Are experimental economists prone to framing effects? A natural field experiment Gächter, Simon; Orzen, Henrik; Renner, Elke; Starmer, Chris

Similar documents
Are Experimental Economists Prone to Framing Effects? A Natural Field Experiment

The effect of the refusal avoidance training experiment on final disposition codes in the German ESS-2 Schnell, Rainer; Trappmann, Mark

Cognitive consequences of novelty and familiarity : how mere exposure influences level of construal Förster, Jens

Sad, thus true: negativity bias in judgments of truth Hilbig, Benjamin E.

The Influence of Facial Attractiveness on Imitation Leeuwen, Matthijs L. van; Veling, Harm; Baaren, Rick B. van; Dijksterhuis, Ap

Cognitive abilities and behavioral biases Oechssler, Jörg; Roider, Andreas; Schmitz, Patrick W.

Media violence and the self: the impact of personalized gaming characters in aggressive video games on aggressive behavior

A comparison between factor analysis and smallest space analysis of the comprehensive scoring system of the Rorschach Cohen, Arie

Get Angry, Get Out: The Interpersonal Effects of Anger Communication in Multiparty Negotiation

Group dynamic processes in groups

'Automatic' evaluation? Strategic effects on affective priming Teige-Mocigemba, Sarah; Klauer, Karl Christoph

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

Self-Reference Effect and the Group-Reference Effect in the Recall of Shared and Unshared Information in Nominal Groups and Interacting Groups

Rezension: Timothy A. Brown, 2006: Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research Schmidt, Peter

Hofmann, Wilhelm; Friese, Malte; Roefs, Anne Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Analyzing quota sample data and the peer-review process

When and how communicated guilt affects contributions in public good dilemmas Wubben, Maarten J.J.; Cremer, David de; Dijk, Eric van

Embodied perception with others bodies in mind : stereotype priming influence on the perception of spatial environment Chambon, Michel

Riding the O Train: Comparing the Effects of Ostracism and Verbal Dispute on Targets and Sources Zadro, Lisa; Williams, Kipling D.

On the goal-dependency of unconscious thought Bos, Maarten W.; Dijksterhuis, Ap; Baaren, Rick B. van

Is Compensation Enough? Relational Concerns in Responding to Unintended Inequity

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

A twin study into the genetic and environmental influences on academic performance in science in 9-year-old boys and girls

The issue of fuzzy concepts in test construction and possible remedies

The Effect of Group Decision Making on Cooperation in Social Dilemmas

How Dissociated Are Implicit and Explicit Racial Attitudes? A Bogus Pipeline Approach Nier, Jason A.

The German Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP) as reference data set Siedler, Thomas; Schupp, Jürgen; Spiess, Katharina; Wagner, Gert G.

Verbal and nonverbal behaviour as a basis for credibility attribution: the impact of task involvement and cognitive capacity

Prospects for Group Processes and Intergroup Relations Research: A Review of 70 Years' Progress

Gender Differences in the Relational and Collective Bases for Trust Maddux, William W.; Brewer, Marilynn B.

The politics of opinion assignment: a conditional logit model with varying choice set Gschwend, Thomas; King, Chad M.

Changing explicit and implicit attitudes: the case of self-esteem Grumm, Mandy; Nestler, Steffen; Collani, Gernot von

Surface-Level Diversity and Decision-Making in Groups: When Does Deep-Level Similarity Help?

How Love and Lust Change People s Perception of Partners and Relationships

The rhythm of rapport: interpersonal synchrony and social perception

Autism spectrum disorders in children and adolescents with Moebius sequence

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

The aaersion to lying Lundquist, Tobias; Ellingsen, Tore; Gribbe, Erik; Johannesson, Magnus

Socioeconomic inequalities in cancer incidence and mortality - a spatial analysis in Bremen, Germany

Incorporating eye tracking into cognitive interviewing to pretest survey questions

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

Direct evidence on income comparisons and their welfare effects Senik, Claudia

Popular evolutionary psychology in the UK: an unusual case of science in the media? Cassidy, Angela

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

Contact, Perspective Taking, and Anxiety as Predictors of Stereotype Endorsement, Explicit Attitudes, and Implicit Attitudes

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Speaking for Others: The Pros and Cons of Group Advocates using Collective Language Hornsey, Matthew J.; Blackwood, Leda; O brien, Anne

