Representing subset relations with tree diagrams or unit squares?

Similar documents
Visualizing Bayesian situations about the use of the unit square Andreas Eichler 1, Markus Vogel 2 and Katharina Böcherer-Linder 3

In press, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. Frequency Illusions and Other Fallacies. Steven A. Sloman.

Numeracy, frequency, and Bayesian reasoning

A model of parallel time estimation

Can Bayesian models have normative pull on human reasoners?

Consider the alternative: The effects of causal knowledge on representing and using. alternative hypotheses in judgments under uncertainty

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints

The psychology of Bayesian reasoning

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Generalization and Theory-Building in Software Engineering Research

SEMINAR ON SERVICE MARKETING

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Data and Statistics 101: Key Concepts in the Collection, Analysis, and Application of Child Welfare Data

Simple heuristics in a social world. Ralph Hertwig

The Psychology of Inductive Inference

Descending Marr s levels: Standard observers are no panacea. to appear in Behavioral & Brain Sciences

Does the knowledge of Synthetic Biology effect its acceptance?

Definitions of Nature of Science and Scientific Inquiry that Guide Project ICAN: A Cheat Sheet

Decisions based on verbal probabilities: Decision bias or decision by belief sampling?

D F 3 7. beer coke Cognition and Perception. The Wason Selection Task. If P, then Q. If P, then Q

Lecture 6. Perceptual and Motor Schemas

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

Support system for breast cancer treatment

Name Class Date. 1. How does science differ from other kinds of human endeavors such as art, architecture, and philosophy?

Publisher: Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Prentice Hall

Spatial Visualization Measurement: A Modification of the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test - Visualization of Rotations

Chapter 02 Developing and Evaluating Theories of Behavior

Advances in Environmental Biology

Computers in Human Behavior

Students Biases in Conditional Probability Reasoning

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

PORTUGUESE AND BRAZILIAN CHILDREN UNDERSTANDING THE INVERSE RELATION BETWEEN QUANTITIES THE CASE OF FRACTIONS

Bayesian models of inductive generalization

Causal explanation improves judgment under uncertainty, but rarely in a Bayesian way. 1 School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Australia

CHAPTER 3 DATA ANALYSIS: DESCRIBING DATA

Reference: Vol. 106, no. 24

Effect of Positive and Negative Instances on Rule Discovery: Investigation Using Eye Tracking

Is a picture worth a thousand words? The interaction of visual display and attribute representation in attenuating framing bias

Study on perceptually-based fitting line-segments

Semantic Coherence and Fallacies in Estimating Joint Probabilities

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

WOMEN CREATE SPACE A SKILLS DEVELOPMENT

A Matrix of Material Representation

Sparse Coding in Sparse Winner Networks

The Scientific Method

Running head: INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 1. Why to treat subjects as fixed effects. James S. Adelman. University of Warwick.

The Effect of Perspective on Misconceptions in Psychology: A Test of Conceptual Change Theory 1. Eric Amsel. Department of Psychology

The interplay of domain-specific and domain general processes, skills and abilities in the development of science knowledge

TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing?

Fundamental Concepts for Using Diagnostic Classification Models. Section #2 NCME 2016 Training Session. NCME 2016 Training Session: Section 2

Heuristics & Biases:

Book Information Jakob Hohwy, The Predictive Mind, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2013, ix+288, 60.00,

Gathering and Repetition of the Elements in an Image Affect the Perception of Order and Disorder

January 2, Overview

The Scientific Method

SDT Clarifications 1. 1Colorado State University 2 University of Toronto 3 EurekaFacts LLC 4 University of California San Diego

THE IMPORTANCE OF MENTAL OPERATIONS IN FORMING NOTIONS

Chapter 7. Mental Representation

Principles of Sociology

Running head: How large denominators are leading to large errors 1

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

ADAPTING COPYCAT TO CONTEXT-DEPENDENT VISUAL OBJECT RECOGNITION

Effects of Cognitive Load on Processing and Performance. Amy B. Adcock. The University of Memphis

Between Fact and Fiction: Artifacts and Ethics in Social Research. Toh Wah Seng ABSTRACT

NATIONAL UNIVERSITY OF PHYSICAL EDUCATION AND SPORTS - BUCHAREST DOCTORAL SCHOOL ABSTRACT OF DOCTORAL THESIS

Study on perceptually-based fitting elliptic arcs

Systems Theory: Should Information Researchers Even Care?

