Title: Determinants of intention to get tested for STI/HIV among the Surinamese and Antilleans in the Netherlands: results of an online survey

Similar documents
Title:Continuity of GP care is associated with lower use of complementary and alternative medical providers A population-based cross-sectional survey

Title: Identifying work ability promoting factors for home care aides and assistant nurses

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS VERSION 1 - REVIEW. Veronika Williams University of Oxford, UK 07-Dec-2015

Title: Healthy snacks at the checkout counter: A lab and field study on the impact of shelf arrangement and assortment structure on consumer choices

Title: Home Exposure to Arabian Incense (Bakhour) and Asthma Symptoms in Children: A Community Survey in Two Regions in Oman

Reproductive Health s Knowledge, Attitudes, and Practices A European Youth Study Protocol October 13, 2009

Revision notes 7.5. Section 7.5 Health promotion

Title: Attitude toward Contraception and Abortion among Curacao women. Ineffective contraception due to limited sexual education?

Author's response to reviews

What Constitutes a Good Contribution to the Literature (Body of Knowledge)?

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS TITLE (PROVISIONAL)

batyr: Preventative education in mental illnesses among university students

Title: The Limitations of Voluntary Medical Male Circumcision and the Importance of Sustained Condom Use: A Kenyan Newspaper Analysis

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

ID BMJ R4

Title: Correlates of STI symptoms among female sex workers with trucker driver clients in two Mexican border towns

Title:Decisions on statin therapy by patients' opinions about survival gains: Cross sectional survey of general practitioners.

Title: Risser Patient Satisfaction Scale: A Validation study in Greek Cancer Patients

The Cochrane Collaboration

Research Approaches Quantitative Approach. Research Methods vs Research Design

Assignment 4: True or Quasi-Experiment

HANDOUTS UNITED REPUBLIC OF TANZANIA MINISTRY OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL WELFARE. HIV and AIDS Voluntary Counselling and Testing MODULE 1.

Title: Reliability and validity of the adolescent stress questionnaire in a sample of European adolescents - the HELENA study

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS VERSION 1 - REVIEW. Ball State University

David O Malley, Ph.D., LISW Case Western Reserve University Cleveland, Ohio

Counseling and Testing for HIV. Protocol Booklet

User Experience: Findings from Patient Telehealth Survey

Title:Problematic computer gaming, console-gaming, and internet use among adolescents: new measurement tool and association with time use

Computer tailored persuasion: effectiveness and side-effects. Arie Dijkstra University of Groningen The Netherlands

Title: Use of food labels by adolescents to make healthier choices on snacks: a cross sectional study from Sri Lanka

DON M. PALLAIS, CPA 14 Dahlgren Road Richmond, Virginia Telephone: (804) Fax: (804)

Author's response to reviews

Title:Modern contraceptive use among sexually active men in Uganda: Does discussion with a health worker matter?

Title: A Central Storage Facility to Reduce Pesticide Suicides- A Feasibility Study from India

Title: Overlap of Cognitive Concepts in Chronic Widespread Pain: An Exploratory Study

INFLUENCING FLU VACCINATION BEHAVIOR: Identifying Drivers & Evaluating Campaigns for Future Promotion Planning

Title: Understanding consumer acceptance of intervention strategies for healthy food choices: a qualitative study

BASIC VOLUME. Elements of Drug Dependence Treatment

Hae Won KIM. KIM Reproductive Health (2015) 12:91 DOI /s x

Author s response to reviews

Title: Can self-reported disability assessment behaviour of insurance physicians be explained? Applying the ASE model

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION

Summary and conclusion Replication of a measurement instrument for social exclusion

Active Bystander Behavior: Extended Analysis from the Sexual Conduct: Culture and Respect Survey Tyler Anderson 16 Grinnell College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

Romania (2006): HIV/AIDS TraC Study Evaluating the Effect of a POL-type Program among Men who have Sex with Men in Bucharest.

To test or not to test: A psychosocial perspective of HIV testing

Responsibilities in a sexual relationship - Contact tracing

2019 Judging Form Suicide Prevention Category

Title: Socioeconomic conditions and number of pain sites in women

Radon: A Behavioural as well as a Technical & Health Challenge.