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Impact of drug discount contracts on pharmacies and on patients' drug supply Gröber-Grätz, Dagmar; Gulich, Markus

"How much do you like your name?" An implicit measure of global self-esteem

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Exploring the Meaningfulness and Relevance of Subjective Well-being for India Neff, Daniel

The answer is blowing in the wind: weather effects on personality ratings

Undesired self-image congruence in a lowinvolvement

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Parental rating of sleep in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder Hvolby, Allan; Jørgensen, Jan; Bilenberg, Niels

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Tropa, Mihai Alternative pedagogical strategies

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

Scandals and the public's trust in politicians: assimilation and contrast effects

Attitudinal and socio-structural determinants of cervical cancer screening and HPV vaccination uptake: a quantitative multivariate analysis

Strik Lievers, Luisa; Curt, Florence; Wallier, Jenny; Perdereau, Fabienne; Rein, Zoé; Jeammet, Philippe; Godart, Nathalie

econstor Make Your Publications Visible.

Ignoring the Non-ignorables? Missingness and Missing Positions König, Thomas; Finke, Daniel; Daimer, Stephanie

Enabling healthy choices: is ICT the highway to health improvement?

econstor Make Your Publications Visible.

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

What makes revenge satisfactory: seeing the offender suffer or delivering a message?

Income, Relational Goods and Happiness Becchetti, Leonardo; Trovato, Giovanni; Londono Bedoya, David Andres

Public culture and public understanding of genetics: a focus group study

Pueblo chico, infierno grande Castro-Vazquez, Genaro; Tarui, Masayoshi Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

How Newly Acquainted Dyads Develop Shared Stereotypic Impressions through Conversation

Translating Answers to Open-ended Survey Questions in Cross-cultural Research: A Case Study on the Interplay between Translation, Coding, and Analysis

Style of practice and assortative mating: a recursive probit analysis of cesarean section scheduling in Italy Fabbri, Daniele; Monfardini, Chiara

Time's Social Metaphors Torre, Ramón Ramos Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

A comparison of response effects in selfadministered

Using a multilevel structural equation modeling approach to explain cross-cultural measurement noninvariance

Scientific Knowledge through Involvement How to Do Respectful Othering

The role of self-help efforts in the reintegration of politically motivated former prisoners: implications from the Northern Irish experience

Veröffentlichungsversion / Published Version Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Practising Performativity Markussen, Turid Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Social Insecurities and Fear of Crime: a Cross- National Study on the Impact of Welfare State Policies on Crime-related Anxieties

Lay Theories and Intergroup Relations Levy, Sheri R.; Chiu, Chi-yue; Hong, Ying-yi

Incentives in two German mail surveys 1996/97 & 1997

Modulation of facial mimicry by attitudes Likowski, Katja U.; Mühlberger, Andreas; Seibt, Beate; Pauli, Paul; Weyers, Peter

The Neuroscience of Stigma and Stereotype Threat

Expanding social identity theory for research in media effects : two international studies and a theoretical model Trepte, Sabine; Krämer, Nicole

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Transcription:

www.ssoar.info Are experimental economists prone to framing effects? A natural field experiment Gächter, Simon; Orzen, Henrik; Renner, Elke; Starmer, Chris Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with: www.peerproject.eu Empfohlene Zitierung / Suggested Citation: Gächter, Simon ; Orzen, Henrik ; Renner, Elke ; Starmer, Chris: Are experimental economists prone to framing effects? A natural field experiment. In: Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 70 (2009), 3, pp. 443-446. DOI: http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2007.11.003 Nutzungsbedingungen: Dieser Text wird unter dem "PEER Licence Agreement zur Verfügung" gestellt. Nähere Auskünfte zum PEER-Projekt finden Sie hier: http://www.peerproject.eu Gewährt wird ein nicht exklusives, nicht übertragbares, persönliches und beschränktes Recht auf Nutzung dieses Dokuments. Dieses Dokument ist ausschließlich für den persönlichen, nicht-kommerziellen Gebrauch bestimmt. Auf sämtlichen Kopien dieses Dokuments müssen alle Urheberrechtshinweise und sonstigen Hinweise auf gesetzlichen Schutz beibehalten werden. Sie dürfen dieses Dokument nicht in irgendeiner Weise abändern, noch dürfen Sie dieses Dokument für öffentliche oder kommerzielle Zwecke vervielfältigen, öffentlich ausstellen, aufführen, vertreiben oder anderweitig nutzen. Mit der Verwendung dieses Dokuments erkennen Sie die Nutzungsbedingungen an. Terms of use: This document is made available under the "PEER Licence Agreement ". For more Information regarding the PEER-project see: http://www.peerproject.eu This document is solely intended for your personal, non-commercial use.all of the copies of this documents must retain all copyright information and other information regarding legal protection. You are not allowed to alter this document in any way, to copy it for public or commercial purposes, to exhibit the document in public, to perform, distribute or otherwise use the document in public. By using this particular document, you accept the above-stated conditions of use. Diese Version ist zitierbar unter / This version is citable under: http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:0168-ssoar-283110

