A Note on False Positives and Power in G 3 E Modelling of Twin Data

Similar documents
Objectives. 6.3, 6.4 Quantifying the quality of hypothesis tests. Type I and II errors. Power of a test. Cautions about significance tests

Estimating shared copy number aberrations for array CGH data: the linear-median method

Decision Analysis Rates, Proportions, and Odds Decision Table Statistics Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) Analysis

Author's personal copy

Automatic System for Retinal Disease Screening

Presymptomatic Risk Assessment for Chronic Non- Communicable Diseases

Detecting Specific Genotype by Environment Interactions Using Marginal Maximum Likelihood Estimation in the Classical Twin Design

Do People s First Names Match Their Faces?

SIMULATIONS OF ERROR PROPAGATION FOR PRIORITIZING DATA ACCURACY IMPROVEMENT EFFORTS (Research-in-progress)

Chapter 4: One Compartment Open Model: Intravenous Bolus Administration

Dental X-rays and Risk of Meningioma: Anatomy of a Case-Control Study

Bayesian design using adult data to augment pediatric trials

The Application of a Cognitive Diagnosis Model via an. Analysis of a Large-Scale Assessment and a. Computerized Adaptive Testing Administration

Introducing Two-Way and Three-Way Interactions into the Cox Proportional Hazards Model Using SAS

Severe Psychiatric Disorders in Mid-Life and Risk of Dementia in Late- Life (Age Years): A Population Based Case-Control Study

The Model and Analysis of Conformity in Opinion Formation

Remaining Useful Life Prediction of Rolling Element Bearings Based On Health State Assessment

Regret theory and risk attitudes

Patterns of Inheritance

Origins of Hereditary Science

carinzz prophylactic regimens

Treating Patients with HIV and Hepatitis B and C Infections: Croatian Dental Students Knowledge, Attitudes, and Risk Perceptions

Child attention to pain and pain tolerance are dependent upon anxiety and attention

IN a recent article (Iwasa and Pomiankowski 1999),

Polymorbidity in diabetes in older people: consequences for care and vocational training

This is an author-deposited version published in: Eprints ID: 15989

Cognitive Load and Analogy-making in Children: Explaining an Unexpected Interaction

Risk and Rationality: Uncovering Heterogeneity in Probability Distortion

Randomized controlled trials: who fails run-in?

Title: Correlates of quality of life of overweight and obese patients: a pharmacy-based cross-sectional survey

Anchor Selection Strategies for DIF Analysis: Review, Assessment, and New Approaches

Assessment of Growth Using Mandibular Canine Calcification Stages and Its Correlation with Modified MP3 Stages

Sampling methods Simple random samples (used to avoid a bias in the sample)

An Intuitive Approach to Understanding the Attributable Fraction of Disease Due to a Risk Factor: The Case of Smoking

The vignette, task, requirement, and option (VITRO) analyses approach to operational concept development

STAT 200. Guided Exercise 7

Does Job Strain Increase the Risk for Coronary Heart Disease or Death in Men and Women?

Individual Differences in Processing Speed and Working Memory Speed as Assessed with the Sternberg Memory Scanning Task

Transitive Relations Cause Indirect Association Formation between Concepts. Yoav Bar-Anan and Brian A. Nosek. University of Virginia

Differences in the local and national prevalences of chronic kidney disease based on annual health check program data

Relating mean blood glucose and glucose variability to the risk of multiple episodes of hypoglycaemia in type 1 diabetes

Medical Center, Van der Boechorststraat 7, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands

SOME ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN BLOOD GROUPS AND DISEASE

Khalida Ismail, 1 Andy Sloggett, 2 and Bianca De Stavola 3

Computerized semen analysis: objective measurement of semen characteristics is biased by subjective parameter setting

Annie Quick and Saamah Abdallah, New Economics Foundation

The meaning of Beta: background and applicability of the target reliability index for normal conditions to structural fire engineering

Reinforcing Visual Grouping Cues to Communicate Complex Informational Structure

A model of HIV drug resistance driven by heterogeneities in host immunity and adherence patterns

Gender Differences and Predictors of Work Hours in a Sample of Ontario Dentists. Cite this as: J Can Dent Assoc 2016;82:g26

Sex-ratio distortion driven by migration loads

Acute Myocardial Infarction Mortality in Comparison with Lung and Bladder Cancer Mortality in Arsenic-exposed Region II of Chile from 1950 to 2000

Min Kyung Hyun. 1. Introduction. 2. Methods

Syncope in Children and Adolescents

Mendel Laid the Groundwork for Genetics Traits Genetics Gregor Mendel Mendel s Data Revealed Patterns of Inheritance Experimental Design purebred

Acute Myocardial Infarction Mortality in Comparison with Lung and Bladder Cancer Mortality in Arsenic-exposed Region II of Chile from 1950 to 2000

Merging of Experimental and Simulated Data Sets with a Bayesian Technique in the Context of POD Curves Determination

Research. Dental Hygienist Attitudes toward Providing Care for the Underserved Population. Introduction. Abstract. Lynn A.

Course: Animals Production. Unit Title: Genetics TEKS: 130.3(C)(6)(B) Instructor: Ms. Hutchinson. Objectives:

Linear Theory, Dimensional Theory, and the Face-Inversion Effect

Inconsistencies of echocardiographic criteria for the grading of aortic valve stenosis

Research Article ABSTRACT. Amanda Myhren-Bennett College of Nursing, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC 29208, USA

THE QUANTITATIVE GENETICS OF HETEROSIS. Kendall R. Lamkey and Jode W. Edwards INTRODUCTION

Since its introduction in 1975, extracorporeal membrane

The Mississippi Social Climate of Tobacco Control,

Cocktail party listening in a dynamic multitalker environment

COPD is a common disease. Over the prolonged, Pneumonic vs Nonpneumonic Acute Exacerbations of COPD*

An Algorithm for Probabilistic Least{Commitment Planning 3. Nicholas Kushmerick Steve Hanks Daniel Weld. University ofwashington Seattle, WA 98195

King s Research Portal

Internet-based relapse prevention for anorexia nervosa: nine- month follow-up

Constipation in adults with neurofibromatosis type 1

CRIKEY - A Temporal Planner Looking at the Integration of Scheduling and Planning

Correlation between pattern and mechanism of injury of free fall

Inadequate treatment of ventilator-associated and hospital-acquired pneumonia: Risk factors and impact on outcomes

Multilevel analysis quantifies variation in the experimental effect while optimizing power and preventing false positives

The Harris Benedict equation reevaluated: resting energy requirements and the body cell mass13

Online publication date: 01 October 2010

Theory of mind in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome

Family Dysfunction Differentially Affects Alcohol and Methamphetamine Dependence: A View from the Addiction Severity Index in Japan

Comparison of Water Seal and Suction After Pulmonary Lobectomy: A Prospective, Randomized Trial

Association of anxiety with body mass index (BMI) and waist to hip ratio (WHR) in medical students

Lecture 2: Linear vs. Branching time. Temporal Logics: CTL, CTL*. CTL model checking algorithm. Counter-example generation.

