Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

Similar documents
21/01/2013. Binaural Phenomena. Aim. To understand binaural hearing Objectives. Understand the cues used to determine the location of a sound source

Sound localization psychophysics

3-D Sound and Spatial Audio. What do these terms mean?

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening

Binaural Hearing. Steve Colburn Boston University

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 THE DUPLEX-THEORY OF LOCALIZATION INVESTIGATED UNDER NATURAL CONDITIONS

HST.723J, Spring 2005 Theme 3 Report

Juha Merimaa b) Institut für Kommunikationsakustik, Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Germany

3-D SOUND IMAGE LOCALIZATION BY INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES AND THE MEDIAN PLANE HRTF. Masayuki Morimoto Motokuni Itoh Kazuhiro Iida

Publication VI. c 2007 Audio Engineering Society. Reprinted with permission.

Localization in the Presence of a Distracter and Reverberation in the Frontal Horizontal Plane. I. Psychoacoustical Data

Lecture 8: Spatial sound

William A. Yost and Sandra J. Guzman Parmly Hearing Institute, Loyola University Chicago, Chicago, Illinois 60201

Hearing II Perceptual Aspects

IN EAR TO OUT THERE: A MAGNITUDE BASED PARAMETERIZATION SCHEME FOR SOUND SOURCE EXTERNALIZATION. Griffin D. Romigh, Brian D. Simpson, Nandini Iyer

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization

P. M. Zurek Sensimetrics Corporation, Somerville, Massachusetts and Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02139

Localization in the Presence of a Distracter and Reverberation in the Frontal Horizontal Plane: II. Model Algorithms

Systems Neuroscience Oct. 16, Auditory system. http:

Effects of Age and Hearing Loss on the Processing of Auditory Temporal Fine Structure

Effect of spectral content and learning on auditory distance perception

What Is the Difference between db HL and db SPL?

Binaural processing of complex stimuli

Combination of binaural and harmonic. masking release effects in the detection of a. single component in complex tones

Physiological measures of the precedence effect and spatial release from masking in the cat inferior colliculus.

HEARING AND PSYCHOACOUSTICS

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100 (4), Pt. 1, October /96/100(4)/2352/13/$ Acoustical Society of America 2352

Sound Localization PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 31. Audition

Neural correlates of the perception of sound source separation

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080

Localization 103: Training BiCROS/CROS Wearers for Left-Right Localization

Sound Localization in Multisource Environments: The Role of Stimulus Onset Asynchrony and Spatial Uncertainty

Brian D. Simpson Veridian, 5200 Springfield Pike, Suite 200, Dayton, Ohio 45431

Speech segregation in rooms: Effects of reverberation on both target and interferer

Minimum Audible Angles Measured with Simulated Normally-Sized and Oversized Pinnas for Normal-Hearing and Hearing- Impaired Test Subjects

Effect of microphone position in hearing instruments on binaural masking level differences

The Effects of Hearing Loss on Binaural Function

Trading Directional Accuracy for Realism in a Virtual Auditory Display

A NOVEL HEAD-RELATED TRANSFER FUNCTION MODEL BASED ON SPECTRAL AND INTERAURAL DIFFERENCE CUES

The use of interaural time and level difference cues by bilateral cochlear implant users

Hearing in the Environment

Binaural detection with narrowband and wideband reproducible noise maskers. IV. Models using interaural time, level, and envelope differences

Effect of mismatched place-of-stimulation on the salience of binaural cues in conditions that simulate bilateral cochlear-implant listening

PERIPHERAL AND CENTRAL AUDITORY ASSESSMENT

B. G. Shinn-Cunningham Hearing Research Center, Departments of Biomedical Engineering and Cognitive and Neural Systems, Boston, Massachusetts 02215

Neural Recording Methods

This will be accomplished using maximum likelihood estimation based on interaural level

Discrimination and identification of azimuth using spectral shape a)

Abstract. 1. Introduction. David Spargo 1, William L. Martens 2, and Densil Cabrera 3

J Jeffress model, 3, 66ff

The extent to which a position-based explanation accounts for binaural release from informational masking a)

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

Relating binaural pitch perception to the individual listener s auditory profile

Review Auditory Processing of Interaural Timing Information: New Insights

The role of spatial cues for processing speech in noise

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Acoust Soc Am. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 April 29.