Are Experimental Economists Prone to Framing Effects? A Natural Field Experiment Simon Gächter, Henrik Orzen 1, Elke Renner 2, Chris Starmer 3 Centre for Decision Research and Experimental Economics, University of Nottingham, UK 11 October 2007 Corrected 17 November 2008 Abstract An extensive literature demonstrates the existence of framing effects in the laboratory and in questionnaire studies. This paper reports new evidence from a natural field experiment using a subject pool one might expect to be particularly resistant to such effects: experimental economists. We find that while the behaviour of junior experimental economists is affected by the description of the decision task they face, this is not the case for the more senior members of our subject pool. JEL Classification: C93; D01 Keywords: Framing; Field experiments Corresponding author: Simon Gächter, University of Nottingham, School of Economics, Sir Clive Granger Building, University Park, Nottingham, NG7 2RD, United Kingdom. Email: simon.gaechter@nottingham.ac.uk. Tel.: +44 (0) 115 84 66132. Fax: +44 (0)115 951 4159. 1 Email: henrik.orzen@nottingham.ac.uk. Tel.: +44 (0) 115 84 67847. Fax: +44 (0)115 951 4159 2 Email: elke.renner@nottingham.ac.uk. Tel.: +44 (0) 115 95 15399. Fax: +44 (0)115 951 4159 3 Email: chris.starmer@nottingham.ac.uk. Tel.: +44 (0) 115 84 66067. Fax: +44 (0)115 951 4159 1 Page 1 of 7

1. Introduction This paper reports a natural field experiment designed to test the principle of description invariance, that is, the presumption that an agent s choice from a feasible set of alternatives should be unaffected by any re-description of it that leaves all objective characteristics unchanged. While this principle has considerable normative appeal, there is evidence that it fails descriptively in simple laboratory settings; choices do depend on how a task is framed. Most studies on framing effects (including ours) describe logically equivalent decision situations in either a positive or a negative light. 1 The Asian Disease Problem, due to Tversky and Kahneman (1981), is a well known example. Describing a medical programme in terms of lives saved or lives lost led to dramatically different answers, although the problems were logically equivalent. Levin et al. (1998) provide an overview and classification of framing effects found in numerous laboratory and questionnaire studies, both in the context of individual decision making as well as in experimental games. Our paper extends this literature by testing for the existence of a framing effect within a natural field experiment. In our experiment, the participants were junior and senior experimental scientists (mostly economists) who registered for the 2006 Economic Science Association (ESA) meeting in Nottingham, UK. As part of the normal organizational process, all people who had submitted a paper received an email reminding them to register. The experimental framing manipulation concerned the fee for late registration. Half of the participants were told that there is a penalty of $50 for registering after the first deadline. The other half of participants were told that there is a discount of $50 for registering before the first deadline. If we observe different registration patterns for these groups, we have field evidence for a framing effect. 2 A few methodological remarks on our experiment are in order. First, registering for a conference is part of the usual business of any scientist, and while doing so for ESA- Nottingham our 120 participants were not aware that they were taking part in an experiment. Our experiment therefore classifies as a natural field experiment, to borrow the terminology of Harrison and List (2004). Second, the stakes were substantial and beyond those used in typical lab experiments. Third, if as an (experimental) economist you were sceptical about the relevance of framing effects, you ought to be especially persuaded by evidence from a natural field experiment using economists as subjects. So, are experimental economists prone to framing effects? Read on if you would like to know the answer. 2. The experiment We conducted the experiment in the run-up to the 2006 ESA conference in Nottingham. After the submission deadline we sent an email to all prospective conference participants informing them about the acceptance of their paper and other organizational details. To investigate whether participants decisions when to pay the conference fee is independent of the description of the task, we randomly assigned them to one of two treatment groups. Both 1 Not all studies of framing focus on the positive/negative contrast. For example, Frey and Meier (2004) use field data to investigate whether a change in the timing of a decision affects charitable giving. 2 In the terminology of Levin et al. (1998) our experiment manipulates goal framing. They list 27 studies on goal framing. Almost all of them are laboratory or questionnaire studies. A rare example of a field experiment investigating framing is Ganzach and Karsahi s (1995) study of credit card use. 2 Page 2 of 7

