Dopamine modulation of prefrontal delay activity - Reverberatory activity and sharpness of tuning curves

Similar documents
Different inhibitory effects by dopaminergic modulation and global suppression of activity

Basics of Computational Neuroscience: Neurons and Synapses to Networks

Modeling Depolarization Induced Suppression of Inhibition in Pyramidal Neurons

Part 11: Mechanisms of Learning

Lecture 22: A little Neurobiology

A neural circuit model of decision making!

Rolls,E.T. (2016) Cerebral Cortex: Principles of Operation. Oxford University Press.

A Biophysical Model of Cortical Up and Down States: Excitatory-Inhibitory Balance and H-Current

Predictive Features of Persistent Activity Emergence in Regular Spiking and Intrinsic Bursting Model Neurons

Elliot D. Menschik, Shih-Cheng Yen, and Leif H. Finkel

Synfire chains with conductance-based neurons: internal timing and coordination with timed input

VS : Systemische Physiologie - Animalische Physiologie für Bioinformatiker. Neuronenmodelle III. Modelle synaptischer Kurz- und Langzeitplastizität

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories?

Cellular mechanisms of information transfer: neuronal and synaptic plasticity

Exploring the Functional Significance of Dendritic Inhibition In Cortical Pyramidal Cells

Long-term depression and recognition of parallel "bre patterns in a multi-compartmental model of a cerebellar Purkinje cell

Holding Multiple Items in Short Term Memory: A Neural Mechanism

Model-Based Reinforcement Learning by Pyramidal Neurons: Robustness of the Learning Rule

Dynamic Stochastic Synapses as Computational Units

You submitted this quiz on Sun 19 May :32 PM IST (UTC +0530). You got a score of out of

Why is our capacity of working memory so large?

Neurobiology: The nerve cell. Principle and task To use a nerve function model to study the following aspects of a nerve cell:

Investigation of Physiological Mechanism For Linking Field Synapses

The firing rates of cortical neurons carry information about

Input-speci"c adaptation in complex cells through synaptic depression

Evaluating the Effect of Spiking Network Parameters on Polychronization

Synaptic Transmission: Ionic and Metabotropic

Cellular Bioelectricity

Perturbations of cortical "ringing" in a model with local, feedforward, and feedback recurrence

Axon initial segment position changes CA1 pyramidal neuron excitability

File name: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Table and Supplementary References

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Supplementary Figure 1

QUIZ YOURSELF COLOSSAL NEURON ACTIVITY

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010

Dendritic compartmentalization could underlie competition and attentional biasing of simultaneous visual stimuli

Working models of working memory

Decision-making mechanisms in the brain

Computational cognitive neuroscience: 2. Neuron. Lubica Beňušková Centre for Cognitive Science, FMFI Comenius University in Bratislava

Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP. Reading: BCP Chapter 25

Neuroscience: Exploring the Brain, 3e. Chapter 4: The action potential

Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140

Bursting dynamics in the brain. Jaeseung Jeong, Department of Biosystems, KAIST

ASSOCIATIVE MEMORY AND HIPPOCAMPAL PLACE CELLS

Neuromorphic computing

MOLECULAR BIOLOGY OF DRUG ADDICTION. Sylvane Desrivières, SGDP Centre

Resonant synchronization of heterogeneous inhibitory networks

When cells are already maximally potentiated LTP is occluded.

Chapter 11 Introduction to the Nervous System and Nervous Tissue Chapter Outline

Heterogeneous networks of spiking neurons: self-sustained activity and excitability

Antiepileptic agents

The Role of Mitral Cells in State Dependent Olfactory Responses. Trygve Bakken & Gunnar Poplawski

Simulating inputs of parvalbumin inhibitory interneurons onto excitatory pyramidal cells in piriform cortex

A Dynamic Neural Network Model of Sensorimotor Transformations in the Leech

GABAA AND GABAB RECEPTORS

CYTOARCHITECTURE OF CEREBRAL CORTEX

A U'Cortical Substrate of Haptic Representation"

What is working memory? (a.k.a. short-term memory) Sustained activity, Working Memory, Associative memory. Sustained activity in PFC (1) Readings:

Active Control of Spike-Timing Dependent Synaptic Plasticity in an Electrosensory System

A Neurocomputational Theory of the Dopaminergic Modulation of Working Memory Functions

Synaptic Plasticity and the NMDA Receptor

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Synaptic Plasticity and Memory

Dopamine-Mediated Stabilization of Delay-Period Activity in a Network Model of Prefrontal Cortex

Physiology of synapses and receptors

Shadowing and Blocking as Learning Interference Models

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Dopamine modulation in a basal ganglio-cortical network implements saliency-based gating of working memory

Multi compartment model of synaptic plasticity

Temporally Asymmetric Hebbian Learning, Spike Timing and Neuronal Response Variability

3) Most of the organelles in a neuron are located in the A) dendritic region. B) axon hillock. C) axon. D) cell body. E) axon terminals.

