Paired samples CFA for the multivariate detection of change in small samples

Similar documents
PSY 216: Elementary Statistics Exam 4

Repeated Measures ANOVA and Mixed Model ANOVA. Comparing more than two measurements of the same or matched participants

(C) Jamalludin Ab Rahman

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

A Brief (very brief) Overview of Biostatistics. Jody Kreiman, PhD Bureau of Glottal Affairs

Analysis of Variance: repeated measures

Overview of Non-Parametric Statistics

Lessons in biostatistics

Biostatistics II

Overview of Lecture. Survey Methods & Design in Psychology. Correlational statistics vs tests of differences between groups

Tutorial 3: MANOVA. Pekka Malo 30E00500 Quantitative Empirical Research Spring 2016

Study Guide for the Final Exam

Analysis of Variance (ANOVA)

THE STATSWHISPERER. Introduction to this Issue. Doing Your Data Analysis INSIDE THIS ISSUE

Statistics as a Tool. A set of tools for collecting, organizing, presenting and analyzing numerical facts or observations.

Using Mosaic Displays in Configural Frequency Analysis

Examining differences between two sets of scores

8/28/2017. If the experiment is successful, then the model will explain more variance than it can t SS M will be greater than SS R

A review of statistical methods in the analysis of data arising from observer reliability studies (Part 11) *

One-Way Independent ANOVA

Daniel Boduszek University of Huddersfield

Teaching A Way of Implementing Statistical Methods for Ordinal Data to Researchers

Chapter 23. Inference About Means. Copyright 2010 Pearson Education, Inc.

What you should know before you collect data. BAE 815 (Fall 2017) Dr. Zifei Liu

Day 11: Measures of Association and ANOVA

Bayesian and Frequentist Approaches

Basic Features of Statistical Analysis and the General Linear Model

Investigating the robustness of the nonparametric Levene test with more than two groups

10. LINEAR REGRESSION AND CORRELATION

MMI 409 Spring 2009 Final Examination Gordon Bleil. 1. Is there a difference in depression as a function of group and drug?

Preliminary Conclusion

Measures of Dispersion. Range. Variance. Standard deviation. Measures of Relationship. Range. Variance. Standard deviation.

PSYCHOLOGY 320L Problem Set #4: Estimating Sample Size, Post Hoc Tests, and Two-Factor ANOVA

Midterm Exam MMI 409 Spring 2009 Gordon Bleil

Basic Biostatistics. Chapter 1. Content

Prepared by: Assoc. Prof. Dr Bahaman Abu Samah Department of Professional Development and Continuing Education Faculty of Educational Studies

Nonparametric Linkage Analysis. Nonparametric Linkage Analysis

Intro to SPSS. Using SPSS through WebFAS

Applications. DSC 410/510 Multivariate Statistical Methods. Discriminating Two Groups. What is Discriminant Analysis

ANOVA in SPSS (Practical)

New Enhancements: GWAS Workflows with SVS

Main Study: Summer Methods. Design

From Bivariate Through Multivariate Techniques

Kidane Tesfu Habtemariam, MASTAT, Principle of Stat Data Analysis Project work

A COMPARISON BETWEEN MULTIVARIATE AND BIVARIATE ANALYSIS USED IN MARKETING RESEARCH

Common Statistical Issues in Biomedical Research

1. You want to find out what factors predict achievement in English. Develop a model that

Evaluation of CBT for increasing threat detection performance in X-ray screening

Evaluation of CBT for increasing threat detection performance in X-ray screening

Advanced ANOVA Procedures

Health Consciousness of Siena Students

Construct Validity of the MBTI in Management Development: A Test of Two Interpretations. Robert B. Kaiser & S. Bartholomew Craig

City, University of London Institutional Repository

bivariate analysis: The statistical analysis of the relationship between two variables.

Power of the test of One-Way Anova after transforming with large sample size data

Business Statistics Probability

SUMMER 2011 RE-EXAM PSYF11STAT - STATISTIK

Profile Analysis. Intro and Assumptions Psy 524 Andrew Ainsworth

Kepler tried to record the paths of planets in the sky, Harvey to measure the flow of blood in the circulatory system, and chemists tried to produce

Psychology Research Process

Choosing an Approach for a Quantitative Dissertation: Strategies for Various Variable Types

Summary & Conclusion. Lecture 10 Survey Research & Design in Psychology James Neill, 2016 Creative Commons Attribution 4.0

Increased Detection Performance in Airport Security Screening Using the X-Ray ORT as Pre-Employment Assessment Tool

Conditional Distributions and the Bivariate Normal Distribution. James H. Steiger

Statistics Guide. Prepared by: Amanda J. Rockinson- Szapkiw, Ed.D.

