the animals, a tone of 1 Hz was carried to a level of 1 dynes/sq cm, at which the curve began to bend downward. At the end of these experiments the ears were perfused in preparation for a histological study of details of the structures of the middle and inner ear. Our animals produced vocalizations that are best described as grunts and brayings. To our ears the sounds seemed predominantly low-pitched. Tape recordings made on other individuals of this same species by Dr. P. P. Kellogg of the Laboratory of Ornithology of Cornell University were studied by means of a sound spectrograph. This analysis showed, in line with our preceptions, that the principal energy of the vocalizations lies below 2 Hz. It was somewhat surprising, however, to find that there is appreciable energy in the range above 2 Hz all the way to 6 Hz and a little beyond. The spectrographs showed further that the "grunt" and the "bray" have about the same frequency composition and differ only in duration. Another component of the "bray," which is the inspiration phase following the prolonged expiration phase of 1.5-1.7 sec, mainly contains frequencies between 12 and 17 Hz, with a lesser admixture of frequencies around 3 Hz. The vocalizations reported here are probably not the only ones of which these birds are capable; they are the ones that were readily noticed and that could easily be recorded. There is reasonable agreement between the frequency pattern of these vocalizations and the region of best sensitivity of the ears as measured by cochlear potentials. 1~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~. E FIG. 4.-Intensity functions for a specimen of Spheniscus demersus..1 Sound pressure, dynes per sq cm

68 PSYCHOLOGY: WEVER ET AL. PROC. N. A. S. The form of the sensitivity function as observed for these penguins is surprisingly similar to what we know about the auditory sensitivity of other bird species. Wever and Bray in 1936 used the cochlear potential method to study the auditory sensitivity of the pigeon and found maximum sensitivity at 3 Hz.' In 1948, Schwartzkopff determined the threshold sensitivity of bullfinches (Pyrrhula p. minor-brehm) by a training method, and found the best sensitivity at 32 Hz.2 In a summary of observations on a number of different species of birds, Schwartzkopff showed that for most the best sensitivity lies in the region of 1-3 Hz, although in a few-especially the owls-it lies appreciably higher.3 Our penguins therefore show good agreement with the smaller birds in their region of best sensitivity. They show good agreement also in their upper frequency range. In general, birds appear to have upper limits of frequency around 12, to 2, Hz, though a few may go a little higher-to as much as 25, Hz. In this respect it is notable that birds are inferior to most of the mammals whose heaxing has been suitably investigated in the high frequencies. Many of these hear tones up to 5, Hz and some as far as 1,. Our penguins were so poorly responsive at 15, Hz that we did not wish to run the risk of damage to their ears by testing at still higher frequencies, and we consider that 15, Hz may be regarded as their practical limit. We express our thanks to Dr. P. P. Kellogg of the Laboratory of Ornithology, Cornell University, for the use of tape recordings of penguin vocalizations. * From the Department of Psychology. This investigation was supported by grants from the National Institute of Neurological Diseases and Stroke, U.S. Public Health Service, and was also aided by a contract with the Office of Naval Research and by Higgins funds allotted to Princeton University. 1 Wever, E. G., and C. W. Bray, "Hearing in the pigeon as studied by the electrical responses of the inner ear," J. Comp. Psychol., 22, 353-363 (1936). 2 Schwartzkopff, J., "Die Horschwellen des Dompfaffen," Naturwissenschaften, 35, 287 (1948). 3 Schwartzkopff, J., "Uber die Gehorsinn der Vogel," J. Ornithol., 91-13 (1952).