Cell Responses in V4 Sparse Distributed Representation

Modeling the Primary Visual Cortex

Multiple-object Working Memory A Model for Behavioral Performance

1) Drop off in the Bi 150 box outside Baxter 331 or to the head TA (jcolas).

STRUCTURAL ELEMENTS OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM

Recurrently Connected Silicon Neurons with Active Dendrites for One-Shot Learning

Using An Expanded Morris-Lecar Model to Determine Neuronal Dynamics In the Event of Traumatic Brain Injury

Modeling of Hippocampal Behavior

The storage and recall of memories in the hippocampo-cortical system. Supplementary material. Edmund T Rolls

What is Anatomy and Physiology?

Neurons: Structure and communication

Lisa M. Giocomo & Michael E. Hasselmo

Noise in attractor networks in the brain produced by graded firing rate representations

Chapter 6 subtitles postsynaptic integration

Anatomy of the basal ganglia. Dana Cohen Gonda Brain Research Center, room 410

Modeling Excitatory and Inhibitory Chemical Synapses

A Computational Model of Prefrontal Cortex Function

Activity-Dependent Development II April 25, 2007 Mu-ming Poo

Shunting Inhibition Does Not Have a Divisive Effect on Firing Rates

The Nervous System. Nervous System Functions 1. gather sensory input 2. integration- process and interpret sensory input 3. cause motor output

Study Guide Answer Key Nervous System

EE 791 Lecture 2 Jan 19, 2015

Spike-Timing Theory of Working Memory

Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings. Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014

Why do we have a hippocampus? Short-term memory and consolidation

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

Mechanisms underlying the basal forebrain enhancement of top-down and bottom-up attention

Report. An In Vivo Assay of Synaptic Function Mediating Human Cognition

Copyright Dr. Franklin B. Krasne, Swimmy

How has Computational Neuroscience been useful? Virginia R. de Sa Department of Cognitive Science UCSD

Transcription:

Dopamine modulation of prefrontal delay activity - Reverberatory activity and sharpness of tuning curves Gabriele Scheler+ and Jean-Marc Fellous* +Sloan Center for Theoretical Neurobiology *Computational Neurobiology Laboratory The Salk Institute 10100 N. Torrey Pines Road - La Jolla, CA 92037 Recent electrophysiological experiments have shown that dopamine (D1) modulation of pyramidal cells in prefrontal cortex reduces spike frequency adaptation and enhances NMDAtransmission. Using four models, from multicompartmental to integrate and fire, we examine the effects of these modulations on sustained (delay) activity in a reverberatory network. We find that D1 modulation may enable robust network bistability yielding selective reverberation among cells that code for a particular item or location. We further show that the tuning curve of such cells is sharpened, and that signal-to-noise ratio is increased. We postulate that D1 modulation affects the tuning of ''memory fields'' and yield efficient distributed dynamic representations. Introduction Recordings from the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of behaving monkeys on delayed visuospatial tasks, have shown enhanced neuronal firing during the delay period in a subset of neurons that represent the specific object location to be memorized while other cells, coding for different spatial locations were slightly inhibited [6,11]. This delay activity, as well as the overall performance on this task, but not on a simpler sensorimotor task, was decreased by local infusion of D1 receptor antagonists [10]. There have been several computational models of delay activity in prefrontal cortex. Most use a reverberatory mode of activity that relies on specific predefined synaptic patterns to achieve a stable state of firing activity [1,2,18]. Because of the transient nature of working memory, it is however likely that attractor-type synaptic weight patterns might not have the time to form, in order to store a specific memory item. An alternative view has recently been proposed that involves network bistability, through NMDA receptor activation [5,15]. According to this view, specific patterns of active cells can be dynamically created and reset in few hundreds milliseconds, while synaptic weights remain unchanged. We present here three models from single cell to large network that illustrate this idea. In a fourth model we show that this mechanism contributes to the sharpening of the tuning curves of an attractor-based network that stores 5 features. Results In a multicompartmental model of a reconstructed pyramidal cell, we have shown that the voltagedependence of NMDA channels was sufficient to induce network bistability [5]. The model neuron received 150 AMPA/NMDA and 30 GABAA presynaptic Poisson distributed synaptic inputs. Excitatory inputs were uniformly distributed on the dendritic tree (1 Hz average individual discharge), while inhibitory inputs were perisomatic (5 Hz average individual discharge).intrinsic currents included INa, IK, IAHP, ICa, and a calcium pump in the soma, and INa, INap, IM in the dendrites (kinetics were tuned for 36 oc, and are available upon request). In this model, the synaptic inputs were not sufficient to induce spiking when the cell was initially at rest (about -70 mv). However, a brief somatic depolarization was sufficient to depolarize the distal dendrites to the point were the incoming NMDA current was strong enough to maintain the firing of the cell for several seconds after the somatic depolarization was terminated (Fig 1A).

