Dimitris Pnevmatikos a a University of Western Macedonia, Greece. Published online: 13 Nov 2014.

Similar documents
Costanza Scaffidi Abbate a b, Stefano Ruggieri b & Stefano Boca a a University of Palermo

Back-Calculation of Fish Length from Scales: Empirical Comparison of Proportional Methods

To link to this article:

To link to this article:

Anne A. Lawrence M.D. PhD a a Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada Published online: 11 Jan 2010.

Published online: 17 Feb 2011.

Lora-Jean Collett a & David Lester a a Department of Psychology, Wellesley College and

Richard Lakeman a a School of Health & Human Sciences, Southern Cross University, Lismore, Australia. Published online: 02 Sep 2013.

Wild Minds What Animals Really Think : A Museum Exhibit at the New York Hall of Science, December 2011

NANCY FUGATE WOODS a a University of Washington

Laura N. Young a & Sara Cordes a a Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

Marie Stievenart a, Marta Casonato b, Ana Muntean c & Rens van de Schoot d e a Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Universite

Cognitive Enhancement Using 19-Electrode Z-Score Neurofeedback

Advanced Projects R&D, New Zealand b Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Online publication date: 30 March 2011

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Les McFarling a, Michael D'Angelo a, Marsha Drain a, Deborah A. Gibbs b & Kristine L. Rae Olmsted b a U.S. Army Center for Substance Abuse Programs,

Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA b University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA

A Project to Clone Companion Animals Mark Greene Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Online publication date: 08 June 2010

EHPS 2012 abstracts. To cite this article: (2012): EHPS 2012 abstracts, Psychology & Health, 27:sup1, 1-357

The Flynn effect and memory function Sallie Baxendale ab a

EILEEN K. McMILLEN a, HERBERT W. HELM JR. a & DUANE C. McBRIDE a a Department of Behavioral Sciences, Andrews

Life Events, Social Support, and Depression Among Taiwanese Female Homemakers

Published online: 14 Dec 2007.

9SA, UK Published online: 04 Jan 2008.

Randy A. Sansone a b & Michael W. Wiederman c a Departments of Psychiatry and Internal Medicine, Wright State

Key Knowledge Generation Publication details, including instructions for author and Subscription information:

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

André L. Souza a & Cristine H. Legare a Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, Available online: 17 Aug 2011

Published online: 11 Sep 2012.

University of the West Indies, Kingston, Jamaica PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

To link to this article:

Available online: 09 Jun 2011

Version of record first published: 25 Apr 2012.

Autogynephilia in Women

Kelantan, Malaysia b Department of Agribusiness and Information Systems, Universiti. Putra Malaysia, Selangor, Malaysia Published online: 03 Mar 2015.

Please complete the medical history section below so that we can be sure to respond to any

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Andrea W. Clay a b, Mollie A. Bloomsmith a b, M. Jackson Marr b & Terry L. Maple b a Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory

Time Perspective Therapy: A New Time-Based Metaphor Therapy for PTSD

Moneyball: The Art of Winning the American Dental Association Membership Renewal Game

Lee N. Johnson a, Scott A. Ketring b & Shayne R. Anderson c a Department of Child and Family Development, University of

Chapter 10. Published online: 08 Oct 2008.

What happens if I cannot make decisions about my care and treatment?

Influencing young adolescents motivation in the lowest level of secondary education

MEMBER SHARE A Pastoral Medical Association - Private Membership Program MEMBER SHARE AGREEMENT (MSA)

Advance Statements. What is an Advance Statement? Information Line: Website: compassionindying.org.uk

To link to this article:

Psychological Approach to Comparative Education Aneela Farooq Afshan Nisar

hij Teacher Resource Bank

The interplay of domain-specific and domain general processes, skills and abilities in the development of science knowledge

PSK409-Health Psychology

Memory Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:

Kyle Richard Stephenson a & Cindy M. Meston a a The University of Texas at Austin, Psychology, Austin, Texas, USA

Guelph, Guelph, Ontario, Canada c Department of Animal Science, Faculty of Agricultural Science, University of Guilan,

Small Group Facilitator s Guide Doctoring 101 The ETHNICS Mnemonic

South Australia, Adelaide, Australia Published online: 15 Jun To link to this article:

Ted H. Shore a, Armen Tashchian b & Janet S. Adams c. Kennesaw State University b Department of Marketing and Entrepreneurship,

Certificate in the Principles of End of Life Care

APPLICATION INSTRUCTIONS

BEST PRACTICES FOR PLANNING A VACCINATION CAMPAIGN FOR AN ENTIRE POPULATION

Measurement invariance of self-continuity strategies: Comparisons of early adolescents from Brazil, Canada and Colombia

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

Eliminative materialism

*To reserve your place in the training, you must submit the completed application along with a minimum

Learn how to more effectively communicate with others. This will be a fun and informative workshop! Sponsored by

BEST PRACTICES IN MICROPLANNING FOR CHILDREN OUT OF THE HOUSEHOLD: AN EXAMPLE FROM NORTHERN NIGERIA

Department of Psychology, University of Maryland at College Park, b Department of Psychology, Texas Tech University,

Ziasma Haneef Khan a, P. J. Watson b & Zhuo Chen c a Department of Psychology, University of Karachi, University

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Supporting the grieving student

Testing Lesniaski s Revised Brief Test

The Amount of Practice Really Matters

Andrew Hyland a, David Bradford PhD b & David Bradford PhD b a the Roswell Park Cancer Institute

Magalhães, Porto, Portugal

3: (Semi-)Structured Interviews. Tina FREYBURG [Introduction to Qualitative Methods]

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Jesus said to him, I am the way and the truth and the life John 14:6

Christian H. Nevill a, Ted H. Friend b & Amy G. Windom b a Southwest Foundation for Biomedical Research,

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

Kirsten T. Verkooijen a, Pepijn van Hove b & Giel Dik a a Wageningen University. Available online: 27 Dec 2011

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age

Neuropsychological. Justin D. Lane a & David L. Gast a a Department of Special Education, The University of

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE

Classification without identification in visual search Joan Brand a a

To paraphrase the foreword of the First Edition of the Big Book:

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

International Standard on Auditing (UK) 530

Cage Toys Reduce Abnormal Behavior in Individually Housed Pigtail Macaques Amy L. Kessel & Linda Brent Published online: 04 Jun 2010.

Benefits and constraints of qualitative and quantitative research methods in economics and management science

Chapter 12: Talking to Patients and Caregivers

Group Assignment #1: Concept Explication. For each concept, ask and answer the questions before your literature search.

Optimal Health Questionnaire

Steve Barns & Associates The Counseling Center of Denton Bible Church Christian Counseling Services Individual, Marriage, & Family

Sense-making Approach in Determining Health Situation, Information Seeking and Usage

The Good, the Bad and the Blameworthy: Understanding the Role of Evaluative Reasoning in Folk Psychology. Princeton University

Please take time to read this document carefully. It forms part of the agreement between you and your counsellor and Insight Counselling.

Module 3 - Scientific Method

Transcription:

This article was downloaded by: [Dimitrios Pnevmatikos] On: 14 November 2014, At: 22:15 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Journal of Beliefs & Values: Studies in Religion & Education Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/cjbv20 Prayer as a cause of recovery from illness Dimitris Pnevmatikos a a University of Western Macedonia, Greece Published online: 13 Nov 2014. To cite this article: Dimitris Pnevmatikos (2014) Prayer as a cause of recovery from illness, Journal of Beliefs & Values: Studies in Religion & Education, 35:2, 218-221, DOI: 10.1080/13617672.2014.953304 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13617672.2014.953304 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the Content ) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/termsand-conditions