Charactering the individual ear by the Auditory Profile

J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 116 (2), August /2004/116(2)/1057/9/$ Acoustical Society of America

Modeling Physiological and Psychophysical Responses to Precedence Effect Stimuli

Dynamic-range compression affects the lateral position of sounds

On the influence of interaural differences on onset detection in auditory object formation. 1 Introduction

The neural code for interaural time difference in human auditory cortex

Spatial processing in adults with hearing loss

Neural System Model of Human Sound Localization

Lecture 3: Perception

Study of perceptual balance for binaural dichotic presentation

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

I. INTRODUCTION. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 112 (3), Pt. 1, Sep /2002/112(3)/1037/9/$ Acoustical Society of America

Group Delay or Processing Delay

ABSTRACT. Figure 1. Stimulus configurations used in discrimination studies of the precedence effect.

Auditory System & Hearing

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Binaural interference and auditory grouping

Audibility of time differences in adjacent head-related transfer functions (HRTFs) Hoffmann, Pablo Francisco F.; Møller, Henrik

Cochlear implant patients localization using interaural level differences exceeds that of untrained normal hearing listeners

J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 114 (2), August /2003/114(2)/1009/14/$ Acoustical Society of America

Human Sensitivity to Interaural Phase Difference for Very Low Frequency Sound

Effect of Reverberation Context on Spatial Hearing Performance of Normally Hearing Listeners

Effect of source spectrum on sound localization in an everyday reverberant room

Modeling HRTF For Sound Localization in Normal Listeners and Bilateral Cochlear Implant Users

THE PHYSICAL AND PSYCHOPHYSICAL BASIS OF SOUND LOCALIZATION

Relating binaural pitch perception to the individual listener's auditory profile

Archives LIBRARP.! BINAURAL INTERACTION IN HEARING IMPAIRED LISTENERS. Kaigham Jacob Gabriel 1. B.S. University of Pittsburgh ( 1977 )

Thresholds for different mammals

The perception of speech in noise with bilateral and bimodal hearing devices

An Auditory System Modeling in Sound Source Localization

Study of sound localization by owls and its relevance to humans

9/29/14. Amanda M. Lauer, Dept. of Otolaryngology- HNS. From Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics, Green & Swets (1966)

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Ear Hear. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 March 01.

16.400/453J Human Factors Engineering /453. Audition. Prof. D. C. Chandra Lecture 14

I. INTRODUCTION. NL-5656 AA Eindhoven, The Netherlands. Electronic mail:

Temporal binding of auditory spatial information across dynamic binaural events

Spatial hearing and sound localization mechanisms in the brain. Henri Pöntynen February 9, 2016

Temporal weighting of interaural time and level differences in high-rate click trains

Literature Overview - Digital Hearing Aids and Group Delay - HADF, June 2017, P. Derleth

Representation of sound in the auditory nerve

Neural Correlates and Mechanisms of Spatial Release From Masking: Single-Unit and Population Responses in the Inferior Colliculus

Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Week 5. The peripheral auditory system: The ear as a signal processor

Localization of sound in rooms. V. Binaural coherence and human sensitivity to interaural time differences in noise

Transcription:

Binaural Hearing Why two ears? Locating sounds in space: acuity is poorer than in vision by up to two orders of magnitude, but extends in all directions. Role in alerting and orienting? Separating sound sources: signals from two receivers can be compared and operated on to enhance the reception of one sound source over another. The same applies to signals in a reverberant environment. Definitions Monaural: Sound presented to one ear Binaural: Sound presented to both ears Diotic: Sounds identical at the two ears Dichotic: Sounds different at the two ears (see Blauert, 1997)

Spatial cues Two primary cues for determining the azimuth of a sound: a) Interaural time differences (ITDs) Left Right c 340 m/s; max. path difference 0.23 m Max. natural ITD 680 µs. For sinusoids, this can be expressed as a phase difference. As the maximum delay approaches a lag of 180, the phase difference becomes ambiguous. This occurs for f > 1.5 khz. Leading or lagging? b) Interaural intensity differences (IIDs also known as ILDs). The head starts to create an acoustic shadow at frequencies above about 500 Hz. The effect is most apparent above 2 khz.

Sound at the two ears Interaural time differences Interaural level differences See Moore, B.C.J (1997). An introduction to the psychology of hearing. 4th edition. Academic: London.

Localization of sinusoids in the horizontal plane Duplex theory (Rayleigh, 1907): f < 1500 Hz: ITDs dominant (ILDs are small) f > 1500 Hz: ILDs dominant (ITDs cannot be perceived and would be ambiguous) Stevens and Newman (1936): Localization worst around 2-3 khz. Front-back reversals common, especially below 2 khz. Can be explained by the cone of confusion. See Moore, B.C.J (1997). An introduction to the psychology of hearing. 4th edition. Academic: London.

