The Self Group Distinction Scale: A new approach to measure individualism and collectivism in adolescents

Similar documents
SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Preschool Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (1.5 5 yrs.) among Canadian children

Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model

Marie Stievenart a, Marta Casonato b, Ana Muntean c & Rens van de Schoot d e a Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Universite

Measures of children s subjective well-being: Analysis of the potential for cross-cultural comparisons

Modeling the Influential Factors of 8 th Grades Student s Mathematics Achievement in Malaysia by Using Structural Equation Modeling (SEM)

National Culture Dimensions and Consumer Digital Piracy: A European Perspective

Title: The Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) and Texting While Driving Behavior in College Students MS # Manuscript ID GCPI

Development and Psychometric Properties of the Relational Mobility Scale for the Indonesian Population

The Psychometric Properties of Dispositional Flow Scale-2 in Internet Gaming


Impact and adjustment of selection bias. in the assessment of measurement equivalence

SHORT NOTES I or we : Family socialization values in a national probability sample in Taiwan

ASSESSING THE UNIDIMENSIONALITY, RELIABILITY, VALIDITY AND FITNESS OF INFLUENTIAL FACTORS OF 8 TH GRADES STUDENT S MATHEMATICS ACHIEVEMENT IN MALAYSIA

Assessing Measurement Invariance in the Attitude to Marriage Scale across East Asian Societies. Xiaowen Zhu. Xi an Jiaotong University.

On the Performance of Maximum Likelihood Versus Means and Variance Adjusted Weighted Least Squares Estimation in CFA

Materialism and Cultural Orientation: The Role of Vertical/Horizontal Indi vidualism within and across Cultures

Comprehensive Statistical Analysis of a Mathematics Placement Test

A methodological perspective on the analysis of clinical and personality questionnaires Smits, Iris Anna Marije

Abstract. The current research examined differences in career decision-making profiles (CDMP)

Analysis of the Reliability and Validity of an Edgenuity Algebra I Quiz

Problem with Cross-Cultural Comparison of User-Generated Ratings on Mechanical Turk

Alternative Methods for Assessing the Fit of Structural Equation Models in Developmental Research

Chapter 9. Youth Counseling Impact Scale (YCIS)

A Hierarchical Comparison on Influence Paths from Cognitive & Emotional Trust to Proactive Behavior Between China and Japan

International Conference on Humanities and Social Science (HSS 2016)

Personality and beliefs about the world revisited: Expanding the nomological network of social axioms

Assessing the Validity and Reliability of a Measurement Model in Structural Equation Modeling (SEM)

The Influence of Psychological Empowerment on Innovative Work Behavior among Academia in Malaysian Research Universities

College Student Self-Assessment Survey (CSSAS)

Examining the efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) to understand pre-service teachers intention to use technology*

Culture and Social Behavior, Self and Identity. Chapter 13. Defining the Concept of Self CULTURE AND THE CONCEPT OF SELF

Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment

Paul Irwing, Manchester Business School

Basic concepts and principles of classical test theory

Measurement models for orderedcategorical indicators: A factor analytic approach for testing the level of measurement

Jumpstart Mplus 5. Data that are skewed, incomplete or categorical. Arielle Bonneville-Roussy Dr Gabriela Roman

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Expressions of the Self in Individualistic vs. Collective Cultures: a cross-cultural-perspective teaching module

Investigating the Invariance of Person Parameter Estimates Based on Classical Test and Item Response Theories

Personal Style Inventory Item Revision: Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Original Papers. Emotional Frankness and Friendship in Polish Culture

Gezinskenmerken: De constructie van de Vragenlijst Gezinskenmerken (VGK) Klijn, W.J.L.

Measurement Invariance (MI): a general overview

Measurement invariance of the strength of motivation for medical school: a multigroup confirmatory factor analysis

Kuusisto, E., Gholami, K., Schutte, I.W., Wolfensberger, M.V.C., & Tirri, K. (2014).