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

Standard Logic and Probability Theory as Criteria for True Logical Propositions. Bayesian Logic

Correlation vs. Causation - and What Are the Implications for Our Project? By Michael Reames and Gabriel Kemeny

INFORMATION SERVICES AND USER STUDIES

Learning Deterministic Causal Networks from Observational Data

6. A theory that has been substantially verified is sometimes called a a. law. b. model.

Critical Thinking: Science, Models, & Systems

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2)

Public Attitudes toward Nuclear Power

NATURE OF SCIENCE. Professor Andrea Garrison Biology 3A

Response to the ASA s statement on p-values: context, process, and purpose

Reactivity and Deliberation in Decision-Making Systems

Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions

Identity Theory: Reconstruction and Application

Long Term: Systematically study children s understanding of mathematical equivalence and the ways in which it develops.

Competition Elicits Arousal and Affect

Chapter 02 Lecture Outline

Recognizing Scenes by Simulating Implied Social Interaction Networks

Using Perceptual Grouping for Object Group Selection

DEVELOPING THE RESEARCH FRAMEWORK Dr. Noly M. Mascariñas

The Scientific Enterprise. The Scientific Enterprise,

Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 13 (emphasis on Chapter 12) Online readings: Appendix D, E & F

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Competing Frameworks in Perception

THE SCIENTIFIC WORLD VIEW

An Ideal Observer Model of Visual Short-Term Memory Predicts Human Capacity Precision Tradeoffs

ST440/550: Applied Bayesian Statistics. (10) Frequentist Properties of Bayesian Methods

UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society

Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination

Transcription:

Representing subset relations with tree diagrams or unit squares? Katharina Böcherer-Linder 1 and Andreas Eichler 2 1 University of Education Freiburg, Germany; katharina.boechererlinder@ph-freiburg.de 2 University of Kassel, Germany; eichler@mathematik.uni-kassel.de In this paper, we refer to the efficiency of different visualizations for mathematical problem solving. Particularly, we investigate how set relations - that are potentially important in probability and also fractions - are made transparent by two different visualizations, i.e. the tree diagram and the unit square. In this paper, we use these two visualizations as representations of statistical information. First, we analyze theoretically the quality of visualizing set relations by tree diagrams and unit squares. Second, we report on a study with students of mathematics education (N = 148) where the unit square outperformed the tree diagram when the perception of subset relations was regarded. Finally, we discuss the significance of our results, specifically for the teaching and learning of conditional probabilities. Keywords: Representation of statistical information, set relations, tree diagram, unit square. Introduction In mathematics education, it is widely accepted that representations and visualizations could have a considerable impact on students learning. For example, Duval claims that visualizations of mathematical concepts are at the core of understanding in mathematics (Duval, 2002, p. 312). However, research in mathematics education and cognitive psychology gave evidence that visualization does not necessarily foster students understanding. For this reason, a crucial question in research in mathematics education is to identify the one of potentially different visualizations that is most efficient referring to students learning. We refer to this question concerning competing visualizations aiming to support students learning in statistics and probability, but also in fractions. The transparency of set relations plays a crucial role in probability (Böcherer-Linder & Eichler, accepted), but is also important in many other domains of mathematics education, such as the teaching and learning of fractions. Theoretical framework In the context of Bayesian reasoning, research in cognitive psychology has shown that the transparency of set relations in the visualization of statistical information impacts on the performance in tasks concerning the Bayes rule: any manipulation that increases the transparency of the nested-sets relation should increase correct responding (Sloman, Over, Slovak, & Stibel, 2003, p. 302).