Minimum Audible Angles (MAA) for sinusoids, first measured by Mills (1958). Points to note: 1 corresponds to an ITD of about 10 µs. (!) MAA is a function of frequency and reference azimuth. The dip in performance around 1.5 khz may be explained in terms of the duplex theory: neither mechanism is operating efficiently in that region. Measurements could not be made with the reference at 90 at any frequency: Due to cone of confusion.

Lateralization of sinusoids ITDs and ILDs can be manipulated independently when sounds are presented over headphones, but the sound generally appears to come from inside the head. Lowest JNDs: 10 µs ITD; 0.5-1 db ILD. Localization: Free field Lateralization: Headphones No effect of ITD on lateralization is observed above about 1.5 khz, if sound is gated on and off slowly. Note that lateralization occurs for a 600-Hz tone, even though large ILDs do not occur naturally at this frequency. See Blauert (1997)

Lateralization of complex sounds ITDs much greater than found naturally can be discriminated and can produce lateralization. See Blauert (1997) Role of envelope fluctuations ITDs at high frequencies can be discriminated, and do cause lateralization, in complex sounds. Henning (1974): ITD discrimination of a 3900-Hz tone, amplitude modulated with a 300-Hz modulator, was as good as ITD discrimination of a 300-Hz tone. Listeners were not sensitive to the phase (delay) of the carrier; even slightly different carrier frequencies produced lateralization. Ongoing ITDs in the stimulus envelope can be detected at high frequencies.

Time-intensity trades in lateralization Can an ITD and an ILD cancel each other out? See Blauert (1997)

Possible underlying mechanisms Jeffress (1948) coincidence counter to account for ITD sensitivity: Neural spatial map. For tones, peaks occur at multiples of the period, leading to possible ambiguities. These are resolved with wide-band stimuli. See Stern and Trahiotis (1995)

Single-mechanism hypotheses Ability to trade time and intensity differences led to one-mechanism proposals: Level-dependent latency: Increased level leads to decreased neural latency, transforming ILD to ITD. Neural inhibition: Earlier signal leads to inhibition in contralateral pathways, transforming ITD to ILD. Evidence against single-mechanisms hypotheses: ITDs cannot compensate for ILDs of 25 db or more. Time-intensity trades do not evoke the same percept as a diotic stimulus: image is more diffuse or may separate into two separate images. Sensitivity to ongoing ITDs in envelope cannot be explained by simple inhibition.

Resolving the Cone of Confusion: Hearing Height The cone of confusion can be resolved using head movements. But, for broadband sounds, position in the median plane can sometimes be determined even without head movements. Due primarily to direction-dependent filtering effects of the head and pinnae. The filter between the source and the eardrum is referred to as the Head-Related Transfer Function (HRTF). See Blauert (1997) A realistic simulated HRTF can result in externalization of sounds over headphones. Important for virtual reality applications.

Listening in rooms: Echo suppression and the precedence effect In many environments, the direct path of a sound to our ears is only one of many. Yet we mostly hear only one source at a location usually corresponding to the that of the source. 2 1 This situation can be modeled using 2 loudspeakers (at 1 & 2) Three time regions identified (times valid for clicks): 1) Summing localization (< 1 ms delay): two events fused: perceived location is a weighted sum of the two. 2) Precedence effect (ca. 1-5 ms delay): Only one sound perceived: direction of first sound dominant. 3) Echo threshold (> 5 ms delay): Two sounds heard.

Build-up and Breakdown of the Precedence Effect The lagging click of a click pair with a delay of 8 ms is initially clearly heard. However, after a few repetitions (say, 4 per second), the lagging click is no longer perceived as a separate event (Freyman et al., 1991). The auditory system seems to have adjusted to the new acoustic environment. Changing the delay, direction, or spectrum of a lagging click in a series will suddenly render it audible. After a few more repetitions, it will disappear again (Clifton, 1987). The auditory system is alerted to a change in acoustic environment. (The precedence effect, or law of first wavefront is used in the design of PA systems to make the amplified sound appear to come from the source.)

Binaural Masking Level Differences BMLDs can be as much as 15 db for a tone in broadband noise, and even more for a tone in narrowband noise. The effect is robust and can also be demonstrated for speech in noise. See Moore, B.C.J (1997). An introduction to the psychology of hearing. 4th edition. Academic: London.

Motion Perception Binaural system is often referred to as being sluggish. Using continuously varying ITDs or ILDs, Blauert (1972) found the maximum rate at which movement could be followed in detail was less than 3 Hz. Above about 10-20 Hz, no movement is perceived at all just a steady blur (Grantham & Wightman, 1979). Similar time constants govern BMLDs in a changing environment (Kollmeier & Gilkey, 1990):