The Youth Experience Survey 2.0: Instrument Revisions and Validity Testing* David M. Hansen 1 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign

Instrument equivalence across ethnic groups. Antonio Olmos (MHCD) Susan R. Hutchinson (UNC)

TESTING PSYCHOMETRIC PROPERTIES IN DYADIC DATA USING CONFIRMATORY FACTOR ANALYSIS: CURRENT PRACTICES AND RECOMMENDATIONS

Sense-making Approach in Determining Health Situation, Information Seeking and Usage

Reliability. Internal Reliability

Culture & Survey Measurement. Timothy Johnson Survey Research Laboratory University of Illinois at Chicago

Research on Software Continuous Usage Based on Expectation-confirmation Theory

A Modification to the Behavioural Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire to Include an Assessment of Amotivation

Measurement invariance of self-continuity strategies: Comparisons of early adolescents from Brazil, Canada and Colombia

THE STRUCTURE AND MEASUREMENT OF SELF-CONSTRUALS: A CROSS- CULTURAL STUDY OF THE SELF-CONSTRUAL SCALE LILIA GABRIELA MIRAMONTES

Author Note. LabDCI, Institute of Psychology, University of Lausanne, Bâtiment Antropole, CH-1015

Methodological Issues in Measuring the Development of Character

The Family Allocentrism Idiocentrism Scale: Further Convergent Validity Exploration

FACTOR VALIDITY OF THE MERIDEN SCHOOL CLIMATE SURVEY- STUDENT VERSION (MSCS-SV)

Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling. Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto

Boys, be independent! Conformity develop- ment of Japanese children in the Asch ex- periment without using confederates

Isabel Castillo, Inés Tomás, and Isabel Balaguer University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain

Self-Construal Orientation: Validation of an Instrument and a Study of the Relationship to Leadership Communication Style

The Development of Scales to Measure QISA s Three Guiding Principles of Student Aspirations Using the My Voice TM Survey

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the BCSSE Scales

Scale Building with Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Construct Invariance of the Survey of Knowledge of Internet Risk and Internet Behavior Knowledge Scale

Structural Equation Modeling of Multiple- Indicator Multimethod-Multioccasion Data: A Primer

Psychometric Evaluation of the Major Depression Inventory at the Kenyan Coast

Assessing Measurement Invariance of the Teachers Perceptions of Grading Practices Scale across Cultures

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

Factorial Validity and Consistency of the MBI-GS Across Occupational Groups in Norway

Aging, Gender, and Self: Dimensionality and Measurement Invariance Analysis on Self-Construal

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SOCIOMETRIC STATUS AND ANXIETY1. Nara Gakugei University

Validation of Intercultural Sensitivity Measure Individualism Collectivism

Development of self efficacy and attitude toward analytic geometry scale (SAAG-S)

existing statistical techniques. However, even with some statistical background, reading and

Personal Well-being Among Medical Students: Findings from a Pilot Survey

Packianathan Chelladurai Troy University, Troy, Alabama, USA.

The MHSIP: A Tale of Three Centers

Recent developments for combining evidence within evidence streams: bias-adjusted meta-analysis

Critical Evaluation of the Beach Center Family Quality of Life Scale (FQOL-Scale)

Deakin Research Online Deakin University s institutional research repository DDeakin Research Online Research Online This is the published version:

Validating the Factorial Structure of the Malaysian Version of Revised Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2 among Young Taekwondo Athletes

MAKE A DIFFERENCE IN THE LIVES OF STUDENTS: INVESTIGATION OF INTEREST AND SOCIAL SUPPORT IN PHYSICAL EDUCATION SETTING

Between- and Within-Domain Relations of Students Academic Emotions

Multifactor Confirmatory Factor Analysis

The measurement of media literacy in eating disorder risk factor research: psychometric properties of six measures

VALIDATION OF TWO BODY IMAGE MEASURES FOR MEN AND WOMEN. Shayna A. Rusticus Anita M. Hubley University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada

Self-Related and Other-Related Pathways to Subjective Well-Being in Japan and the United States

Self-Regulation of Academic Motivation: Advances in Structure and Measurement

Measurement Models for Behavioral Frequencies: A Comparison Between Numerically and Vaguely Quantified Reports. September 2012 WORKING PAPER 10

Perceived Fairness of Pay: The Importance of Task versus Maintenance Inputs in Japan, South Korea, and Hong Kong

The Ego Identity Process Questionnaire: Factor Structure, Reliability, and Convergent Validity in Dutch-Speaking Late. Adolescents

Reliability Analysis: Its Application in Clinical Practice

Validation of Intercultural Sensitivity Measure Individualism Collectivism

Cross-Cultural Validation of the Task and Ego Orientation in Sport Questionnaire

Keywords culture, social support seeking, self-esteem, relational concerns, influence and adjustment