Citation for published version (APA): Lutgers, H. L. (2008). Skin autofluorescence in diabetes mellitus Groningen: s.n.

Application of a score system to evaluate the risk of malnutrition in a multiple hospital setting

SPECTRAL ENVELOPE ANALYSIS OF SNORING SIGNALS

Induced Mild Hypothermia for Ischemic Stroke Patients

The Mississippi Social Climate of Tobacco Control,

The Usage of Emergency Contraceptive Methods of Female Students in Hawassa University: A Case Study on Natural and Computational Science

in adolescent wrestlers.

American Journal of Epidemiology Copyright 2001 by The Johns Hopkins University School of Hygiene and Public Health All rights reserved

Performance of the Vocal Source Related Features from the Linear Prediction Residual Signal in Speech Emotion Recognition

BMC Medical Research Methodology

State-Trace Analysis of the Face Inversion Effect

Comparative study of anxiety and depression following maxillofacial and orthopedic injuries. Study from a Nigerian University Teaching Hospital

Surgical resection is the primary curative treatment modality

Effect of training frequency on the learning curve on the da Vinci Skills Simulator

TO help 25.8 million Americans [1] with diabetes, a growing

Quality of Life and Symptom Control in Patients with Cancer

Citation for published version (APA): Ebbes, P. (2004). Latent instrumental variables: a new approach to solve for endogeneity s.n.

Acne Itch: Do Acne Patients Suffer From Itching?

Transcription:

Behav Genet (01) 4:10 18 DOI 10.100/s10519-011-9480- ORIGINAL RESEARCH A Note on False Positives and Power in G E Modelling of Twin Data Sohie van der Sluis Danielle Posthuma Conor V. Dolan Received: 1 August 010 / Acceted: 14 June 011 / Published online: July 011 Ó The Author(s) 011. This article is ublished with oen access at Sringerlink.com Abstract The variance comonents models for gene environment interaction roosed by Purcell in 00 are widely used. In both the bivariate and the univariate arameterization of these models, the variance decomosition of trait T is a function of moderator M. We show that if M and T are correlated, and moderator M is correlated between twins as well, the univariate arameterization roduces a considerable increase in false ositive moderation effects. A simle extension of this univariate moderation model revents this elevation of the false ositive rate rovided the covariance between M and T is itself not also subject to moderation. If the covariance between M and T varies as a function of M, then moderation effects observed in the univariate setting should be interreted Edited by Stacey Cherny. S. van der Sluis (&) D. Posthuma Comlex Trait Genetics, Deartment of Functional Genomics, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), FALW-VUA, Neuroscience Camus Amsterdam, VU University Medical Center (VUmc), Amsterdam, The Netherlands e-mail: s.vander.sluis@vu.nl S. van der Sluis D. Posthuma Comlex Trait Genetics, Deartment of Clinical Genetics, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), FALW-VUA, Neuroscience Camus Amsterdam, VU University Medical Center (VUmc), Amsterdam, The Netherlands D. Posthuma Comlex Trait Genetics, Deartment of Medical Genomics, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), FALW-VUA, Neuroscience Camus Amsterdam, VU University Medical Center (VUmc), Amsterdam, The Netherlands C. V. Dolan Deartment of Psychology, FMG, University of Amsterdam, Roeterstraat 15, 1018 WB Amsterdam, The Netherlands with care as these can have their origin in either moderation of the covariance between M and T or in moderation of the unique aths of T. We conclude that researchers should use the full bivariate moderation model to study the resence of moderation on the covariance between M and T. If such moderation can be ruled out, subsequent use of the extended univariate moderation model, as roosed in this aer, is recommended as this model is more owerful than the full bivariate moderation model. Keywords Introduction G 9 E interaction Moderation Twins In the classical twin model, henotyic variance is decomosed into genetic and environmental variance comonents, which are usually assumed to be homoskedastic, i.e., constant across relevant environmental or genetic conditions. Heteroskedasticity will arise if the genetic and/ or environmental variance comonents vary in size as a function of a given variable, or moderator. Such a moderator can be truly environmental in nature (e.g., exosure to radiation from a nuclear lant, the level of iodine in soil or drinking water 1 ), or be a trait that itself is subject to genetic influences (e.g., eating or exercise habits, educational attainment level, ersonality traits). If moderators have a limited number of levels, their effects can be modelled in a multi-grou design. However, a multi-grou aroach does not naturally account for grou order, and 1 Note that such environmental factors could indeed be urely environmental, but could also in art be subject to genetic influences. For examle, the chance of exosure to radiation may deend on one s occuation or residential area, and such social-economic factors may again be under genetic influence. 1