groups received in essence the same acceptance email except for one sentence. 3 In one version we framed the difference between early and late registration fee as a discount: We take this opportunity to remind you that the discounted conference fee for early registration is available until 10 July 2006. In a second version we used a penalty frame: We take this opportunity to remind you that the conference fee will include a penalty for late registration after 10 July 2006. As is usual for scientific conferences there were two types of registration fee: a regular fee for faculty and a reduced fee for PhD students. The early-registration fee was $195 for faculty and $145 for PhD students, and the late-registration fee was $245 for faculty and $195 for PhD students. The difference in fees due to people s eligibility for the student reduction naturally divides our subjects into two subgroups, which we classify as senior and junior conference participants and which we will examine separately in our data analysis. Registration for the conference took place through the ESA website, which recorded the dates of all payments and automatically switched to the late fee on July 11 th. The final number of experimental subjects (some withdrew their submissions) was 120. Table 1 shows how junior and senior experimental economists were distributed between the two treatment groups. 4 3. Results Table 1: Participants and decision frames Type of participant Junior Senior Total Discount frame 18 43 61 Penalty frame 27 32 59 Total 45 75 120 Most of our subjects, 101 out of 120 (84%), registered early (82% of the junior participants and 85% of the senior participants). There are many reasons why participants might register early rather than late, but we suspect a major reason is the difference in price. The predominance of early registrants in our sample is exactly what we would expect if incentives matter. But does it make a difference whether early registration is described as an opportunity to avoid a penalty or as an opportunity to take advantage of a discount? We examine our data in two main steps. First, we report results of a simple nonparametric statistical analysis using the Fisher exact test. Second, we conduct a probit regression analysis to investigate whether 3 The full text of the email is provided in the appendix. 4 The random assignment resulted in a slightly uneven distribution of junior and senior participants across treatments. Note that when we did the assignment we were unable to discriminate between these two groups since at the time we did not know who would be eligible for the reduced student fee. 3 Page 3 of 7

including, and controlling for, additional explanatory variables confirms our findings from step one and/or adds any other insights on the behaviour of our subjects. Table 2 provides details of our nonparametric analysis. As is evident from the bottom row, in the aggregate data there is no evidence of a framing effect. However, this disguises an interesting difference between the behaviour of junior and senior participants. When we disaggregate, although we still see no framing effect for senior participants, for junior researchers we do observe a substantial and statistically significant change in behaviour: one third in the discount group registered late, whereas in the penalty frame almost everybody registered early. Table 2: Proportions of early registration by treatment and type of participant p-values from two-tailed Fisher exact tests Discount frame Penalty frame p-value for difference Senior 88% 81% 0.513 Junior 67% 93% 0.045 Overall 82% 86% 0.338 We have two potentially relevant pieces of background data on each conference attendee: their gender and the geographical location of their home institution. In our sample 68% are male and 85% are from European institutions (almost all the others came from North American institutions). We included this information as explanatory variables in a probit regression. As seen in Table 2, the effect of the treatment variable on behaviour interacts with the subject type, so we also allow for interaction effects between treatment and subject characteristics in the regression. The regression results are shown in Table 3, and they confirm and extend our earlier findings. The impact of the treatment variable on senior researchers is negligible. In contrast, being a junior participant in the discount treatment is associated with a large and significant drop in the probability of registering early, which is then reversed by a similarly sized increase for juniors in the penalty treatment. Taken together, these results suggest that the members of the penalty junior group and both senior groups all behave in a similar way, whereas the members of the discount junior group display an increased inclination to register late. Gender has no significant effect on registration behaviour, and males and females do not respond differently to the treatment variation. A conference participant s home location, however, has a very strong effect on behaviour: non-european attendees are almost 40% more likely to register late. However, there is no evidence of a stronger framing effect among non-europeans. 4 Page 4 of 7