Figure 1: Nested sets of the Bayesian situation of a medical diagnosis visualized by an Euler diagram (cf. Binder, Krauss & Bruckmaier, 2015). Proponents of this point of view, called nested-sets account, attribute the difficulties of Bayesian reasoning to the fact that some sets of events are nested (Lesage, Navarrete, & Neys, 2013; Sloman et al., 2003). For the example of a medical diagnosis, Figure 1 illustrates the nested-sets situation. Transparency of set relations means that it is easy to see, how many elements are in the sets and how the sets relate. The cognitive model into which the nested-sets account has been incorporated is the dual process theory (Barbey & Sloman, 2007): Representing the statistical information in a standard probability format (without visualization) obscures the nested-sets structure of the problem and, therefore, triggers the associative system which may lead to biases. Representing the statistical information with natural frequencies and / or appropriate visualizations in contrast trigger the rulebased system because nested sets relations are made transparent, enabling people to reason consciously and according to the logic of set inclusion (Barbey & Sloman, 2007). For the design of effective visualizations, proponents of the nested-sets account claim that visualizations are helpful to the extent that they make the nested set structure of the problem transparent (Barbey & Sloman, 2007; Sloman et al., 2003). There are different competing visualizations that claim to visualize efficiently set relations or situations that necessitates applying Bayes rule and it is an open question which visualizations are the most efficient and which properties explain these visualizations efficiencies. In this paper, we investigate how set relations are made transparent by two competing different visualizations, i.e. the tree diagram and the unit square. We use these two visualizations as representations of statistical information (Venn diagrams are not considered in this paper because we focus on the visualization of statistical information and Venn diagrams are pure set representations but not representations of statistical information). First, we analyze theoretically the quality of visualizing set relations by tree diagrams and unit squares. Second, we report on a study (N = 148) where we investigate whether the tree diagram or the unit square is more efficient to support the perception of subset relations. Finally, we discuss the significance of our results, specifically for the teaching and learning of conditional probabilities.

Visualizing set relations A flower girl is selling red and white roses and carnations. We use this situation as an example to illustrate how the tree diagram and the unit square visualize set relations. In this situation, we have sets (for example the set of all roses) and subsets (for example the subset of all red roses) and subset relations (for example the red flowers among the roses). If we attribute some numerical values to the number of roses and carnations, the situation can be visualized by showing absolute numbers in the tree diagram and the unit square: Figure 2: Representing statistical information with the tree diagram and the unit square Both, the tree diagram and the unit square can be seen as nested-sets representations. In the tree diagram, the logical relations between sets and subsets are visualized by lines. The subsets are on a lower level than the sets in the tree and the branches connect the subsets with the sets. For example the subset red roses is on a lower level than the set roses and the branch connecting red roses and roses visualizes the relation between both sets. The tree implies a hierarchical structure which means that subsets are always on a lower level than sets. Therefore, only those subset relations that are in line with the hierarchy are salient. For example, the subset relation of roses among the red flowers where the roses are the subset and all the red flowers are the including set is not visualized by a branch in the tree and thus, is not transparent. In the unit square, subset relations are visualized by areas being embedded in other areas. For example the subset of white roses is represented by a partial area of the rectangle that represents all roses. In contrast to the tree diagram, the unit square implies no hierarchy. That means that subset relations can be grasped vertically (e.g. white roses among the roses ) as well as horizontally (e.g. roses among the red flowers ). Therefore, all subset relations that are possible in this situation are transparent in the unit square. Because of these differences in the properties of the two visualizations, we expected a difference between the tree diagram and the unit square when the perception of different subset relations is regarded. Therefore we hypothesized:

If the subset-relation is not in line with the hierarchy of the tree diagram, the unit square is more efficient to make the subset-relation transparent (hypothesis 1). If the subset-relation is in line with the hierarchy of the tree diagram, there is no significant difference between the unit square and the tree diagram (hypothesis 2). Method We administered a questionnaire to 148 students that were enrolled in a course of mathematics education. We asked the students (among other questions concerning conditional probabilities) in one task that we indicate below to calculate proportions and to indicate the result in form of fractions: #subset #set In this way, we could analyze if the right subsets and right sets have been grasped from the visualization. The questionnaire had two versions, one showing tree diagrams, the other showing unit squares to represent the statistical information. The rest of the test-items remained constant and the participants were randomly assigned to one of the two groups. Thus, any potential difference in the results could directly be attributed to the influence of the visualizations. To assess the influence of representation on the perception of subset relations we designed testitems that each addressed structurally different subset relations. In Figure 3, we show the questions that were accompanied by either the tree diagram or the unit square shown above. Note that the item (d) addresses a subset relation that is not in line with the hierarchy of the tree diagram and therefore a higher performance for the unit square was expected. The items a, b, c and e address subset relations that are in line with the hierarchy of the tree diagram and therefore no significant difference between the tree diagram and the unit square was expected. We rated correct answers with 1 and incorrect answers with 0.