Behav Genet (01) 4:10 18 11 quickly becomes imractical if the moderator is characterized by many levels (i.e., continuous in the extreme case). As few as, say, or 4 levels may already require a challenging number of grous, esecially if the moderator differs within twin airs (i.e., is not shared ), and the samle includes additional family members (e.g., arents, siblings, artners). In such circumstances, behavioural geneticists often turn to the moderation models roosed by Purcell (00). The oularity of these model is evident given its frequent use in twin studies on moderation in the context of, for instance, cognitive ability (Bartels et al. 009; Grant et al. 010; Harden et al. 00; Johnson et al. 009a; Turkheimer et al. 00; van der Sluis et al. 008), ersonality (Bartels and Boomsma 009; Brendgen et al. 009; Distel et al. 010; Hicks et al. 009a, b; Johnson et al. 009b; Tuvblad et al. 00; Zhang et al. 009), health (Johnson and Krueger 005; Johnson et al. 010; McCaffery et al. 008, 009), and brain morhology (Lenroot et al. 009; Wallace et al. 00). In both the univariate and the bivariate moderation models roosed by Purcell (00), the moderation effects are modelled directly on the ath loadings of the genetic (A), shared environmental (C) and nonshared environmental (E) variance comonents. In this moderation model, the variances of A, C, and E are fixed to 1 (standard identifying scaling), but the ath coefficients are modelled as (a? b a M i ), (c? b c M i ), and (e? b e M i ), resectively. In these exressions for the moderated loadings, a, c and e are intercets, i.e., the arts of the variance comonents that are indeendent of moderator M, M i is the value of the moderator for a secific twin i, and b a, b c, and b e, are the regression weights of the moderator for the genetic and the environmental variance comonents, resectively. In the standard homoskedastic ACE-model, the b coefficients are assumed to be zero. In the moderation model roosed by Purcell, the total variance of trait T is thus calculated as: VarðTjM i Þ ¼ ða þ b a M i Þ þðc þ b c M i Þ þðe þ b e M i Þ ð1þ for i = 1,, and the exected covariances within MZ and DZ twin airs are: Cov MZ ðt 1 ; T jm 1 ; M Þ¼ðaþb a M 1 Þða þ b a M Þ þðcþb c M 1 Þðc þ b c M Þ Cov DZ ðt 1 ; T j M 1 ; M Þ¼:5ðaþb a M 1 Þða þ b a M Þ þðcþb c M 1 Þðc þ b c M Þ: ðþ We limit our discussion to the ACE model. To ease resentation, we limit ourselves to the linear moderation model. We note, however, that non-linear effects of the moderator on variance comonents A, C and E are discussed by Purcell (00). Besides moderating the variance decomosition of trait T, the moderator itself may be correlated with the trait T via A, C, and/or E. The full bivariate moderation model (deicted in Fig. 1a), in which the variances of both trait T and moderator M, as well as their covariance, are decomosed into the three sources of variation (A, C, and E), allows one to test both the resence of moderation on the variance comonents unique to trait T, and the resence of moderation effects on the variance comonents common to trait T and moderator M, i.e., on the cross aths. Investigation of moderation of the covariance between the M and T, such as modeled via the cross aths, is of interest if one wishes to understand the nature of, or the rocess underlying, the relation between M and T. With 1 arameters (15 describing the variance art of the model: arameters unique to the moderator, to describe the covariance between T and M, and to describe the variance decomosition unique to T; and arameters describing the means art of the model: the estimated means of M and T, resectively), this bivariate moderation model describes the relations between T and M in great detail. In ractice, describing (decomosing) a small covariance between M and T with as much as arameters, can be comutationally challenging, and solutions can be quite sensitive to starting values. Also, Rathouz et al. (008) have shown that this model sometimes roduces surious moderation effects. More ractically, rograms like Mx (Neale et al. 00) do not allow the simultaneous modeling of categorical and continuous variables, which comlicates this bivariate arameterization of T and M if the two variables do not have the same measurement level. 4 Finally, when the moderator of interest is a family-level variable, i.e., a variable that is by definition equal for twin 1 and twin, such as socioeconomic status in childhood (SES) or arental educational attainment level, then a bivariate arameterization is infeasible as the moderator does not show any variation within families. For these reasons, researchers have resorted to what we call Purcell s (00) univariate moderation model (e.g., Bartels et al. 009; Bartels and Boomsma 009; Dick et al. 009; Distel et al. 010; Grant et al. 010; Harden et al. 00; McCaffery et al. 008, 009; Taylor et al. 010; Timberlake et al. 00; Turkheimer et al. 00; Wallace et al. 00). In this model, the moderator M is included in the means model of T as follows: T 1 ¼ b 0 þ b 1 M 1 ; ðþ T ¼ b 0 þ b 1 M ; 4 Discretizing either variable to render T and M comarable in scale entails a loss of information and is therefore undesirable. 1

1 Behav Genet (01) 4:10 18 (a) (b) Fig. 1 a Full bivariate moderation model for a twin air as roosed by Purcell (00). Ac, Cc, and Ec are the variance comonents common to the moderator M and the trait T; Au, Cu, and Eu are the variance comonents unique to T. All latent variables have unit variance. Path loadings for M are denoted by a m,c m, and e m. The loadings of the cross-aths connecting M to T consist of arts that are unrelated to moderator M, i.e., a c,c c, and e c, and arts that deend on M via weights b ac, b cc, and b ec. Similarly, the loadings of the aths unique to T consist of arts that are unrelated to M, i.e., a u,c u, and e u, and arts that deend on M via weights b a, b c, and b e. b Univariate moderation model for a twin air as roosed by Purcell (00). All latent variables have unit variance. Path loadings for T consist of arts that are unrelated to moderator M, i.e., a, c, and e, and arts that deend on M via weights b a, b c, and b e. M is also included in the means model of T, where b 0 denotes the intercet and b 1 the regression weight of T on M where T 1 and T are the trait values of twins 1 and, M 1 and M are their individual moderator values, b 0 is the intercet, and b 1 is the regression weight for moderator M. The arameters b 0 and b 1 are assumed to be equal across twins within a air, and across zygosity (Fig. 1b). With 8 arameters ( to describe the variance art of the model: related, and unrelated to the moderator; and arameters to describe the means model: the regression weight b 1 and the intercet b 0 ), this arameterization is more arsimonious than the bivariate moderation model, and often less suscetible to comutational roblems. In addition, low correlations between M and T, or different measurement levels of M and T, do not cause roblems in this univariate moderation model. It is imortant to realize that the bivariate moderation model considers the joint distribution of M and T, while the univariate moderation model considers moderation of the variance decomosition of T conditional on M. With M included in the means model of T, the univariate moderation model does not allow further investigation of the nature of the covariance between M and T but secifically focuses on the question whether the decomosition of the variance unique to T deends on M. Entering M in the means model of T to allow for a main effect is believed to effectively remove from the covariance model any (genetic) effects that are shared between trait and moderator (Purcell 00,. 5). In essence, the variance common to M and T is artialled out, and the moderator effects of M are modeled on the residual variance of T, T 0, i.e., the variance of T that was not shared with M. As a result, the effects that M has on the variance decomosition of the residual T 0 are believed to be indeendent of (i.e., not due to) any (unmodeled) (genetic) correlation between M and T (Purcell 00,. 5). However, although M 1 is indeed unrelated to the for- M 1 -corrected residual T 0 1, this residual T0 1 is not necessarily uncorrelated to the moderator M of the co-twin. In this aer, we first show that non-zero semi-artial correlations 1