Figure 1: Multi compartmental models of NMDA induced network bistability. A: Reconstructed cell bombarded by AMPA/NMDA and GABAA synaptic inputs. A brief somatic current injection is sufficient to depolarize the dendrites of this cell, so that the incoming NMDA current maintains enough overall depolarization so that the cell keeps on firing. Firing is stopped by a brief hyperpolarization. B: Fully connected network of simplified multi-compartment cells including 10 pyramidal cells, and 2 interneurons. All synaptic weights between pyramidal cells are equal. 1) The introduction of D1 modulation reduces the spontaneous firing rates of pyramidal cells, and increases the spontaneous firing rate of the interneurons (not shown). 2) Without D1 modulation, a brief somatic depolarization applied simultaneously to 5 pyramidal cells fails to increase their firing rate. 3) In modulated conditions, the same depolarization as in 2) yields reverberatory activity among the simulated group, while cells which were not depolarized are slightly inhibited (4). Reverberatory activity is terminated by a brief hyperpolarization applied to the 5 pyramidal cells.

The bistability in this model depended on a sufficient density of NMDA channels and a significant contribution of INap to the intrinsic excitability of the cell. The sustained firing of the cell could be terminated by a brief hyperpolarization (Fig 1A), or anincoming volley of inhibitory inputs (30 Hz average,200 ms). Modifying the strength of GABAergic andnmda conductances could be used to modulate the frequency of discharge of the cell within the 20-45 Hz range. Lower firing frequencies could not be sustained for several seconds, and higher frequencies resulted in the inactivation of sodium channels and the termination of spiking in the depolarized state. This model showed further that this bistability was robust to significant variations in synaptic parameters (up to 24% in frequency, and up to 19 % in conductances). This model was then morphologically simplified and reduced to 7 compartments. Calcium currents were removed and the conductance of IM was increased to re-establish spike frequency adaptation. A generic 3 compartmental interneuron model was then built (Ina and IK only). A fully connected network of 10 pyramidal cells and 2 interneurons was then set up. Synaptic connections included AMPA/NMDA and GABA-A synapses (one per pair, Fig 1B). Somatic noise current was added to all cells to yield a spontaneous firing rate of about 5-7 Hz in pyramidal cells, in nonmodulated conditions. D1 modulation was simulated as a 45% reduction of IM, a 10% increase in INap, and a 40% increase in NMDA conductances, in accordance with recent experimental findings [12,17]. D1 modulation resulted in an overall lowering of the spontaneous firing rate of all pyramidal cells in the network (Fig 1B1). Five pyramidal cells were then briefly depolarized. In the modulated condition, this depolarization yielded sustained reverberating firing (~20-30 Hz) among the chosen cells, while the other pyramidal cells were inhibited by feedback inhibition from the interneurons (Fig 1B3,4 downregulated to <1 Hz, all pyramidal cells fired 2-5 Hz before stimulation). In nonmodulated condition, the pyramidal cell activity returned to baseline firing (5-7 Hz) after a short transient (Fig 1B2). These simulation show that network bistability can support delayed-sustained activity in a manner very similar to what is observed experimentally [3,7]. We then used two simplified models to study the effect of dopamine modulation of pyramidal neurons on stability of firing and sharpness of tuning curves during delay period activity in a large network. In the first model, we used a population of 1000 pyramidal neurons and 200 interneurons with a synaptic connectivity of 10% and uniform synaptic weights. Interneurons were modeled as basic integrate-and-fire units that match the physiological behavior of fast-spiking interneurons and GABA-A transmission[4]. Synaptic transmission was modulated by changing the probability of release (''synaptic reliability'', [13]), and including synaptic depression. We modeled synaptic depression with a time course of 200 ms and a percentage of reduction of 30%, and assumed it to be present at the output of both pyramidal cells and interneurons [4]. For pyramidal neurons we used an enhanced version of the integrate-and-fire neuron (''temporal integrate-and-fire neurons'', [14]) which was fitted to experimental measurements of unmodulated, deep-layer pyramidal cells in rat prefrontal cortex (with spike frequency adaptation (SFA)) and D1 modulated cells (no SFA). Finally, we connected pyramidal cells with 80% fast glutamatergic synapses (AMPA) and 20% NMDA synapses, and used two variants for the NMDA receptor: without D1 receptor activation, and with D1 receptor activation in accordance to experimental data [12,17]. The global reduction of glutamatergic output was modeled by a uniform 50% reduction of synaptic weights. This reduction proved sufficient to counterbalance the increase in activity due to D1 modulation of intrinsic properties.