Attitudes to HIV among year olds in London

Perceived usefulness. Intention Use E-filing. Attitude. Ease of use Perceived behavioral control. Subjective norm

IT S A WONDER WE UNDERSTAND EACH OTHER AT ALL!

Role of Health Education and Communication Part 3

Bass Line :The African Health and Sex Survey

gender and violence 2 The incidence of violence varies dramatically by place and over time.

Thank you very much for your guidance on this revision. We look forward to hearing from you.

Author's response to reviews

Chapter 3-Attitude Change - Objectives. Chapter 3 Outline -Attitude Change

Title: Prognostic factors for non-success in patients with sciatica and disc herniation

Chapter 8. Summary and general discussion Samenvatting (Dutch summary)

What Solution-Focused Coaches Do: An Empirical Test of an Operationalization of Solution-Focused Coach Behaviors

Lecture (chapter 1): Introduction

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2)

Title: Living alone and antidepressant medication use: a prospective study in a working-age population

The relation between self-efficacy and feedback perception and between feedback and intrinsic motivation

Author's response to reviews

In this chapter we discuss validity issues for quantitative research and for qualitative research.

Funnelling Used to describe a process of narrowing down of focus within a literature review. So, the writer begins with a broad discussion providing b

Author's response to reviews

Author's response to reviews

Title: Elevated depressive symptoms in metabolic syndrome in a general population of Japanese men: a cross-sectional study

Standards for Sexuality Education in Europe: Frequently asked questions

Title: The effects of videogames therapy on balance and attention in chronic ambulatory traumatic brain injury: an exploratory study.

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS TITLE (PROVISIONAL)

Sexual Risk and STI/HIV

Computer tailored persuasion: Working mechanisms. Arie Dijkstra University of Groningen The Netherlands

NICE guidelines. Flu vaccination: increasing uptake in clinical risk groups and health and social care workers

Steady Ready Go! teady Ready Go. Every day, young people aged years become infected with. Preventing HIV/AIDS in young people

Author's response to reviews

Title: Evaluation of the Housing First Program in patients with severe mental disorders in France: study protocol for a randomised controlled trial

STI/HIV prevalence among Female Sex Workers(FSW s) and treatment seeking behavior for STI in Maharashtra

Chapter 3 - Does Low Well-being Modify the Effects of

Negotiating safe sex among women of Afro-Surinamese and Dutch Antillean descent in the Netherlands

CHAPTER 3: METHODOLOGY

Title:Video-confidence: a qualitative exploration of videoconferencing for psychiatric emergencies

Communication for Change A S H O R T G U I D E T O S O C I A L A N D B E H A V I O R C H A N G E ( S B C C ) T H E O R Y A N D M O D E L S

Coming Together to Change Campus Culture. A Survival Guide for Applying the Social Norms Approach

SMS USA PHASE ONE SMS USA BULLETIN BOARD FOCUS GROUP: MODERATOR S GUIDE

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT

Introduction to Psychology Social Psychology Quiz

Shoplifting Inventory: Standardization Study

Appendix D: Statistical Modeling

2019 Judging Form Through the Lens of Culture Mental Health Matters Category

Title: Estimation of the burden of varicella in Europe before the introduction of universal childhood immunization

PRELIMINARY EXAM EVALUATION FACULTY SCORE SHEET

Author's response to reviews

Lecture Outline. Biost 517 Applied Biostatistics I. Purpose of Descriptive Statistics. Purpose of Descriptive Statistics

The effects of narrative versus non-narrative information in school health education about alcohol drinking for low educated adolescents

Transcription:

Author's response to reviews Title: Determinants of intention to get tested for STI/HIV among the Surinamese and Antilleans in the Netherlands: results of an online survey Authors: Alvin H Westmaas (alvin.westmaas@maastrichtuniversity.nl) Gerjo Kok (g.kok@maastrichtuniversity.nl) Pjer Vriens (pj.vriens@rotterdam.nl) Hannelore M Götz (hm.gotz@rotterdam.nl) Jan H Richardus (jh.richardus@rotterdam.nl) Hélène Voeten (h.voeten@rotterdam.nl) Version: 4 Date: 15 September 2012 Author's response to reviews: see over