Behav Genet (01) 4:10 18 1 between T 0 1 and M can result in a considerable increase in false ositive moderation effects on variance comonents A and C, esecially if the correlation between T and M runs fully (or redominantly) via E (rather than via A and/ or C). We subsequently study whether a simle extension of the univariate moderation model revents this increase of false ositive rate. In the first art of this aer, we focus on illustrations and simulations in which the correlation between trait T and moderator M runs either exclusively via A, or via C or via E. Although these settings may be considered quite secial, they conveniently simlify the exlanation of the roblem of non-zero semi-artial correlations in the univariate moderation model roosed by Purcell, and clarify how this model would need to be extended. In the subsequent investigation of the usefulness of this extended version of the univariate model, the simulations are extended to more realistic conditions. Semi-artial correlations Consider a moderator M and a trait T, both with variance 1 and mean 0, and measured in a samle of MZ and DZ twins. Suose that in both M and T, variance comonents A, C, and E account for 40%, 0%, and 0% of the variance, resectively, and that these ercentages are stable across the entire oulation (i.e., there is no moderation). This imlies that rmz t1,t = rmz m1,m =. and rdz t1,t = rdz m1,m =.5. Now suose that the cross-trait correlation between T and M equals.4 (i.e., r m1,t1 = r m,t =.4) and that the T M correlation is either exclusively due ffiffiffiffiffiffi to A (loading cross-ath equals :15 ), or to C (loading ffiffiffiffi cross-ath equals : ), or to E (loading cross-ath equals ffiffiffiffi : ; see Fig. a c). If the relation between T and M runs exclusively via E, then in both MZ and DZ twins, the cross-trait cross-twin correlation between M (the moderator of twin ) and T 1 (the trait of twin 1) is 0, just as the correlation between M 1 and T is 0, i.e., r m,t1 = r m1,t = 0. If the correlation between T and M runs via C, then the correlation between M and T 1, and between M 1 and T, is in both MZ and DZ ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi twins calculated as : : ¼ :4. Finally, if the correlation between T and M runs via A, then this correlation is ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi ffiffiffiffi ffiffiffiffiffiffi :4 :15 ¼ :4 in MZ twins and :4 :5 :15 ¼ :1 in DZ twins. Now suose that we want to investigate whether M moderates the variance decomosition of T, and rather than modeling this in a bivariate model (Fig. 1a), we choose to include M in the means model of T (Fig. 1b), such that we can study the moderation effects of M on the variance decomosition of the residual variance T 0. We thus regress T 1 on M 1 and T on M, and obtain residuals T 0 1 and T0. We know that T 0 1 and M 1 (and T 0 and M ) will be uncorrelated, but what is the correlation between the residual T 0 1 and M (or between T 0 and M 1)? The semi-artial correlation between T 0 1 and M (which equals the semi-artial correlation between T 0 and M 1), denoted as r m(t1m1),is calculated as 5 : r mðt1m1þ ¼ r t1;m r t1;m1 r m1;m q ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi : ð4þ 1 rt1;m1 Table 1 shows the correlations between T 1 and M, and the semi-artial correlations between T 0 1 and M for MZ and DZ twins in the three different scenarios (i.e., withintwin correlation between T and M runs either exclusively via A, exclusively via C, or exclusively via E). Clearly, as a result of artialling out M 1 from T 1, the semi-artial correlation between T 0 1 and M is lower than the correlation between T 1 and M. However, the semiartial correlation between T 0 1 and M is often not equal to zero: esecially if the correlation between T 1 and M was zero to begin with (i.e., if T and M are correlated via E), the semi-artial correlation between T 0 1 and M is quite large and negative. Estimated across an entire study samle (while weighing for the MZ/DZ ratio), these non-zero semiartial correlations can be quite considerable (e.g., in the case that T and M are correlated via E), and are likely to cause roblems in the univariate moderation model. After all, these non-zero semi-artial correlations, whether ositive or negative, will somehow need to be accommodated in the model. Considering the univariate moderation model as deicted in Fig. 1b, a non-zero semi-artial correlation between T 0 1 and M is most likely to be accommodated via the effects that M has on the variance comonents A and C, i.e., via b a and b c, as these are the only links between M and T 0 1, and M 1 and T 0, resectively. In Simulation study 1, we investigated first whether these non-zero semi-artial correlations do indeed cause roblems in the univariate moderation model. We exect roblems to be greatest if the semi-artial correlation deviates more from zero (i.e., in the case that T and M are correlated via E). Second, we investigated whether these roblems indeed manifest themselves mostly through b a and b c. Simulation study 1 To investigate the effect of artialling out M on T within each individual twin on the significance of arameters b a, 5 Note the difference between a artial correlation and a semi-artial correlation. The artial correlation between X and Y given Z, is the correlation between the residual X 0 and the residual Y 0, where Z is artialled out in both variables. The semi-artial correlation is the correlation between the residual X 0 and the uncorrected variable Y, i.e., Z is artialled out only in X but not in Y. 1

14 Behav Genet (01) 4:10 18 Fig. a c Bivariate models in which the correlation of *.4 between moderator M and trait T either exclusively runs via A (a), exclusively via C (b), or exclusively via E (c). The variance of both trait T and moderator M are for 40% due to A, for 0% to C, and for 0% due to E. All latent variables have unit variance (a) (b) (c) b c, and b e, we simulated data according to the models shown in Figs. a c, i.e., with correlations between T and M running either exclusively via A, exclusively via C, or exclusively via E. In these simulated data, T and M were both standard normally distributed. Also, T and M were correlated, but moderation effects of M on the cross aths 1