Flowers A flower girl is selling red and white roses and carnations. Altogether, she has 120 flowers. Calculate the following proportions. Indicate the results in form of fractions. The proportion of a) red carnations among all carnations. b) white roses among all flowers. c) white flowers among all flowers. d) carnations among the red flowers. e) roses among all flowers. Figure 3: Items to assess the perception of subset relations Results Figure 4 shows the results for each of the five test-items. As it has been hypothesized, the unit square (M = 0.66, SD = 0.476) was more efficient than the tree diagram (M = 0.38, SD = 0.488) for the item (d) that addressed a subset relation that is not in line with the hierarchy of the tree diagram. The difference for the item (d) was significant (t (146) = 3.579, p <.001) with an effect size of d =.58. Thus, the hypothesis 1 was confirmed. Figure 4: Participants performance (error bars indicate SE) To investigate a potential difference for the other items addressing subset relations within the hierarchy of the tree diagram, we yielded a t-test for the accumulated score referring to these four items (α =.739). No significant difference was found between the tree diagram (M = 3.46, SD = 1.023) and the unit square (M = 3.46, SD = 1.036), t (146) = 0.000, p = 1.000. In addition, we tested each of the four items a, b, c and e individually. None of the items yielded a significant difference

between the unit square and the tree diagram. Thus, there was no reason to reject our hypothesis 2. (Note, even if we tested the results by referring to each of the five items as representing different subset relations, using the Bonferroni adjustment, the difference between the effectiveness of the tree diagram and the unit square concerning item (d) is still significant 5.01.) Interestingly, the mean values of correct answers for the tree diagram were almost equal for all of the four items a, b, c and e addressing subset relations that were in line with the hierarchy of the tree diagram (88%, 85%, 86%, 86%). In contrast, the performance for item (d) was lower for both visualizations. This might indicate that the subset relation (d) is more difficult to perceive than the other subset relations and that the visualization with the unit square is more helpful in this case. At Cerme10, we will also present a replication of this finding, which we do not present here in the current paper since the work is still in progress. Discussion of the results For the situation of the flower girl, our results show a very clear effect in favor of the unit square. Nevertheless, we suggest for future research to prove this effect also for other contexts. In another study with 143 students of electrical engineering, we replicated the effect for subset relations that are not in line with the hierarchy of the tree diagram for two more different contexts but it is desirable to also replicate the effect of those subset relations used in the items a, b, c and e. Another aspect that could be investigated in more detail is the question of the order in the sequence in the tree diagram. It might be interesting to study the effect of the transposed order (roses / carnation on a lower level than red / white) and to compare it with a rotated unit square (roses / carnation arranged vertically and red / white arranged horizontally). This setting could be clarify if the hierarchy of the tree actually is the reason for the results in our study. Moreover for the context of Bayesian reasoning, the results of Binder et al. (2015, p.6) suggest an advantage of the 2 2-table compared to the tree diagram, although no statistical difference between 2 2-tables and tree diagrams was reported. Thus, it is an open question if 2x2-tables are equally efficient than unit squares to make subset relations transparent or if there is an additional effect of the unit square due to the redundant geometrical and numerical representation. Finally, there are further possibilities for visualizing set relations. One of these possibilities that was used in mathematics teaching is the double tree (Wassner, 2004). Thus, it could be interesting if a specific version of the tree diagram is able to decrease the weakness of the tree diagram to identify appropriately set relations. Implications The main result of our research seems nearly trivial: Visualizations have to visualize the main aspects of a mathematical concept if they aim to support students understanding of this concept. Accordingly, a subset relation must be transparent when the aim of the visualization is to represent subset relations. However, it is by no means at all trivial to identify the crucial aspects of a