Assessing Measurement Invariance in the Attitude to Marriage Scale across East Asian Societies. Xiaowen Zhu. Xi an Jiaotong University.

Possible Joint Involvement of the Cochlea and Semicircular Canals in the Perception of Low-Frequency Tinnitus, Also Called The Hum or Taos Hum

Statistical analysis DIANA SAPLACAN 2017 * SLIDES ADAPTED BASED ON LECTURE NOTES BY ALMA LEORA CULEN

Learning Objectives 9/9/2013. Hypothesis Testing. Conflicts of Interest. Descriptive statistics: Numerical methods Measures of Central Tendency

Constructing a Bivariate Table:

Analysis and Interpretation of Data Part 1

List of Figures. List of Tables. Preface to the Second Edition. Preface to the First Edition

Reliability of Ordination Analyses

Quantitative Methods in Computing Education Research (A brief overview tips and techniques)

Type I Error Of Four Pairwise Mean Comparison Procedures Conducted As Protected And Unprotected Tests

Readings Assumed knowledge

Theoretical Exam. Monday 15 th, Instructor: Dr. Samir Safi. 1. Write your name, student ID and section number.

PSYCH-GA.2211/NEURL-GA.2201 Fall 2016 Mathematical Tools for Cognitive and Neural Science. Homework 5

Statistical Methods and Reasoning for the Clinical Sciences

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo

Survey research (Lecture 1) Summary & Conclusion. Lecture 10 Survey Research & Design in Psychology James Neill, 2015 Creative Commons Attribution 4.

Survey research (Lecture 1)

Designing Psychology Experiments: Data Analysis and Presentation

Analysis of data in within subjects designs. Analysis of data in between-subjects designs

Previously, when making inferences about the population mean,, we were assuming the following simple conditions:

Contingency Tables Summer 2017 Summer Institutes 187

Correlation and Regression

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

Research Methods II, Spring Term Logic of repeated measures designs

Ecological Statistics

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

Comparing 3 Means- ANOVA

The Impact of Continuity Violation on ANOVA and Alternative Methods

Choosing the Correct Statistical Test

C.3 Repeated Measures ANOVA

9/4/2013. Decision Errors. Hypothesis Testing. Conflicts of Interest. Descriptive statistics: Numerical methods Measures of Central Tendency

SPRING GROVE AREA SCHOOL DISTRICT. Course Description. Instructional Strategies, Learning Practices, Activities, and Experiences.

Statistics for EES Factorial analysis of variance

Transcription:

Psychology Science, Volume 47, 2005 (3/4), p. 440-446 Paired samples CFA for the multivariate detection of change in small samples MARCUS ISING 1 & WILHELM JANKE 2 Abstract Paired samples Configuration Frequency Analysis (CFA) introduced by Stemmler and Hammond (1997) is suggested as a valuable method for the multivariate detection of change in small sample designs. We examined the assumption that impulsive behavior in a Go/NoGo task, which is bivariately defined by increased speed and decreased accuracy, is not only determined by personality traits like impulsivity, but can also be manipulated by reinforcement conditions. By applying paired samples CFA we could show that impulsive behavior could be induced by positive reinforcement (reward), as well as suppressed by indirect punishment (loss of reward). We could confirm this result parametrically with an analysis of variance for repeated measures. A significant multivariate treatment effect on reaction latency and passive avoidance errors was obtained. We argue that paired sample CFA is an appropriate method for experimental and clinical small sample designs, which can be applied to data of all dignity levels (e.g. response latencies, rating scales, syndrome categories), does not imply assumptions about sample distributions, following a case oriented approach, which means that cases contributing to the significant effects can be directly identified from the cross-tabulation. In conclusion, paired samples CFA can be considered as a transparent, easy to interpret method for detecting changes in small sample within-subjects designs. Key words: Configuration Frequency Analysis (CFA), paired samples, multivariate analysis of change, small samples 1 Dr. Marcus Ising, Max-Planck Institut für Psychiatrie, Kraepelinstr. 10, D-80804 München, Germany; email: ising@mpipsykl.mpg.de 2 Prof. Dr. Wilhelm Janke, Institut für Psychologie, Röntgenring 10, D-97070 Würzburg, Germany; email: wilhelm.janke@t-online.de