Behav Genet (01) 4:10 18 15 Table 1 Correlations and semi-artial correlations between M and T 1 if the within-twin correlation between T and M runs via A, via C, or via E r m,t1 r m(t1m1) T and M correlated via A MZ.4.04 DZ.1 0 T and M correlated via C MZ.4.04 DZ.4.14 T and M correlated via E MZ 0 -.1 DZ 0 -.14 Note:r m,t1 denotes the correlation between moderator of twin (M ), and the trait of twin 1 (T 1 ). r m(t1m1) denotes the semi-artial correlation between the moderator of twin (M ) and the trait of twin 1 (T 1 ) corrected for the moderator of twin 1 (M 1 ). In these calculations, the variances of both T and M were 40%, 0%, and 0% due to A, C, and E, resectively and on the variance comonents of the residual of T were absent. For each scenario, we simulated 000 datasets each comrising Nmz = Ndz = 500 airs. We then fitted to these datasets the standard univariate moderation model with moderator M modeled on the means (Fig. 1b), and then constrained either b a, b c,orb e to zero to test for the significance of each arameter individually, i.e., a 1-df test. The difference between the - log-likelihood of the full model (the secific moderation arameter estimated freely) and the - log-likelihood of the restricted model (the secific moderation arameter fixed to 0), denoted as v diff, is v -distributed. Since moderation arameters b a, b c, and b e were zero in these simulated data, we exect the distribution of the v diff as calculated across all 000 data sets to follow a central v (1) distribution. Given an nominal a of.05, we exected 5% of v diff test to be significant, i.e., larger than the critical value of.84. Figure shows PP-lots for the simulations in which T and M are correlated via A (uer art), via C (middle art) or via E (lower art). In these lots, the -values observed in the simulations are lotted against the nominal -values. The observed -value for each v diff -value observed in the 000 simulations, is calculated as the roortion of the remaining 1999 v diff -values that is equal to or larger than this secific v diff -value. The nominal -value for each v diff - value observed in the 000 simulations, is obtained by reference to the v (1)-distribution. Deviations from the 45 line show whether the use of the regular v (1) test would result in conservative (above the line) or liberal (below the line) decision. The figures also show the ercentage of hits calculated across the 000 simulations. Given a =.05, the ercentage of hits is exected to be close to 5%. Note that the standard error of the ML estimator of the -value in the simulations is calculated as sqrt( *(1-)/N), where denotes the ercentage of significant tests observed in the simulations (nominal -value) given a chosen a, and N the total number of simulations. The 95% confidence interval for a correct nominal -value of.05 (given a =.05) and N = 000 thus corresonds to CI-95 = ( - 1.9 * SE,? 1.9 * SE), and thus equals.04.0. This imlies that, given a =.05, any observed nominal -value outside the.04.0 range should be considered incorrect: -values \.04 suggest that the model is too conservative, while - values [.0 suggest that the model is too liberal. The results of Simulation study 1 as deicted in Fig. are summarized in Table, which also includes results for some additional simulations settings. In testing b a under these three scenarios, the number of false ositives (i.e., v diff tests [.84) was inflated if the correlation between T and M ran via A or C:.85 and.0%, rather than 5%, resectively. If the correlation between T and M ran via E, however, the false ositive rate was even more seriously inflated: 5.%. This inflation is clearly visible in Fig. (PP-lot lower left corner). Similar results were obtained for the tests of b c : if the correlation between T and M ran via A or C, the false ositive rate was.85 and 8.5%, resectively, while the false ositive rate was 55.% if the correlation between T and M ran via E. In testing for the significance of b e, the false ositive rate was only significantly elevated if the correlation between T and M ran via E (.50%), but not if the correlation ran via A or C (4.5 and 5.%, resectively). The additional simulations summarized in Table show that the false ositive rate of the univariate moderation model is a) correct if M 1 and M are unrelated, i.e., when the variance in M is comletely due to nonshared environmental influences E, b) slightly too low if M 1 and M are correlated 1, i.e., when the variance in M is comletely due to shared environmental influences C, and c) correct if the covariance between M and T runs in equal roortions via A, C and E. This latter result shows that the extent to which the false ositive rate of the univariate moderation model is elevated deends on the secific mix of, or ratio in which, A, C and E contribute to the covariance between M and T since the ositive and negative semiartial correlations as described in Table 1 can more or less cancel each other out. Summarizing, Simulation study 1 shows that under the univariate moderation model, in which T 1 is corrected for M 1 only, and T is corrected for M only, the false ositive rate can be (much) higher than the nominal a-level, esecially if the correlation between T and M runs redominantly via E. 1

1 Behav Genet (01) 4:10 18 Fig. PP-lots for the univariate moderation model in Simulation study 1, in which T and M are correlated exclusively via A (uer art), exclusively via C (middle art), or exclusively via E (lower art). Deviations from the 45 line show whether the use of the regular v (1) test would result in conservative (above the line) or liberal (below the line) decision. % hits denotes the ercentage of likelihood-ratio tests smaller than the critical value.84 (i.e., significant given a =.05). A hit rate of.05 is exected given a =.05, and given that moderation effects were absent in the data. Hit rates outside the.04.0 range should be considered incorrect (i.e., significantly too low or too high) Solution: extension of the univariate moderation model? In this section we aim to investigate whether we can solve the roblems that can result from non-zero semi-artial correlations by extending the univariate moderation model. An obvious solution is to extend the means model such that the trait value of twin 1 is not only corrected for the moderator value of twin 1, but also for any residual association to the moderator value of the co-twin, as this would result in a residual T1 00 (T00 ) that is uncorrelated to both M 1 and M. Taking into account the way regression coefficients in a multile regression model with two redictors are calculated, it is easy to show that the arameters in the means models should generally also differ across zygosity. 1

Behav Genet (01) 4:10 18 1 Table Results Simulation study 1: false ositive rates under Purcell s univariate moderation model Dro b a Dro b c Dro b e Settings Simulation study 1 r(t,m) =.4 via A only % hits.0.0.05 r(t,m) =.4 via C only % hits.08.09.05 r(t,m) =.4 via E only % hits.5.55.08 Additional simulations r(t,m) =.4 via A, C and E in equal roortions a % hits.04.05.0 r(t,m) =. via A, C and E in equal roortions b % hits.05.05.0 r(m 1,M ) = 0 c % hits.05.05.04 r(m 1,M ) = 1 d % hits.0.0.05 Note: The first three simulation settings are described under Simulation study 1. b a, b c, and b e denote the moderation arameters on the variance comonents unique to T (see Fig. 1). For all settings, 000 datasets were simulated and analyzed. % hits denotes the ercentage of likelihoodratio tests smaller than the critical value.84 (i.e., significant given a =.05). A hit rate of.05 is exected given a =.05, and given that moderation effects were absent in the data. Hit rates outside the.04.0 range should be considered incorrect (i.e., significantly too low or too high) ffiffiffiffiffiffi a Loadings of the cross aths for A, C and E all equaled :0 ffiffiffiffiffiffi b Loadings of the cross aths for A, C and E all equaled :1 c M 1 and M are uncorrelated between twins, i.e., fully E d M 1 and M are correlated 1 between twins, i.e., fully C Consider the regression of T 1 on both M 1 and M : T 1 ¼ b 0 þ b 1 M 1 þ b M ð5þ where b 0 denotes the intercet, and b 1 and b denote the regression weight of M 1 and M, resectively. Regression weight b 1 is a measure of the relationshi between T 1 and M 1 while controlling for M, and is in the comletely standardized case calculated as b 1 ¼ r t1;m1 r t1;m r m1;m 1 rm1;m : ðþ Similarly, regression weight b is a measure of the relationshi between T 1 and M while controlling for M 1, and is calculated as b ¼ r t1;m r t1;m1 r m1;m 1 rm1;m : ðþ Note that b = zero, if M 1 and M are uncorrelated, because then r m1,m = r t1,m = 0, in which case this extension equals the general univariate model. Note also the similarity between Eqs. and and Eq. 4: the calculation of the regression weights in multile regression with two redictors resembles the calculation of semi-artial correlations, excet for the square root in the denominator. From Eqs. and, it can be seen that b 1 and b will only be equal across zygosity grous if both r t,m1 (= r t1,m ) and r m1,m are equal across zygosity, i.e., if neither M and the relation between M and T are affected by genetic factors. In all other situations, b 1 and b, and as a result b 0, should be estimated searately in MZ and DZ twins, allowing their values to differ across zygosity. Allowing all three betas in the means model to differ across zygosity will result in a general extended univariate moderation model, the secification of which is indeendent of the nature of the correlations between M and T and M 1 and M. This extension imlies that b 0, b 1 and b need to be different across MZ and DZ grous, so that the means models for MZ and DZ twins 1 and become: MZ: T 1 ¼ b 0;mz þ b 1;mz M 1 þ b 1;mz M ; ð8þ T ¼ b 0;mz þ b 1;mz M þ b ;mz M 1 ; DZ: T 1 ¼ b 0;dz þ b 1;dz M 1 þ b ;dz M ; ð9þ T ¼ b 0;dz þ b 1;dz M þ b ;dz M 1 : With 1 arameters ( to describe the variance art of the model: related, and unrelated to the moderator; and arameters to describe the means models: for MZ twins, and for DZ twins), this extended univariate moderation model is still more arsimonious than the bivariate moderation model (1 arameters of which 15 concern the variance decomosition). We conducted Simulation study to investigate whether the false ositive rate of this extended univariate moderation model is correct, and comarable to the false ositive rate of the full bivariate moderation model. Simulation study To investigate whether the extended univariate moderation model results in the correct false ositive rate of 5%, we re- 1