Figure 2: A. Delay activity after a cue input of 250 ms in a network of 1000 pyramidal cells and 200 interneurons. Right: nonmodulated neurons, mean firing rate 5Hz (background activity). Left: D1 receptor modulated neurons, mean firing rate 45 Hz. B. Tuning curve in a network of 500 pyramidal cells and 100 interneurons. Five features were stored in the network, so that each feature involved about 60 pyramidal cells. The first feature (coded by the first 60 pyramidal cells) was presented to the network. Left: maximal firing rate of the first 110 neurons (bin width 10ms) in unmodulated condition. The tuning curve is broad and shallow (neurons 1-60). Right: Maximal firing rate of the same neurons in modulated conditions. The tuning curve is sharp, and signal-to-noise is increased.

We then compared a set of neurons without D1 receptor activation and a set of neurons with D1 receptor activation. When both groups of neurons are unconnected and each is given a brief input (250ms) comparable to cue activity, we can see the difference in the stability and duration of the signal. In the D1 modulated group of neurons, activity is kept up for at least 2 s and results in high frequency interactions between neurons (40-60 Hz) (Fig. 2A right). In the control condition (no D1 modulation) an input signal results in a strong response that gradually fades within 100-800ms, depending on the strengths of synaptic connections (Fig. 2A left). Changing synaptic reliability (0.90.1) as a source of internal noise has little effect on the D1modulated network up to 0.5, but results in a considerable disturbance of processing in the control network. Looking at the contribution of individual parameters, we found that the modification of temporal dependence of neuronal processing due to the suppression of SFA and synaptic depression is sufficient to keep up delay activity, and NMDA enhancement can be supplanted by increasing synaptic strengths for AMPA or NMDA receptors. However, we found that neurons fire less continuously and less synchronously without NMDA enhancement. For the second model, we built a network of 500 pyramidal cells and 100 interneurons. We manually tuned the synaptic weights to achieve an attractor-like representation for five features and we then computed their tuning curves (''memory fields''). Each feature involved a population of 60 pyramidal cells. There have been indications that the shape of tuning curves in prefrontal cortex is influenced by D1receptor modulation [8,16] and it can be understood as an effect of selective 'attentional' enhancement for currently active features [9]. We looked at this question using our model of D1 activation on pyramidal neurons. D1 modulation alone, by enabling higher frequencies for strongly activated neurons without affecting weakly activated neurons, increased the sharpness of tuning curves (Fig. 2B right, compared to left). Through feedback inhibition, the firing rates of neurons participating in the coding of other features were actually decreased, in effect increasing signal-to-noise ratio. The effect of dopamine modulation on locally tuned interneurons [7] probably raises inhibition by depolarizing the cells, and is expected to synergistically contribute to the sharpening of tuning curves. Conclusion Working memory tasks require that memory items are temporarily stored by a population of cells. Experimental findings showed that prefrontal cortex cells elevate their firing during delay period activity if the memory item matches the cell preference. This elevation of firing rate depends on the co-activation of D1 dopamine receptors. Unlike short-term or longterm memory, working memory is a transient and flexible phenomenon that is activated and reset within a few hundreds milliseconds. This suggests that longterm synaptic plasticity such as LTP or LTD may not support this form of memory, and that the storing of memory reside in the dynamical binding of spatially specific populations of neurons whose memory field matches the sensory input to store. We presented here a mechanism by which a specific subset of cells can be activated for several seconds following the brief presentation of a stimulus. This mechanism relies on the network bistability introduced by the effects of D1-like receptor activation of intrinsic and synaptic properties. In this regime, a subpopulation of cells is able to maintain an elevated rate of firing through reverberatory activity that is established and reset in few hundreds of milliseconds. Detailed and simplified multicompartmental techniques have been used to implement this mechanism, and show its feasibility.