Dear sir/madam of the editorial board, First we want to thank you and the reviewers for the feedback on the manuscript entitled Determinants of intention to get tested for STI/HIV among the Surinamese and Antilleans in the Netherlands: results of an online survey (1356053106703030). Please find enclosed our revised manuscript. Point-by-point response to the concerns of the reviewers: Reviewer 1, Jochen Mikolajczak Major compulsory revisions 1. Recruitment of participants: Reviewer 1 asked to elaborate on how the different ways of recruitment could have influenced the findings in the study (do people from both sampling waves differ), and to provide more information on the sampling procedure (how many were contacted and how many agreed to participate).. We agree with the reviewer that it is relevant to test if people from both sampling waves differ. We therefore added a table to the Results section which shows these differences (see next page). In the first paragraph of the Results we state: Table 1 demonstrates that there are differences in means between the online and the pen-and-paper questionnaire (PPQ) group for both the Surinamese and Dutch- Antilleans. We also checked the direction of the correlates between intention and the other variables. We found that age was the only variable that changed direction between the online and PPQ group for both the Surinamese and Dutch-Antilleans. As the correlates of the online and PPQ group were not that different from each other, the groups were merged together for both the Surinamese and the Dutch-Antilleans. The impact of this decision will be discussed in the Discussion section. In the paragraph where the limitations are discussed in the Discussion section, we elaborate on the decision of merging the online and pen-and-paper (PPQ) group: Combining the respondents from the different sampling waves could have influenced the results found in the study (Table 1). For the Surinamese, the pen-and-paper questionnaire (PPQ) group is only 3% of the total, and thus its influence on the analyses can be neglected. For the Dutch-Antilleans, when only looking at the variables of the multivariate model, we find that differences between the online PPQ group were found for all variables in the model except for health motivation and open communication. It is possible that the higher perceived severity, higher motivation, and lower mean age of the PPQ group led to a higher mean intention of this group as they are more likely to perform sexual health behavior. However, it is also possible that the higher education of

the online panel led to a more realistic perception regarding the threat of STIs, resulting in lower intention. Although the underlying mechanism remains unclear, we should take the possibility of bias into account [18].. Table 1: Mean difference between respondents of the online and pen-and paper questionnaire (PPQ) group Surinamese Dutch-Antilleans Online group(n=436) PPQ group (N=14) p-value Online group (N=224) PPQ group (N=79) p-value Intention 2.16 3.96 <.001 2.16 3.68 <.001 Perceived susceptibility 4.45 4.58.6 4.50 4.53.8 Perceived severity 4.75 4.89.4 4.69 4.87.003 Health Motivation 3.84 4.39.07 3.83 4.17.02 Perceived benefits 4.16 4.01.6 4.22 4.44.1 Perceived barriers 4.18 3.92.4 4.08 4.25.2 Cues to action 36.2% 92.9% <.001 42.4% 63.6%.001 Self efficacy 4.50 4.57 0.7 4.52 4.49.7 Outcome expectancies 3.11 3.43.3 3.26 3.26 1 Subjective norms* 2.77 3.62.02 2.63 3.55 <.001 Social support 4.02 4.06.9 4.06 4.32.03 Emotional outcomes 2.98 2.97 1 3.09 3.03.7 Knowledge 4.64 4.79.7 4.64 4.58.7 Risk behavior 22.5% 21.4%.9 18.8% 39.2% <.001 Open communication 3.05 3.90.01 3.22 3.56.03 Test history (yes) 47% 64.3%.2 45.1% 79.7% <.001 Gender (female) 64.7% 50%.3 61.6% 64.6%.6 Age 32.00 21.64 <.001 32.14 24.04 <.001 Marital status (married) 45.2% 7.1%.01 46.0% 19.2% <.001 Relationship status (yes) 58.7% 57.1%.9 58% 59.5%.8 Education (high) 26.8% 0%.02 27.2% 16.5%.06 Religious 67% 85.7%.1 63.4% 93.5% <.001 In the Results section - Prediction of intention to get tested for STI/HIV in the coming six months - we elaborate on how the differences in age between the two recruitment waves had implications for the multivariate analyses:

Although the variable age should have been entered into the models for both the Surinamese and Dutch-Antilleans, it was left out of the equations. The reason for this is related to the different recruitment ways (online survey versus pen-and-paper questionnaire, PPQ). After analyzing the correlation between intention and age for the online versus the PPQ group, we found that the correlation changes direction, for the Surinamese as well as the Dutch-Antilleans. We therefore felt it is inappropriate to include age in the regression analyses, the more because we aim to identify determinants of STI/HIV testing that can be changed with an intervention. In the Methods section recruitment of participants - we now provide more information on the sampling procedure, and on how many respondents were invited in which way: The participants for this study were recruited through an Internet research agency called FlyCatcher, an Internet research agency called PanelClix, and by group activities at a migrant organization in Rotterdam, the Netherlands. Both research agencies have an online Internet panel that is representative for the population living in the Netherlands with regards to age, gender, socio-economic status (SES), and ethnicity. The number of invites and response rate was also added at this section in the Methods: Together, the research agencies invited 5267 respondents of which 1160 completed the questionnaire leading to a response rate of 22%. Of this sample, 436 Surinamese and 224 Dutch-Antilleans were included in the survey, after correcting for our definition of ethnicity as the research agencies evidently used another definition of ethnicity that remained unclear. Because of the smaller number of Dutch-Antillean respondents, we organized two sessions of group activities at a migrant organization where most of the Dutch-Antilleans were familiar with. However, this organization stated that the Dutch- Antilleans would be more likely to fill in a pen-and-paper questionnaires (PPQ). Therefore, the online questionnaire was converted to a paper version for the group activities. In total, 99 people participated in the group activities of which 97 completed the questionnaire. The questionnaires were entered into a statistical program by hand; 79 questionnaires were from Dutch-Antillean respondents and 14 from Surinamese (4 were from other ethnic communities and were thus excluded). 2. Reviewer 1 stated that we claim that the PEN-3 model was applied to ensure cultural sensitivity of the questionnaire, and he asked to illustrate how the theoretical models that are used in the study were operationalized in the context of PEN-3 to ensure cultural sensitivity.

In our paper we mention PEN-3 only once, stating that PEN-3 was used in both qualitative studies that will be presented elsewhere. We do not claim that PEN-3 was used in the quantitative survey that we report in this paper. However, we did try to take the cultural values of the target group into account by using the results of the qualitative focus group study to add certain questions to the questionnaire (for example on open communication about sexuality). We did not operationalize questions of existing theoretical models in the context of PEN-3 (nor do we claim so). 3. Reviewer 1 stated that it was better to report the correlation rather that the alpha when only two items are scaled. We thank the reviewer for noticing, and we now report the correlation with p-value for the variables perceived susceptibility, perceived severity, and intention. Secondly, the reviewer was wondering if not more perceived benefits could be linked to taking a test. The reviewer is correct that more benefits could be linked to taking a test when looking at the literature. However, we tried to quantify the findings of the qualitative focus group study and therefore only used the beliefs mentioned by the participants of that study. Thirdly, the reviewer reported that it was confusing how two beliefs were mentioned, but four items were scaled for perceived barriers. We adjusted the sentence in order to make it more clear that there are two sets of two beliefs: Perceived benefits were measured by two beliefs for both STI and HIV: I would get tested for (STI HIV), because I could receive better treatment and I would get tested for (STI HIV), because I can prevent infecting someone. All four questions were combined into one scale (alpha=.93) Fourthly, the reviewer was wondering if the time frame of six months was also used in the other constructs in the questionnaire. No, we did not use the time frame of the coming six months for the other constructs in the questionnaire. For only a few constructs used in the questionnaire it is possible to use a time frame that lies in the future, for example for perceived susceptibility. We tried to do this for the constructs for which it was possible to do so, but during the pretest in became clear that it was too difficult for respondents to answer the questions while considering a certain timeframe in the future. Therefore, we decided to mention the time frame of coming 6 months only for intention. We added in the Results section:

After the pre-test, some of the questions in the questionnaire were redefined for better understanding, such as use of language and removal of time frame of coming 6 months (except for the questions on intention to get tested). Lastly, the reviewer asked to provide the reader with more information on the coding of the socio-demographic variables. In the Methods section analysis - we now provide more information regarding the coding of the socio-demographic variables before using them in the analysis: The third block consisted of the other variables such as risk behavior, open communication and the socio-demographical variables. For the latter, some of the variables were recoded for easy interpretation of the results. Marital status was dichotomized; unmarried versus living together / married / divorced / widow(er). Level of education was recoded into three scores; higher educated (university and HBO), average educated (pre-vocational, vocational, higher general secondary education, and pre-university education) and lower educated (primary school, domestic science and similar education). Religion was also dichotomized by scoring all religions as 1 and no religion as 0. 4. Reviewer 1 questions if multiple testing is not an issue in our study, because we have so many variables in our analyses, and wonders why we used a significance level of p<.05. We believe that the reviewer is right that multiple testing could be an issue in our study, and that using a p-value <.05 was not the best option. To adjust for multiple outcome testing we now consider p<.01 as significant, as mentioned in the Methods section: Given the large number of variables in the analyses, we adjusted for multiple outcome testing by considering a p-value of <.01 as significant, for all analyses. As a result, less statistical differences are reported in the Results. However, the explained variance of the multivariate model is hardly affected for the Dutch-Antilleans and not at all for the Surinamese. We find less direct influences due to the lower p-value. Also, looking at the low and high intenders, we find less significant differences for the Surinamese. 5. Reviewer 1 wonders why social desirability bias was not mentioned as a general limitation because all constructs possibly suffer from this effect, and secondly he wonders if the available data could not explain the strange behavior of the construct self-efficacy. With respect to the first question, we adjusted this point in the limitations mentioned in the Discussion section: Social desirability bias refers to the tendency of respondents to answer questions with responses they believe are socially desired, rather than answering questions by

responses which reflect their actual thoughts or feelings [20]. This phenomenon is not uncommon in social studies regarding widely accepted social norms or attitudes, and often occurs when the respondents feel that their answers could be linked back to them. Within the Afro-Caribbean community, it is still perceived as a taboo to talk about sexuality. Although we used an (internet) survey method, which should increase the perceived feeling of privacy among the respondents and therefore lower the social desirability bias, it is still possible that the respondents answered the questionnaire as they felt it should be answered. Also, the perceived prejudices of this community about their sexual behavior could have prevented them to truthfully fill in the questionnaire in order to prevent meeting the beliefs of the social environment. To answer the second remark of the reviewer, we conducted additional analysis by cross tabulating the variables intention, self-efficacy, and perceived barriers. We did find some evidence that our hypothesis in the Discussion section regarding the strange behavior of the construct could be correct: Our study also shows that for the Surinamese self-efficacy is negatively correlated with the intention to get tested in the coming six months; the more people perceive themselves as being capable to get tested, the less intention these people show; the same was found for perceived severity. We found some evidence in the data that could help explain the negative correlations found. We found that most (63%) of the Surinamese and Dutch-Antillean respondents with a high intention and low self-efficacy, perceived higher barriers regarding testing than their peers with a low intention and high self efficacy. This finding indicates that we are probably dealing with temporal construal [22]; the respondents with a high intention are thinking the behavior through in more detail, because getting tested is relevant for them. However, because these people are analyzing the behavior in detail, they perceive more barriers and show low self efficacy [23]. People who showed high self-efficacy and low intention could be people for whom getting tested is not that relevant. These people could then perceive getting tested as easy, because it does not apply to them. In this case, the negative correlation between self-efficacy and the intention to get tested is caused by the intention, and not by selfefficacy. Therefore, it would be inappropriate to enclose self-efficacy in the model with variables which do predict (i.e. cause) the intention.. 6. Reviewer 1 asked for an explanation on why the strange correlation between religion and intention did not lead to removal from the model, whereas self efficacy was removed. This answer has become irrelevant after using a p-value <.0,1 because religion was automatically removed from the analysis after applying this p-value.