18 Behav Genet (01) 4:10 18 Table Results Simulation study : false ositive rates under the extended univariate moderation model and the full bivariate moderation model when the covariance between T and M is not moderated Dro b a Dro b c Dro b e % hits nsim % hits nsim % hits nsim r M,T.4 via A Ext univariate.04 000.04 000.04 1998 Full bivariate.01 000.01 000.04 000 \.001 \.001 =.88 r M,T.4 via C Ext univariate.04 000.04 000.05 000 Full bivariate.01 1999.01 1998.04 000 \.001 \.001 =.08 r M,T.4 via E Ext univariate.05 1998.04 1999.05 1999 Full bivariate.01 199.01 199.05 000 \.001 \.001 =.5 r M,T =.4 via A, C and E in equal roortions Ext univariate.04 000.05 000.05 000 Full bivariate.0 000.01 000.05 000 \.005 \.001 =.4 r M,T =. via A, C and E in equal roortions Ext univariate.05 1998.05 000.05 000 Full bivariate.0 000.0 000.05 000 \.001 \.001 =.94 r M1,M = 0 (fully E), r M,T =.4 Ext univariate.05 1999.05 000.04 1999 r M1,M = 1 (fully C), r M,T =.4 Ext univariate.0 000.0 000.05 1999 Note: b a, b c, and b e denote the moderation arameters on the variance comonents unique to T (see Fig. 1). nsim denotes the number of data sets (out of 000) for which the extended univariate moderation model and the full bivariate moderation model converged without roblems. % hits denotes the ercentage (of the nsim converged models) that the likelihood-ratio test was smaller than the critical value.84 (i.e., significant given a =.05). % hits outside the.04.0 range should be considered incorrect (i.e., significantly too low or too high). The -values concern -values of the binomial test for comaring two roortions, used to test whether the number of hits under the extended univariate moderation model is significantly different from the number of hits under the full bivariate moderation model. Note that the full bivariate moderation model was not used to analyze data generated according to the final two settings (r M1,M = 0 and r M1,M = 1) because one would never use a bivariate model for data with such a variance covariance structure analyzed the data-sets created under Simulation study 1 using the extended univariate moderator model. Again we tested the significance of moderator arameters b a, b c and b e. As these arameters were simulated to be zero, we exected the v diff to be v(1) distributed across the 000 datasets, indeendent of whether T and M were correlated via A, via C, or via E. In addition, these same data sets were analyzed using the full bivariate moderation model (as deicted in Fig. 1a) in which all 1 arameters were estimated freely (Note that we did not use the full bivariate moderation model to analyze the data with r M1,M = 1or r M1,M = 0 because in ractice one would never choose a bivariate arameterization under these circumstances). The results for both the extended univariate moderation model and the full bivariate moderation model are described in Table, and deicted as PP-lots in Fig. 4 for the extended univariate moderation model. The false ositive rate of the extended univariate moderator model is in most cases not significantly different from 5%, and where it is (when variance in M is comletely due to C, and thus r M1,M = 1), it is too low, i.e. the test is too conservative. In contrast, the false ositive rate of the full bivariate model is always too low, and significantly lower than the false ositive rate of the extended univariate model, when testing b a or b c, irresective of the nature of the correlation between M and T. Aarently, slight misfit in the full bivariate moderation model resulting from droing one arameter can quite easily be accommodated by adjustment of the remaining arameters, resulting in a too low false ositive rate. In addition, it is imortant to 1

Behav Genet (01) 4:10 18 19 Fig. 4 PP-lots for the extended univariate moderation model in Simulation study, in which T and M are correlated exclusively via A (uer art), exclusively via C (middle art), or exclusively via E (lower art). Deviations from the 45 line show whether the use of the regular v (1) test would result in conservative (above the line) or liberal (below the line) decision. % hits denotes the ercentage of realize that the variance comonents unique to trait T under the bivariate model (A u,c u and E u in Fig. 1a) are not the same as the variance comonents of the residual T 00 in the extended univariate arameterization. In Aendix 1, we show how the residual T 00 is calculated when M and T are correlated via A, or C, or E. The fact that the variance decomosition of T 00 is not the same under the bivariate and the univariate moderation model imlies that either model likelihood-ratio tests smaller than the critical value.84 (i.e., significant given a =.05). A hit rate of.05 is exected given a =.05, and given that moderation effects were absent in the data. Hit rates outside the.04.0 range should be considered incorrect (i.e., significantly too low or too high) can constitute a more or less erroneous aroximation, deending on the real data generating rocess, which is generally unknown. Summarizing the results of Simulation study, we conclude that the extension of the univariate moderation model avoids the inflated false ositive scores that were observed for the standard univariate moderation model, while the full bivariate moderation model actually roved 1