mathematical concept. Actually, the tree diagram is very prominent in statistics education research (Veaux, Velleman, & Bock, 2012) and also psychological research (Binder, Krauss, & Bruckmaier, 2015) for visualizing Bayesian situation that necessitates applying Bayes rule. However, our research gave evidence that compared to the unit square - the tree diagram is not efficient to visualize the set relation that is crucial to understand the structure of a Bayesian situation since it requires a subset relation that is not in line with the hierarchy of the tree diagram. Our results have firstly some consequences if statistics education is regarded. Sloman et al. (2003) expressed that bringing out nested set structure has been identified as being important for the improvement in Bayesian reasoning tasks. Thus, restricted to teaching and learning probability, our results imply to reconsider the role of the tree diagram to support students learning referring to probability and Bayesian reasoning. This would be a considerable shift in statistics education (c.f. e.g. (Gigerenzer, 2014; Wassner & Martignon, 2002). A little bit more globally, it could be considered if proportions, and in particular proportions of proportions could be appropriately visualized by a unit square to emphasize the connection between proportions of proportions and conditional probabilities. Thus, the unit square could potentially be understood as a visual connection between fractions and probabilities. More generally, our results imply to focus the discussion of visualizations on the structure of visualization and on its relation to the structure of the represented mathematical concept. While the superiority of visualizations is a consensus in mathematics education as we outlined in the introduction, it is a crucial objective to find out which visualization best fits to a mathematical concept, especially in situations where several competing visualizations exist as it is the case for Bayesian reasoning situations. For example, Binder et al. (2015) show that the tree diagram supports Bayesian reasoning compared to pure symbolic representation, whereas our results imply that the required subset relation is not transparent in the tree diagram. Indeed, in recent research, the unit square outperformed the tree diagram in Bayesian reasoning tasks (Böcherer-Linder & Eichler, accepted; Böcherer-Linder, Eichler & Vogel, accepted). Therefore an ongoing task of educational research should be to precisely identify the relation of a visualization and its structure and the mathematical concept and its structure. One main message of our paper is that this relation is not sufficiently investigated, but could considerably impact on students learning. References Barbey, A. K., & Sloman, S. A. (2007). Base-rate respect: From ecological rationality to dual processes. The Behavioral and brain sciences, 30(3), 241-54; discussion 255-97. doi:10.1017/s0140525x07001653 Binder, K., Krauss, S., & Bruckmaier, G. (2015). Effects of visualizing statistical information - an empirical study on tree diagrams and 2 2 tables. Frontiers in psychology, 6, 1186. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01186

Böcherer-Linder, K., & Eichler, A. (accepted). The impact of visualizing nested sets. An empirical study on tree diagrams and unit squares. Accepted by Frontiers in psychology, Cognition. Böcherer-Linder, K., Eichler, A., & Vogel, M. (accepted). The impact of visualization on flexible Bayesian reasoning. Accepted by AIEM Avances de Investigación en Educación Matemática. Duval, R. (2002). Representation, vision and visualization: Cognitive functions in mathematical thinking. Basic issues for learning. In F. Hitt (Ed.), Representations and mathematics visualization. Papers presented in this Working Group of PME-NA, 1998-2002 (pp. 311 336). Mexico: Cinestav - IPN. Gigerenzer, G. (2014). How I Got Started: Teaching Physicians and Judges Risk Literacy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(4), 612 614. doi:10.1002/acp.2980 Lesage, E., Navarrete, G., & Neys, W. de. (2013). Evolutionary modules and Bayesian facilitation: The role of general cognitive resources. Thinking & Reasoning, 19(1), 27 53. doi:10.1080/13546783.2012.713177 Sloman, S. A., Over, D., Slovak, L., & Stibel, J. M. (2003). Frequency illusions and other fallacies. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91(2), 296 309. doi:10.1016/s0749-5978(03)00021-9 Veaux, R. D. de, Velleman, P. F., & Bock, D. E. (2012). Intro stats (3. ed., international ed., technology update). Boston Mass. u.a.: Pearson/Addison-Wesley. Wassner, C. (2004). Förderung Bayesianischen Denkens. Kognitionspsychologische Grundlagen und didaktische Analysen [Promoting Bayesian Reasoning. Principles of Cognitive Psychology and Didactical Analyses]. Hildesheim: Franzbecker. Wassner, C., & Martignon, L. (2002). Teaching decision making and statistical thinking with natural frequencies. In B. Phillips (Ed.), Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Teaching Statistics. Cape Town (South Africa): International Association for Statistics Education. Retrieved from http://iase-web.org/documents/papers/icots6/10_52_ma.pdf