7. Reviewer 1 alerted the researchers on the fact that inviting everyone from the target group for a test, does not automatically mean that everyone will actually get tested. The reviewer also asked what the proposal of using a social norm approach means in the context of our study, and what the reference to the Larimer study (which used multiple points-of-entry) means in terms of the proposed intervention. First, we want to endorse the fact that inviting everyone to get a test does not mean that every will get tested. However, we believe that inviting everyone from these communities could lower the barrier to get a test, as testing was requested by a governmental facility for everyone. If people would actually get tested once, they will show higher intention to get another test as suggested by the findings of our study. We did adjust this in the Discussion section, to make it more clear what we aim to reach by inviting everyone: By inviting the whole Surinamese and Dutch-Antillean community in the Netherlands to get an STI/HIV test, for example yearly, stigmatization will be lower because no one can see whether or not you have had unprotected sex. People could simply state that they take up the invitation that they received from the testing facility. A similar invitation was sent to youngsters for the participation in a chlamydia screening project in the Netherland regardless whether these youngsters were sexually active or not [2, 31]. Over time, testing will become a social norm. Secondly, we tried to clarify the use of a social norm approach in the context of our study by reformulating the first part of the paragraph. The Larimer study was just a reference for interested people to illustrate how the SNA could be effectively used. However, after reformulating the paragraph the comparison had no additional value. Therefore, it was removed from the text: A possible solution could be found in interventions based on the social norms approach (SNA) [27]. This theory assumes that our behavior is influenced by the perceptions of the social environment on how to behave, and was used in the promotion of safer drinking [28], the prevention of sexual assault [29], safe driving, and smoking behavior [30]. SNA distinguishes three target audiences for an intervention: a whole community including those who are not at-risk (universal), members of a group at-risk (selective), and individuals at-risk. In terms of our study, it could be a good idea to start focusing on the universal audience. By inviting the whole Surinamese and Dutch-Antillean community in the Netherlands to get an STI/HIV test, for example yearly, stigmatization will be lower because no one can see whether or not you have had unprotected sex. People could simply state that they take up the invitation that they received from the testing facility. A similar invitation was sent to youngsters for the participation in a chlamydia screening project in the Netherland regardless whether these youngsters were sexually active or not [2, 31]. Over time, testing will become a social norm. Secondly,

the individuals at-risk should be targeted by providing them with accurate information on the importance of testing, normative feedback, and coping strategies to promote the desired behavior. By targeting these points-of-entry, targeting both the personal perceptions of individuals and the (social) environmental factors, a future intervention is more likely to effectively promote testing behavior Minor essential revisions 1. Reviewer 1 asked to elaborate whether the outcome of the construct subjective norms was the result of multivariate analysis, because it was not mentioned explicitly whereas the univariate analysis was (result paragraph in Abstract). We adjusted this in the Abstract: For both the Surinamese and Dutch-Antilleans, subjective norms were the most salient predictor of the intention to get tested in multivariate analyses, explaining 10% and 13% of the variance respectively 2. Reviewer 1 asked to specify which aspects of social norms health promotion programs should target, since the term social norms is very broad, and our study focuses on specific operationalizations (conclusion paragraph in Abstract). We took this remark into account: Health promoting programs should be aimed at promoting open communication regarding sexuality and testing. Stimulating each other to get tested frequently could also help achieving the desired behavior. 3. Reviewer 1 asked to report how the questionnaire was adapted after pretesting (end of the measurements section). In the Methods, we provide some more information on the adaption of the questionnaire: After the pre-test, some of the questions in the questionnaire were redefined for better understanding, such as use of language and removal of time frame of coming 6 months (except for the questions on intention to get tested). 4. Reviewer 1 suggests to replace generalizable in our statement that the results of the study may not be generalized to smaller sub-communities for applicable, since it refers to applying something that was found in a specific group (discussion section). We agree with the reviewer that applicable fits the context better. We have adjusted it in the manuscript.