Paired samples CFA for the multivariate detection of change in small samples 441 1. Introduction Stemmler and Hammond (1997) introduced a variant of the Configural Frequency Analysis (CFA; Lienert, 1969; von Eye, 1990) for paired samples as a distribution-free alternative to t-tests for dependent means or to analysis of variance for repeated measures. The paired samples CFA may be of value, especially, for detecting treatment effects in small sample pre-post or cross-over designs. A paired samples design offers a number of advantages compared to completely randomized designs. Trivially, all groups compared in a within-subject design are identical at the beginning of the study. In addition, the necessary number of subjects can be reduced by using within-subjects designs. Finally, these designs are statistically more sensitive to treatment effects since error variance caused by sample differences cannot impair the effect size of the study (Ray, 1993). The disadvantages of within-subjects designs are related to their time dependency. In pre-post treatment designs it cannot be decided whether differences are real treatment effects or time related effects, i.e., carry-over effects from the experience with a prior treatment, fatigue or practice. Therefore, time related effects should be counterbalanced, e.g., by applying a crossover design with the possible treatment orders permuted between subjects. Before conducting paired samples analyses in crossover designs it has to be assured that time related effects do not interfere with the treatment order. Otherwise, the groups of subjects with different treatment order have to be analyzed separately. We are presenting data from a small sample study testing the assumption that impulsive behavior, which is bivariately defined by increased speed and decreased accuracy, is not only determined by personality traits like impulsivity, but can also be manipulated by reinforcement conditions. We applied paired samples CFA and demonstrate the utility of this method in comparison to multivariate analysis of variance. 2. Psychometric data In a pilot study to the dissertation thesis of the first author (Ising, 1999) we examined whether impulsive behavior can be manipulated by positive reinforcement and indirect punishment. 25 male subjects (German psychology students) were included. They had to solve twice a Go/NoGo task under different reinforcement conditions in an experimental crossover design. In the Go/NoGo task a face-like sample stimulus had to be compared with three other face-like choice stimuli on a computer monitor. When the sample matched with one of the three choice stimuli a button had to be pressed as quickly as possible (Go condition). If none of the three choice stimuli was identical with the reference no reaction had to be shown (NoGo condition), and the subject had to wait until the next trial started. Impulsive behavior is bivariately defined by increased speed and decreased accuracy, which corresponds to short reaction latencies under Go and high rate of false reactions (passive avoidance errors) under NoGo (Gorenstein & Newman, 1980; Klinteberg et al., 1990-91). All subjects conducted the trials under two experimental conditions, positive reinforcement (C + ) and indirect punishment ( ) of impulsive behavior. 13 subjects started the Go/NoGo task under C + condition, that is, the subjects gained the highest amount of points when they pressed the button under Go (correct response) very quickly. The other twelve subjects conducted the task under condition, that is, the sub-

442 M. Ising & W. Janke jects lost the highest amount of points (up to four) when they pressed the button under NoGo (false response) very quickly. The three subjects with the best results won a gift certificate from a book store. After completing the trials subjects filled in questionnaires on subjective well being. Afterwards, they repeated the Go/NoGo task under the respective other experimental condition. Thus, all subjects conducted the task under both experimental conditions, the first half with the treatment order C +, the other half in the opposite order ( C +, crossover design). We want to show that impulsive behavior can be induced and suppressed in a Go/NoGo task by positive reinforcement (C + ) and indirect punishment ( ), respectively. Behavior under C + was assumed to be predominantly characterized by short reaction latencies under Go and a high number of passive avoidance errors under NoGo. Behavior under, however, should be characterized by high reaction latencies under Go and a low number of passive avoidance errors under NoGo. With a paired samples approach we can rule out that different personality traits interfere with the test outcome, since the same subjects are examined under both treatment conditions. 3. Testing for carry-over effects Before analyzing the effects of positive reinforcement and indirect punishment we examined the presence of carry-over effects, i.e., interactions between treatment order and treatment effects. For the CFA approach, we dichotomized the two dependent interval level variables, reaction latencies under Go and number of passive avoidance errors under NoGo, at the common median of both reinforcement conditions into '+' and '-' categories, and counted the frequencies of the C +, configuration of both variables separately for the two treatment orders, C + and C + (see Table 1). Chi-square tests did not show significant interactions with treatment order, neither for Go reaction latencies (Pearson Chi² (df = 3) = 4.75, p =.191) nor for NoGo passive avoidance errors (Pearson Chi² (df=3) = 5.55, p =.136). We additionally calculated exact Fisher tests for all configuration frequencies (++, +-, -+, --) by cross-classifying the frequencies of the two treatment orders for each of the four configurations with the sum of the respective three other configurations. The level of significance was adjusted to 0.0125 due to the fourfold testing in order to protect the experimentwise alpha level of 0.05. The results are presented in Table 1. No significant differences in C +, configuration frequencies between both treatment orders were observed suggesting that the combined analysis of both treatment orders is justified in the configural approach. We additionally performed an analysis of variance separately for the two response variables, reaction latencies under Go and passive avoidance errors under NoGo. Box M and separate Levene tests did not indicate a violation of the assumption of homogeneity between the two independent treatment order groups (p >.10). We did not observe significant interaction effects between treatment order and treatment effects for Go reaction latencies (F 1,23 = 1.27, p =.271). However, a significant interaction was found for the NoGo passive avoidance errors (F 1,23 = 6.74, p =.016) indicating a semi-disordinal effect with increased error