Table 3: Determinants of registration behaviour Dependent variable: Probability of registering early Independent variables Coefficients from probit regression Marginal effects of changes in variables Constant 1.351 *** (0.3123) Penalty 0 (1) = Discount (Penalty) frame 0.310 (0.4411) Junior 1.059 ** 0 (1) = Senior (Junior) (0.4481) Penalty x Junior 1.559 ** (0.6388) NonEU 1.273 ** 0 (1) = European (Non-European) (0.5841) institution Penalty x NonEU 0.681 (0.7567) Female 0.443 0 (1) = Male (Female) (0.4589) Penalty x Female 0.376 (0.6822) Standard errors in parentheses. Significance at the 10%, 5%, 1% level is denoted by *, **, *** respectively. 4. Conclusion N = 120 Prob. > Chi 2 = 0.0879 Pseudo R 2 = 0.1183 0.066 (0.0946) 0.259 ** (0.1188) 0.212 ** (0.0598) 0.391 ** (0.2092) 0.104 (0.0784) 0.087 (0.0809) 0.093 (0.1913) Our study has produced clear evidence of a framing effect in a natural field experiment. A question raised by our study is why that effect occurs for juniors but not seniors. A variety of possible differences between the characteristics of juniors and seniors might explain this. One such difference is that financing conference visits may be easier for faculty members than for PhD students. Notice that for this to account for our findings, one would have to argue that juniors are prone to the framing effect because they face, due to their tighter budget constraints, higher incentives than the seniors. However, if access to funding were indeed an important factor in determining behaviour, we would expect a generally increased inclination among juniors to register early, yet we do not observe this (overall, the proportion of early registrations is almost identical for juniors and seniors). Another possibly relevant difference is that seniors have more experience in making decisions related to conference attendance, and there is evidence to suggest that more experienced choosers are less prone to some decision anomalies (e.g. List 2003). While these interpretations are speculative, our experiment does provide a clear and simple answer to the question it was designed to address. Are experimental economists prone to framing effects? On the basis of our data the answer is: juniors are and seniors are not. 5 Page 5 of 7

Acknowledgements We thank Martin Sefton, John List, Nikos Nikiforakis and two anonymous referees for very helpful comments. References Frey, B.S., Meier, S., 2004. Pro-social behavior in a natural setting. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 54, 65-88. Ganzach, Y., Karsahi, N., 1995. Message framing and buying behavior: a field experiment. Journal of Business Research 32, 11-17. Harrison, G.W., List, J.A., 2004. Field experiments. Journal of Economic Literature 42, 1009-1055. Levin, I.P., Schneider, S.L., Gaeth, G.J., 1998. All frames are not created equal: a typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 76, 149-188. List, J.A., 2003. Does market experience eliminate anomalies? Quarterly Journal of Economics 118, 41-71. Tversky, A., Kahneman, D., 1981. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science 211, 453-458. 6 Page 6 of 7

Appendix: The full text of the email sent to conference participants { } = Discount frame [ ] = Penalty frame Dear Sir or Madam, Thank you for submitting your abstract to the Economic Science Association 2006 European Meeting. We are pleased to inform you that your paper has been accepted for inclusion in the programme. Please register and pay through the ESA web page. [We take this opportunity to remind you that the conference will include a penalty for late registration after 10 July 2006.] {We take this opportunity to remind you that the discounted conference fee for early registration is available until 10 July 2006.} To register please go to the ESA website (www.economicscience.org) and log in with your email address and password. If you haven't got an account yet, you have to sign up first (just follow the "log in" link on the ESA website). You can sign up for free without becoming a member of the Economic Science Association, and you do not have to be a member to attend the conference. When you make your travel arrangements, keep in mind that the conference will begin with a reception on Thursday, 7 September, at 18:00 and end at 15:00 on Sunday, 10th September. For information about hotels and transportation please visit the Conference website www.nottingham.ac.uk/economics/cedex/esa2006/index.html We will publish the preliminary programme in the second half of July. We look forward to welcoming you in Nottingham. Best regards, ----------------------------------------------------- Local Organising Committee ESA 2006 European Regional Meeting 7 Page 7 of 7