5. Reviewer 1 argues with the remark of the researchers that the findings indicate that older community members should be included in an intervention. The reviewer feels that this finding indicates that the analyses should be stratified by age and see whether or not the difference is explained by one of the variables in the study. If so, the reviewer suggests that two interventions should be build that should tackle the crucial variables. The answer has become irrelevant as we corrected the regression analysis for the different recruitment waves. As a results of the correction, age was removed from the analysis, as described under point 1. Discretionary revisions All discretionary revisions suggested by reviewer 1 were taken into account in the manuscript Reviewer 2, Adrian Mindel 1. Reviewer 2 asked to confirm if the pretest was conducted among six Surinamese and six Dutch- Antilleans We can confirm that the pretest was conducted among both Surinamese and Dutch-Antilleans. However, more Surinamese pretested the questionnaire as described in the Methods: After the development of the questionnaire, it was pretested among seven Surinamese and five Dutch-Antilleans to see if the questions were understandable and valid. 2. Reviewer 2 argues the proxy of risk behavior we used, because some of the items used to build the variable may reflect the worried well. The reviewer asks to justify the use. We agree with the reviewer that our proxy of risk behaviour may label the worried-well as having risk behaviour. We have acknowledged this in the limitations of the Discussion section: Another point of consideration is that we measured a proxy of sexual risk behavior rather than the actual sexual risk behavior, because we felt that questioning the respondents directly on this intimate subject might have led to a dropout of respondents. We believe that the constructed variable is a reasonable proxy for risk behavior, because most respondents would only be afraid of being infected, or afraid of the test results, if they actually had had unsafe sex. However, the constructed variable of sexual risk behavior may have included respondents who perceive themselves as having been at risk while their actual risk was minimal, also known as the worried well [19]. 3. Reviewer 2 asked to explain what being religious means. We agree with the reviewer that in the previous version of the manuscript it was not clear how religion was defined. In the revised manuscript we now state in the Methods section:

religion (phrased as I am., with the answers categories Christian, Protestant, Catholic, Muslim, I have another belief, and I do not have a belief ) In the Methods we explain: Religion was also dichotomized by scoring all religions as 1 and no religion as 0. 4. Reviewer 2 asked to provide more information on what age group was most likely to get tested, as stating that the variable has a direct influence on the intention to get tested was not sufficient. The answer has become irrelevant as we corrected the regression analysis for the different recruitment waves. As a results of the correction, age was removed from the analysis. Although the variable age should have been entered into the models for both the Surinamese and Dutch-Antilleans, it was left out of the equations. The reason for this is related to the different recruitment ways (online survey versus pen-and-paper questionnaire, PPQ). After analyzing the correlation between intention and age for the online versus the PPQ group, we found that the correlation changes direction, for the Surinamese as well as the Dutch-Antilleans. We therefore felt it is inappropriate to include age in the regression analyses, the more because we aim to identify determinants of STI/HIV testing that can be changed with an intervention. Discretionary revisions Reviewer 2 pointed out that the healthcare provider performing the test could also influence the likelihood of testing by the target group and suggests that we consider this fact in the discussion. We agree with the reviewer that the healthcare provider performing the test is an important factor in testing behavior. However, we do not believe we have enough relevant data in the present study to hypothesize the influence of the health care provider. Therefore, we will not discuss this aspect in the Discussion. We will take this very important remark into account for the qualitative studies yet to be published, because those data do fit the hypothesis of the reviewer. All of the authors have read and approved the text as submitted. We hope that you will find the manuscript suitable for publication in BMC Public Health, and we are looking forward to hear from you soon. Yours sincerely, Alvin Henry Westmaas Maastricht University, Department of Work and Social Psychology P.O. Box 616, 6200 MD, Maastricht

The Netherlands Secretariat: +31 43 3881908 Email: alvin.westmaas@maastrichtuniversity.nl