Journal of Beliefs & Values, 2014 Vol. 35, No. 2, 218 221, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13617672.2014.953304 RESEARCH REPORT Prayer as a cause of recovery from illness Dimitris Pnevmatikos* University of Western Macedonia, Greece This research aimed to examine the developmental trajectory of the beliefs about the power of prayer to aide recovery from illness. One hundred and sixty children and young adults divided in four age groups participated in the study. Participants were interviewed with vignettes presenting two sick individuals who differed in terms of prayers that they received from their friends in order to recover from their illness. The results showed that children and pre-adolescents strongly believe in the power of prayer to recover from illness while only about half of the university students share this belief. Thus, contrary to the framework theory approach, children, adolescents, and young adults did not replace these beliefs (as naive explanations), with the scientific explanations. Instead, they continue to hold these beliefs in parallel to their scientific explanations. Keywords: framework theory; prayer; recovery from illness; religious beliefs Introduction The explanations of get sick and get well are highly correlated (Sayer, Willett, and Perrin 1993). Nevertheless, although there is enough evidence about the content of the psycho-social and the biological explanatory framework of illness, we know little about the content of these frameworks concerning the recovery from illness. Despite the importance of the recovery from illness for the formulation of a theory of illness, this aspect has been rarely investigated when scholars study the development of the explanatory framework theory of illness among pre-schoolers and children. Moreover, little is known about the causal explanations that individuals have for recovering from illness within the religious belief system. For instance, individuals usually pray, seeking God s help to maintain their health (for a review see Spilka and Ladd 2013, 109). Woolley and Phelps (2001), investigating the development of three- to eightyear-old children s beliefs about prayer, included questions about the power of praying for someone else to recover from illness. They found that the vast majority of the children (94%) endorsed as acceptable this prayer and almost all expected it to be fulfilled (98%). Even the four-years-old acknowledged that God is the necessary component of the prayer process. However, Woolley and Phelps (2001) did not inform us whether these beliefs continue to exist in the course of the development. This information is crucial for understanding how children develop their causality about illness. Carey (1985) suggested that when 10-year-olds formulate a coherent framework theory of illness, interpreting illness from the perspective of the germ theory, they abandon their previously held naive explanatory framework and replace *Email: dpnevma@gmail.com 2014 Taylor & Francis

Journal of Beliefs & Values 219 the previous prominent psychological causality with the biological one. The question is whether children, after the developmental milestone of the tenth year, continue to expect that prayer could have an impact on their recovery from a common illness. The current study addresses the question of whether children abandon their early established beliefs about the power of prayer to help a friend to a quicker recovery from a common illness before and after the milestone of the tenth year of age. Based on the previous evidence and on the framework theory, it is assumed that, before they acquire the biological theory (tenth year), children would give a higher chance of recovery from an illness to a sick individual for whom their friends are praying than to a sick individual for whose recovery nobody prays. These explanations should be progressively abandoned during the early school years as children will be informed about the biological explanatory framework. Method Participants: 160 children and young adults, equally divided into four age groups (eight-year-olds, mean age 8.16, SD=0.34; 10-year-olds, mean age 10.28, SD=0.22; 12-year-olds, mean age 11.96, SD = 0.22 and university students, mean age 20.01, SD=2.33), with equal numbers of boys and girls in each group, participated in the study. Children were interviewed individually, while university students completed the questionnaire in a written form. Materials and procedure: A vignette depicting two sick individuals who differed in terms of the prayers they received from their friends (e.g. a lot of friends pray for the patient versus nobody prays for the patient), including the time during which they fell ill, was presented to the participants. Then, they were asked which of the two individuals would recover more quickly from their illness. The answer of equally likely was also accepted. Participants were also asked to justify their responses. Results Participants overall at above chance level considered the fact that friends who pray for the sick individual will help him/her to recover from a common cold earlier than the individual whose friends did not pray for his/her recovery, t(159)=6.58, p<.001 (see Table 1). However, the answers were dependent on the age group, χ 2 (6)=26.51, Table 1. Frequencies and percentages (%) per age group of the participants answers. Age Group eightyear-olds 10-yearolds 12-yearolds 20-yearolds Total f % f % f % f % f % Nobody prays 0 0 0 0 0 0 2 5 2 1.3 Equally likely 4 10 7 17.5 10 25 20 50.0 41 25.6 Friends are praying 36** 90 33** 82.5 30** 75 18 45.0 117 73.1 Note: The stars indicate that the reported frequency is the above chance level. **p <.01.