Paired samples CFA for the multivariate detection of change in small samples 443 Table 1: Configuration frequencies of Go reaction latencies and of NoGo passive avoidance errors under C + and condition for both treatment orders Go-Reaction Latencies C + C + C + p (Fisher) + + 5 1.194 + - 1 0 1.000 - + 4 5.880 - - 3 6.326 13 12 N = 25 Passive Avoidance Errors C + C + C + p (Fisher) + + 4 1.371 + - 2 7.067 - + 1 2 1.000 - - 6 3.496 13 12 N = 25 rate under C + only in the C + but not in the C + / group. We combined both treatment order groups for the subsequent comparison with paired samples CFA, although a carry over effect cannot be ruled out for NoGo passive avoidance errors. However, the treatment effects of the two order groups are not disordinal, but present only in one of the two order groups (semi-disordinal interaction), which means that merging of both treatment order groups might weaken but not invalidate the results for this variable. C 4. Assessment of treatment effects with paired samples CFA and MANOVA We assume that behavior under reinforcement (C + ) condition should be characterized by short reaction latencies under Go and a high number of passive avoidance errors under NoGo. On the other hand we suppose that behavior under indirect punishment ( ) condition should be characterized by high reaction latencies under Go and a low number of passive avoidance errors under NoGo. Therefore, we need a bivariate perspective in our statistical analysis since we are assuming change in both dependent variables simultaneously. Behavioral variables frequently present with a multimodal distribution which is a violation of an important prerequisite for parametric analysis. Paired samples CFA as suggested by Stemmler and Hammond (1997) is a promising approach for this kind of analysis, since (1) it does not imply assumptions about the distribution of the variables (distribution-free), and (2) it is a multivariate approach with variables combined to configurations. Paired samples CFA requires that the categories of both variables are paired to a 2 x 2 - dimensional configuration with both experimental conditions (C + / ) cross-classified. The configuration frequencies of the resulting (2 x 2) x (2 x 2) square contingency table are shown in Table 2.

444 M. Ising & W. Janke Table 2: Configuration frequencies of Go reaction latencies (L) and NoGo passive avoidance errors (E) under C + and condition L E + + + - - + - - R C + + + 0 1 0 1 2 + - 0 5 0 0 5 - + 2 4 3 3 12 - - 1 2 1 2 6 C 3 12 4 6 N = 25 We compare each of the four behavioral reaction patterns (++, +-, -+, --) pairwise between the C + and condition according to Lehmacher's marginal homogeneity sign test (Lehmacher, 1980). The Lehmacher test requires the elimination of the no-change cases located in the cells of the main diagonal of Table 2, since they do not contribute to a changerelated test hypothesis (see Siegel & Castellan, 1988, Ch. 5.2). Therefore, we need to calculate corrected marginals F i. and G.i for all of the four behavioral reaction patterns Fi. = Ri. fii, G = C f,. i. i ii with R i. / C.i = row/column marginals, f ii = main diagonal frequencies, and i = 1 (1) 4. The resulting frequencies of F i. and G.i are shown in Table 3. F i. corresponds to the number of changes from the i th reaction pattern under C + to any other under, G.i corresponds to the number of changes from the i th reaction pattern under to any other under C +. Table 3: Paired samples CFA with Go reaction latencies (L) and NoGo passive avoidance errors (E) under C + and condition L E F i. (C + ) G.i ( ) p (Fisher) + + 2 0 = 2 3 0 = 0 1.000 + - 5 5 = 0 12 5 = 7.006 - + 12 3 = 9 4 3 = 1.005 - - 6 2 = 4 6 2 = 4 1.000 Σ 15 15 Under the Null-Hypothesis (H 0 ) of no change between the two experimental conditions the corrected table marginals F i. and G.i are identical for all of the four reaction patterns. We