Our results show that spatially specific patterns of activity can be robustly created and reset in a fully symmetric network (all synaptic weights equal), and that the D1 modulation of NMDA and INap currents is crucial for its implementation. A simplified model consisting of integrate-and-fire neurons further showed that, if synaptic weights are biased to store classical attractors, D1 modulation was able to sharpen the tuning curve of a specific attractor, and increase signal-tonoise ratio, through feedback inhibition. This finding links the subcellular mechanisms of dopamine neuromodulations to physiological measurements made in behaving animals during working memory tasks, and may serve as a testbed for further experimental studies on the role of dopamine in working memory task in prefrontal cortex and attentional enhancement. References [1] D.J. Amit, S. Fusi, V. Yakovlev, Paradigmatic working memory (attractor) cell in IT cortex, Neural Comput 9 (1997) 1071-1092. [2] M. Camperi, X.J. Wang, A model of visuospatial working memory in prefrontal cortex: recurrent network and cellular bistability, J Comput Neurosci 5 (1998) 383-405. [3] P.S. Goldman-Rakic, The "psychic" neuron of the cerebral cortex, Ann N Y Acad Sci 868 (1999) 13-26. [4] A. Gupta, Y. Wang, H. Markram, Organizing principles for a diversity of GABAergic interneurons and synapses in the neocortex, Science 287 (2000) 273-278. [5] J.E. Lisman, J.-M. Fellous, X.-J. Wang, A role for NMDA-receptor channels in working memory, Nature Neuroscience 1 (1998) 273-275. [6] J. Quintana, J. Yajeya, J.M. Fuster, Prefrontal representation of stimulus attributes during delay tasks. I. Unit activity in cross-temporal integration of sensory and sensory- motor information, Brain Res 474 (1988) 211-221. [7] S.G. Rao, G.V. Williams, P.S. Goldman-Rakic, Destruction and creation of spatial tuning by disinhibition: GABA(A) blockade of prefrontal cortical neurons engaged by working memory, J Neurosci 20 (2000) 485-494. [8] S.G. Rao, G.V. Williams, P.S. Goldman-Rakic, Isodirectional tuning of adjacent interneurons and pyramidal cells during working memory: evidence for microcolumnar organization in PFC, J Neurophysiol 81 (1999) 1903-1916. [9] J.H. Reynolds, R. Desimone, The role of neural mechanisms of attention in solving the binding problem, Neuron 24 (1999) 19-29, 111-125. [10] T. Sawaguchi, P.S. Goldman-Rakic, The role of D1-dopamine receptor in working memory: local injections of dopamine antagonists into the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys performing an oculomotor delayed-response task, J Neurophysiol 71 (1994) 515-528. [11] T. Sawaguchi, I. Yamane, Properties of delay-period neuronal activity in the monkey dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during a spatial delayed matching-to-sample task, J Neurophysiol 82 (1999) 2070-2080. [12] J.K. Seamans, D. Durstewitz, T.J. Sejnowski, State-Dependence of Dopamine D1 Receptor Modulation in Prefrontal Cortex Neurons. In: Joint Symposium on Neural Computation, San Diego, CA, 1999, pp. 128-135. [13] C.F. Stevens, Y. Wang, Changes in reliability of synaptic function as a mechanism for plasticity, Nature 371 (1994) 704-707. [14] C.F. Stevens, A.M. Zador, Novel Integrate-andfire-like model of repetitive firing in cortical neurons. In: 5th Joint symposium on Neural Computation, Vol. 8, UCSD - LaJolla, 1998, pp. 172-177. [15] X.J. Wang, Synaptic basis of cortical persistent activity: the importance of NMDA receptors to working memory, J Neurosci 19 (1999) 9587-9603. [16] G.V. Williams, P.S. Goldman-Rakic, Modulation of memory fields by dopamine D1 receptors in prefrontal cortex, Nature 376 (1995) 572-575. [17] P. Zheng, X.X. Zhang, B.S. Bunney, W.X. Shi, Opposite modulation of cortical N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor-mediated responses by low and high concentrations of dopamine, Neuroscience 91 (1999) 527-535. [18] D. Zipser, B. Kehoe, G. Littlewort, J. Fuster, A spiking network model of short-term active memory, J Neurosci 13 (1993) 3406-3420.