220 D. Pnevmatikos p<.001, but not on the gender of the participants, χ 2 (2)=2.43, p=.30. Although the majority of the children endorsed the power of prayer to recover from illness at an above chance level, (t(39)=8.33, p<.001 for eight-year-olds, t(39)=5.34, p<.001 for the 10-year-olds and t(39)=3.61, p<.001 for the 12-year-olds) this tense diminished at the chance level (45%) only among the university students, t(39)=.63, p=.534. That is, children do not abandon the belief in the power of prayer after the age that they expected to change their explanatory theory about the power of prayer to help in recovery from a common illness. Participants who answered that the prayers from his/her friends could help his/ her recovering quicker from his/her sickness, justified their responses to the general value of prayers (50.4%), and a smaller number of the participants (20.5%) implicitly referred to the body mind interaction and to the integration of the superficial entities, to the vital power that is transferred through prayers (12.8%) and to the psychological resilience that prayers could give to the patient who knows that his/her friends are thinking for him/her being ill (3.4%). Other rarely given answers calculated for 10.3%, while only three participants found it difficult to justify their answers (1.9%). Discussion Carey (1985) suggested that the tenth year of age is the developmental milestone during which a coherent biological theory is established and the phenomenon of illness is explained in terms of physical causality, the main characteristic of the biological germ theory. However, like young children in the study of Woolley and Phelps (2001), early adolescents continued to endorse at an above chance level that prayer is an important aspect in helping the sick individual recover quicker from his/her illness, than the individual who does not benefit from friends prayers. Even 45% of the university students considered prayer an important factor in recovering from illness. Thus, prayer continues to be part of the adolescents and young adults explanatory framework and to influence their behaviour. This finding is in agreement with recent evidence showing that, among adults, the biological scientific explanatory framework of illness co-exists with the psycho-social explanatory framework (Legare and Gelman 2008) and that religion can constitute an important alternative explanatory causal framework for individuals (Legare et al. 2012). For many years, scholars in education saw an irreconcilable chasm between science and religion, considering the natural scientific explanations as rational, and supernatural explanations as irrational, and so expected the latter to be replaced by the scientific (rational) ones. Recently, Legare and Visala (2011) asserted the co-existence and access to multiple explanatory frameworks as a universal psychological experience (173). Despite efforts in education settings to replace the alternative explanatory frameworks, individuals usually try to reconcile synthesise (e.g. It can be the biological aspects and prayer that help to recovery from illness) or to integrate the two alternative explanations (i.e. natural and supernatural) in one (e.g. God acts through medicine or through doctors), or they distinguish the natural and the supernatural explanations when they explain different aspects of the same phenomenon (e.g. It is God s free will that helps the patient to recover from illness) (see also Legare et al. 2012).

Journal of Beliefs & Values 221 Conclusion The findings above have important educational implications. First, religious education is not considered as being in conflict with science. Religious belief systems, like the power of prayer to aid recovery from illness, are not considered as naive and superficial beliefs contrary to scientific explanations. When children and adolescents are informed about scientific explanations, they try to synthesise and integrate them with their alternative explanatory framework, one of which is their religious framework. Religious their illness or failing to make use of spiritual support.and spiritual education has potential to contribute to this integration and, indeed, the overall health of the individuals concerned. If individuals do not succeed in this integration and, instead, draw heavy distinctions between natural and supernatural explanations for recovery from illness, it could have negative consequences for public health, including through their either avoiding medicine to help their illness or failing to make use of spiritual support. References Carey, S. 1985. Conceptual Change in Childhood. MIT Press: Bradford Books. Legare, C. H., E. M. Evans, K. S. Rosengren, and P. L. Harris. 2012. The Coexistence of Natural and Supernatural Explanations Across Cultures and Development. Child Development 83: 779 793. Legare, C. H., and A. Visala. 2011. Between Religion and Science: Integrating Psychological and Philosophical Accounts of Explanatory Coexistence. Human Development 54: 169 184. Sayer, A. G., J. B. Willet, and E. C. Perrin. 1993. Measuring Understanding of Illness Causality in Healthy Children and in Children with Chronic Illness: A Construct Validation. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 14 (1): 11 36. Spilka, B., and K. L. Ladd. 2013. The Psychology of Prayer: A Scientific Approach. New York: The Guilford Press. Woolley, J. D., and K. E. Phelps. 2001. The Development of Children s Beliefs about Prayer. Journal of Cognition and Culture 1 (2): 139 166.