180 Behav Genet (01) 4:10 18 Table 4 Results Simulation study : false ositive rates under the extended univariate moderation model and the full bivariate moderation model when the covariance between T and M is moderated Dro b a Dro b c Dro b e % hits nsim % hits nsim % hits nsim Baseline: b ac = b cc = b ec = 0 Ext univariate.05 1999.05 000.05 1999 Full bivariate.0 000.01 000.05 000 \.001 \.001 =.4 b ac =.10; b cc = b ec = 0 Ext univariate.18 1998.18 1999.08 199 Full bivariate.0 000.0 000.0 000 \.001 \.001 \.01 b cc =.10; b ac = b ec = 0 Ext univariate.1 000.1 1999.0 1999 Full bivariate.0 000.0 000.05 000 \.001 \.001 \.01 b ec =.10; b ac = b cc = 0 Ext univariate. 000. 1998.08 199 Full bivariate.0 000.01 000.04 000 \.001 \.001 \.001 Note: b a, b c, and b e denote the moderation arameters on the variance comonents unique to T; b ac, b cc, and b ec denote the moderation arameters on the cross aths between M and T (see Fig. 1). nsim denotes the number of data sets (out of 000) for which the extended univariate moderation model and the full bivariate moderation model converged without roblems. % hits denotes the ercentage (of the nsim converged models) that the likelihood-ratio test was smaller than the critical value.84 (i.e., significant given a =.05). % hits outside the.04.0 range should be considered incorrect (i.e., significantly too low or too high). The -values concern -values of the binomial test for comaring two roortions, used to test whether the number of hits under the extended univariate moderation model is significantly different from the number of hits under the full bivariate moderation model too conservative. However, Simulation studies 1 and concerned scenarios in which the covariance between M and T was itself not subject to moderation, i.e., b ac, b cc, and b ec on the cross aths between M and T in Fig. 1a were fixed to 0. That is, the covariance between M and T did not deendent on the level of M. In ractice, however, it is ossible that the covariance between M and T fluctuates as a function of M. Simulation study was conducted to investigate the false ositive rate of the extended univariate and the full bivariate moderation models in the context of data in which the covariance between M and T is moderated. These simulations are of secific interest since moderator-deendent variation in the strength of the covariance between M and T is not well accommodated by the estimated regression arameters b 0,MZ, b 1,MZ, b,mz, b 0,DZ, b 1,DZ, and b,dz in Eqs. 8 and 9, and roblems are therefore to be exected for the extended univariate moderation model. Simulation study We again simulated data for standard normally distributed moderator M and trait T in 500 MZ and 500 DZ twin airs. Suose again that A, C, and E account for 40%, 0%, and 0%, resectively, of the variance in M. The arts of the cross aths between M and T that do not deend on M (a c, c c, and e c in Fig. 1a) are all set to.05, and A, C, and E unique to T (a u, c u, and e u in Fig. 1a) are set to.5,.5 and.5, resectively. That is, if moderation is fully absent, the correlation between M and T equals.9, while genetic and (common) environmental effects exlain 40%, 0% and 0% of the variance in T, resectively. We now introduce moderation on the cross aths by setting either b ac, b cc,or b ec to.10. Moderation on the unique arts of T is, however, absent (i.e., b a, b c, and b e in Fig. 1a are set to 0). For each of these settings we simulated 000 data sets. Note that we deliberately choose the moderation arameters on the cross aths to be quite substantial: if the false ositive rate of the extended univariate model is affected by moderation of the cross aths, then we are sure to ick it u. If the false ositive rate of the extended univariate model is not affected by moderation of the cross aths, then the size of this moderation should not matter. We then fitted to these datasets a) the full bivariate moderation model including all 1 arameters, and b) the extended univariate moderation model in which both moderators M 1 and M are modeled on the means with means arameters differing across zygosity (Eqs. 8 and 9). 1

Behav Genet (01) 4:10 18 181 Within these models we constrained either b a, b c,orb e to zero to test for the significance of each arameter individually, i.e., a 1-df test. Since moderation arameters b a, b c, and b e were simulated as 0 in these data, we exect the distribution of the v diff as calculated across all 000 data sets to follow a central v (1) distribution. Given an nominal a of.05, we exected 5% of v diff test to be significant, i.e., larger than the critical value of.84. The results of these simulations are summarized in Table 4. When moderation on the cross aths is absent (Baseline: b ac = b cc = b ec = 0), the false ositive rate of the extended univariate model is correct, while the false ositive rate of the full bivariate model is deflated for b au and b cu. The false ositive rate of the full bivariate model, however, remains largely unchanged when moderation on the cross aths is introduced. In contrast, the false ositive rate of the extended univariate model becomes considerably inflated, esecially for b a and b c. Clearly, when the covariance between M and T is not stable across levels of M, the samle-wise regression coefficients b 0,MZ, b 1,MZ, b,mz, b 0,DZ, b 1,DZ, and b,dz in Eqs. 8 and 9 do not sufficiently accommodate the varying association between M and T. As a result, the remaining moderation, which is actually located on the cross aths, is now icked u in the extended univariate moderation model as if it were located on the unique aths of T. Additional simulations (not shown), in which either b ac, b cc or b ec equaled.10 while the other two cross ath moderation arameters equaled.05, showed even higher hit rates for the extended univariate model (u to 5%), while the hit rate of the full bivariate moderation model remained.05 or lower. In summary, the results of Simulation study show that the extended univariate moderation model can be used as a moderation detection method, but is not very suited to establish the exact location of the moderation as it cannot distinguish moderation on cross aths from moderation on the unique aths of T. False negatives We have shown that the false ositive rate (i.e., tye I error rate) is correct under the extended univariate moderation model, but only if the covariance between M and T is free of moderation by M. We now address the false negative rate, i.e., the tye II error, of the extended univariate moderation model comared to the full bivariate model. In a fourth and fifth simulation study, we investigate whether the false negative rate of the extended univariate moderation model is comarable to the false negative rate of the bivariate moderation model when the covariance between M and T is not subject to moderation (Simulation study 4) or when this covariance is subject to moderation as well (Simulation study 5). Simulation study 4 Data were simulated as described in Simulation study 1, with moderation on the cross aths being absent. We now, however, introduced moderation on the aths unique to T, with moderation arameters being either b a =.08, or b c =.10, or b e =.05 (As shown in Table 5, the ower to detect moderation on E variance is much greater than the ower to detect moderation of A or C variance, which is why b e was chosen much smaller than both b a and b c ). Note that the effect size of the chosen moderation arameters deends on the nature of the correlation between T and M. For examle, b a was set at.08. If the correlation ran via C or E, then the genetic variance of trait T was calculated as ( :4?.08 * M). If the correlation between T ffiffiffiffi and M ran via A, however, then the genetic variance of T ffiffiffiffiffiffi was calculated as.15? ( :5?.08 * M), where.15 is associated with the cross-ath relating T and M. For each of these 9 settings (r t,m runs via A, C or E, and moderation is resent on either A, C, or E) we simulated 000 datasets and analyzed these using either the full bivariate moderation model (estimating moderation arameters on the cross aths as well as on the aths unique to T) or the extended univariate moderation model (estimating moderation on the variance comonents of the residual T 00 ). We then tested whether the moderation arameter of interest (either b a,orb c,orb e ) was significant given a =.05. The results of these simulations are resented in Table 5. In 5 out of 9 scenarios, the ower of the extended univariate moderation model was significantly higher than the ower of the full bivariate moderation model. Note that we can indeed seak of higher ower because we know from the results of Simulation study that the false ositive rate of neither models is inflated. The lower ower of the full bivariate moderation model is robably due to the variance being decomosed into as many as 15 arameters, comared to the of the extended univariate moderation model: misfit resulting from fixing one of the moderation arameters to zero can more easily be absorbed by the remaining 14 arameters. Simulation study 5 Data were simulated as described in Simulation study with covariance between M and T running via A, C and E, and moderation on the cross aths was introduced by setting either b ac, b cc,orb ec to.10 (see Table for descrition of the scenarios). Moderation on the unique arts of T was 1