Paired samples CFA for the multivariate detection of change in small samples 445 test the H 0 of no change between the two experimental conditions with Lehmacher's marginal sign test. It can be performed exactly by applying the Fisher test for each of the r = 4 rows in Table 3. We cross-classified the two corrected marginals for each of the four reaction patterns with the sum of the respective three other patterns, and calculated exact Fisher tests for the resulting 2 x 2 tables. Without specifying an alternative to H 0 we need to correct for multiple testing in order to protect experiment-wise alpha. We applied the Bonferroni procedure and adjusted the significance level α* to.0125 (0.05/4). We see from Table 3 that subjects showing high Go reaction latencies and a low rate of passive avoidance errors under NoGo are typical for the condition (7 vs. 0, p =.006), while the complementary pattern (low reaction latencies and a high rate of passive avoidance errors) is characteristic for C + (9 vs. 1, p =.005). We additionally conducted a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with reaction latencies under Go and passive avoidance errors under NoGo as dependent variables and the treatment conditions C + vs. as repeated measures factor. Since we assume simultaneous treatment effects on both dependent variables we focused on the multivariate Wilks Lambda test, which was significant with a Lambda of.628 and a resulting F 2,23 score of 6.82 (p <.006). Mean value of the two dependent variables descriptively suggest a similar outcome pattern as detected with the paired samples CFA, i.e., decreased Go reaction latencies combined with an increased rate of NoGo passive avoidance errors under C + compared to. 5. Discussion By applying paired samples CFA we could show that impulsive behavior in a Go/NoGo task, which is defined by a pattern of short Go reaction latencies and elevated rate of NoGo passive avoidance errors, could be induced by positive reinforcement (reward), as well as suppressed by indirect punishment (loss of reward). This confirms our assumption that impulsive behavior is not only determined by personality traits like impulsivity (Klinteberg et al., 1990-91) but also by situational determinants like reinforcement and punishment conditions. We could confirm this result parametrically with an analysis of variance. A significant multivariate treatment effect on reaction latency and passive avoidance errors was obtained. Albeit parametric tests can be regarded as superior in power, the following points underscore the utility of configural approaches like the paired samples CFA in experimental and clinical small sample designs: (1) Paired samples CFA is applicable to data of all dignity levels (e.g. response latencies, rating scales, syndrome categories), and (2) it is distributionfree, that is, there are no assumptions about the underlying sample distribution. Another advantage of paired samples CFA compared to parametric and also other non-parametric methods (e.g., like log linear modeling) is that it follows (3) a case oriented approach (Bergman et al., 1991; Magnusson, 1985), which means that cases contributing to the significant effects can be directly identified from the cross-tabulation. We conclude that paired samples CFA can be considered as a transparent, easy to interpret method for detecting changes in small sample within-subjects designs.

446 M. Ising & W. Janke References 1. Bergman, L. R., Eklund, G. & Magnusson, D. (1991). Studying individual development: Problems and methods. In D. Magnusson, L. R. Bergman, G. Rudinger and B. Törestadt (Eds.), Problems and methods in longitudinal research. Stability and change (pp. 1-27). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 2. Gorenstein, E. E. & Newman, J. P. (1980). Disinhibitory psychopathology: A new perspective and a model for research. Psychological Review, 87, 301-315. 3. Ising, M. (1999). Prädiktion impulsiven Verhaltens unter Aktivierungs- und Hemmungsbedingungen. Lengerich: Pabst. 4. Klinteberg, B., Oreland, L., Hallman, J., Wirsen, A., Levander, S. E. & Schalling, D. (1990-91). Exploring the connections between platelet monoamine oxidase activity and behavior: relationships with performance in neuropsychological tasks. Neuropsychobiology, 23, 188-196. 5. Lehmacher, W. (1980). Simultaneous sign tests for marginal homogeneity of square contingency tables. Biometrical Journal, 22, 795-798. 6. Lienert, G. A. (1969). [The "Configuration Frequency Analysis" as method for classification in Clinical Psychology]. In M. Irle (Hrsg.), Bericht über den 26. Kongreß der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie in Tübingen 1968 (S. 244-253), Göttingen: Hogrefe. 7. Magnusson, D. (1985). Implications of an interactional paradigm for research on human development. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 8, 115-137. 8. Ray, W. J. (1993). Methods toward a science of behavior and experience. (4 th ed.). Pacific Grove: Brooks/Cole. 9. Siegel, S. & Castellan, N. Jr. (1988). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw Hill. 10. Stemmler, M. & Hammond, S. (1997). Configural frequency analysis of dependent samples for intra-patient treatment comparisons. Studia Psychologica, 39, 167-175. 11. von Eye, A. (1990). Introduction to configural frequency analysis. The search for types and antitypes in cross-classification. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.