18 Behav Genet (01) 4:10 18 Table 5 Results Simulation study 4: false negative rates under the extended univariate moderation model and the full bivariate moderation model when the covariance between T and M is not moderated Dro b a Dro b c Dro b e % hits nsim % hits nsim % hits nsim T and M correlated via A Ext univariate.0 1999.1 1998.48 000 Full bivariate.0 1998.1 000.50 000 \.001 \.001 =.4 T and M correlated via C Ext univariate. 1999.1 1999.4 000 Full bivariate.5 000.0 1998.48 000 =.18 \.001 =.95 T and M correlated via E Ext univariate.48 1999.4 1999.91 199 Full bivariate.5 1998.8 1989.91 000 \.001 \.001 =.8 Note: b a, b c and b e denote the moderation arameters on the variance comonents unique to T (see Fig. 1). nsim denotes the number of data sets (out of 000) for which the extended univariate moderation model and the full bivariate moderation model converged without roblems. % hits denotes the ercentage (of the nsim converged models) that the likelihood-ratio test was smaller than the critical value.84 (i.e., significant given a =.05). % hits outside the.04.0 range should be considered incorrect (i.e., significantly too low or too high). The -values concern -values of the binomial test for comaring two roortions, used to test whether the number of hits under the extended univariate moderation model is significantly different from the number of hits under the full bivariate moderation model now simulated to be resent as well, with moderation arameters being either b a =.08, or b c =.10, or b e =.05 (as in Simulation study 4). For each scenario, we simulated 000 datasets and analyzed these using both the full bivariate moderation model (estimating moderation arameters on the cross aths as well as on the aths unique to T) and the extended univariate moderation model (estimating moderation on the variance comonents of the residual T 00 ). We then tested whether the moderation arameter of interest (either b a,orb c,orb e ) was significant given a =.05. The results of these simulations are resented in Table. In all scenarios, the extended univariate moderation model icks u moderation more often than the full bivariate moderation model. However, these results can, excet for the Baseline model, not be interreted as the extended univariate moderation model having more ower than the full bivariate moderation model. Given the results of Simulation study, which showed that the extended moderation model icks u the moderation on the cross aths as if it is moderation on the unique aths, we conclude that the ower of the extended univariate moderation model is too high, or at least that the location of the moderation that is detected, is uncertain. That is, b a, b c, and b e are biased in the extended univariate moderation model because the moderation on the cross aths (b ac, b cc, and b ec ) is not adequately accommodated by the regression coefficients in the means art of the model. Discussion In this aer, we showed that the univariate moderation model roosed by Purcell (00) roduces (highly) inflated false ositive rates if the moderator M is correlated between twins, and M and T are correlated as well. We investigated an extension of this model as a solution to this roblem, and conclude that the extended univariate moderation model works well, but only if moderation on the covariance between M and T is absent. Moderation of the covariance between M and T is, however, not accommodated adequately in the extended univariate moderation model, and as a result, moderation of the covariance is icked u as moderation on the variance comonents unique to T. In the absence of moderation of the covariance between M and T, the extended univariate moderation model is actually more owerful than the full bivariate moderation model, but in the resence of moderation of the covariance between M and T, the extended univariate moderation model detects moderation of the variance comonents unique to T, as such missecifying the actual location of the moderation. Fortunately, most ublished aers in which the univariate moderation model was used concern moderation effects of family-level moderators such as SES, arental educational attainment level, or the age of the twins, i.e., variables that are by definition equal in both twins. As we have shown, non-zero semi-artial correlations are not a 1

Behav Genet (01) 4:10 18 18 Table Results Simulation study 5: false negative rates under the extended univariate moderation model and the full bivariate moderation model when the covariance between T and M is moderated Dro b a Dro b c Dro b e % hits nsim % hits nsim % hits nsim Baseline: b ac = b cc = b ec = 0 Ext univariate.9 000.4 199.5 1999 Full bivariate.5 000. 1999.5 000 \.01 \.001 =. b ac =.10; b cc = b ec = 0 Ext univariate.5 1989.1 199.0 1998 Full bivariate. 000.0 000.05 000 \.01 \.001 =.01 b cc =.10; b ac = b ec = 0 Ext univariate.14 199.45 1995.0 1998 Full bivariate.04 000.5 000.05 000 \.01 \.001 =.0 b ec =.10; b ac = b cc = 0 Ext univariate. 199. 1998.50 1995 Full bivariate.0 000.01 000.5 000 \.01 \.001 \.001 Note: b a, b c, and b e denote the moderation arameters on the variance comonents unique to T; b ac, b cc, and b ec denote the moderation arameters on the cross aths between M and T (see Fig. 1). nsim denotes the number of data sets (out of 000) for which the extended univariate moderation model and the full bivariate moderation model converged without roblems. % hits denotes the ercentage (of the nsim converged models) that the likelihood-ratio test was smaller than the critical value.84 (i.e., significant given a =.05). % hits outside the.04.0 range should be considered incorrect (i.e., significantly too low or too high). The -values concern -values of the binomial test for comaring two roortions, used to test whether the number of hits under the extended univariate moderation model is significantly different from the number of hits under the full bivariate moderation model roblem in that case and the false ositive rate is rather too low than too high (i.e., the model is slightly conservative). In a few ublished aers, however, moderators were studied that did show variation between twins (e.g., McCaffery et al. 008, 009; Timberlake et al. 00: moderators under study were educational attainment level of the twins, exercise level of the twins, and the twins selfreorted religiosity, resectively). Whether the moderation effects reorted in these aers are genuine or surious (i.e., the result of non-zero semi-artial cross-trait crosstwin correlations) deends, as we have shown, on the nature of the correlation between T and M, on the nature of the correlation between M 1 and M, and on the absence or resence of moderation of the covariance between M and T. Re-analysis of these data using the full bivariate moderation model, or the extended univariate moderation model if the resence of moderation of the covariance has been excluded, is advised. Overall, we conclude that researchers should use the full bivariate moderation model to study the resence of moderation on the covariance between M and T. If such moderation can be ruled out, subsequent use of the extended univariate moderation model is recommended as this model is more owerful than the full bivariate moderation model. Acknowledgement Sohie van der Sluis (VENI-451-08-05 and VIDI-01-05-18) and Danielle Posthuma (VIDI-01-05-18) are financially suorted by the Netherlands Scientific Organization (Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschaelijk Onderzoek, gebied Maatschaij-en Gedragswetenschaen: NWO/MaGW). Simulations were carried out on the Genetic Cluster Comuter which is financially suorted by an NWO Medium Investment grant (480-05- 00), by the VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, and by the Dutch Brain Foundation. Oen Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which ermits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reroduction in any medium, rovided the original author(s) and source are credited. Aendix 1 In this aendix we derive the residual covariance matrix of T 00 in MZ and DZ twins after artialling out the effects of M 1 and M. 1