MISSING DATA AND PARAMETERS ESTIMATES IN MULTIDIMENSIONAL ITEM RESPONSE MODELS. Federico Andreis, Pier Alda Ferrari *

Transcription:

Psychological Test and Assessment Modeling, Volume 56, 2014 (3), 304-313 The Self Group Distinction Scale: A new approach to measure individualism and collectivism in adolescents Takuya Yanagida 1, Dagmar Strohmeier 2, Yuichi Toda 3 & Christiane Spiel 4 Abstract Individualism/Collectivism (I/C) was defined as a group orientation characterized by the degree of the convergence of an individual s opinion with an anchor group opinion. The Self Group Distinction (SGD) Scale as a new measurement using difference scores was developed. In sum, 532 Japanese adolescents with a mean age of 12.3 years (SD = 1.78 years) and 277 Austrian with a mean age of 11.96 years (SD = 1.81 years) were asked to indicate their own and the perceived class opinion with respect to seven items covering different aspects of I/C. Confirmatory factor analyses of difference scores demonstrated scalar measurement invariance between cultural groups. Validity was demonstrated by a smaller self-group distinction in Japanese compared with Austrian adolescents. Keywords: individualism, collectivism, cultural differences, cross-cultural, measurement 1 Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: Mag. Takuya Yanagida, School of Applied Health and Social Sciences, University of Applied Sciences Upper Austria, Garnisonstr. 21, 4020 Linz, Austria; email: takuya.yanagida@fh-linz.at 2 University of Applied Sciences Upper Austria, Austria 3 Osaka University of Education, Japan 4 University of Vienna, Austria

The Self Group Distinction Scale 305 Individualism and collectivism (I/C) are widely used constructs to explain differences in social behaviour between people living in different cultural contexts. Most studies have been conducted with adults while studies on adolescents are rather sparse. The main goal of the present paper is to introduce a new approach to measure I/C in adolescents. Usually, individualism and collectivism (I/C) have been broadly defined referring to several content domains. For instance, Kim, Triandis, Kagitcibasi, Choi and Yoon (1994) proposed four main defining attributes and identified 60 additional attributes. Some instruments captured I/C as a uni-dimensional, bipolar construct (e.g., Hui, 1988), while others distinguished several independent I/C constructs (e.g., Singelis, Triandis, Bhawuk, & Gelfand, 1995). Furthermore, Oyserman, Coon and Kemmelmeier (2002) identified six content domains capturing individualism and eight content domains capturing collectivism. To date, I/C has most often been measured with a selection from these content domains while no agreement on a core definition on I/C has been achieved. Studies that compared Japanese adults with adults stemming from individualistic countries (e.g., Austria; Hofstede, 2001) regarding several content domains (see Oyserman, et al., 2002) produced inconclusive results. For instance, there is an ongoing debate about whether people with Japanese origin should be considered collectivistic or not (Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Takano & Sogon, 2008). We assume that these inconsistencies partly stem from the heterogeneity of content domains investigated in the different studies. Clearly, there is the need for a focused definition of I/C and a new measurement approach which does not solely rely on an arbitrary selection of different content domains. Therefore, the main goals of the present study are (1) to propose a focused definition of I/C and (2) to develop a new measurement approach. We define the I/C construct as group orientation characterized by the degree of the assumed convergence of an individual s opinion with an anchor group opinion. In other words we define individualism / collectivism as a psychological process in which self-group distinction is existent (= individualism) or negligible (= collectivism). Thus, we conceptualize I/C as an uni-dimensional, bipolar construct. Development of a new measurement approach To begin with, it is necessary to use an adequate measurement strategy to represent the essence of the proposed definition of I/C. One option is to rely on the subjective assessment of the distinction between self and group opinion based on a relevant anchor group, for example Compared with my group, I am. This direct approach to measure a possible self-group distinction has several limitations. For instance, participants have to consider their own opinion and the anchor group opinion simultaneously which requires a high level of abstract thinking. In addition, this way of asking might be affected by social desirability when respondents consciously intend to minimize the difference between their personal and an assumed group opinion. Other answering biases located in the individual might also be at work if a direct self-group distinction measurement approach is applied. Another option is to rely on an indirect measurement approach by asking the participants separately about their own opinion and a relevant anchor group

306 T. Yanagida, D. Strohmeier, Y. Toda & C. Spiel opinion and to subsequently calculate difference scores between these two items. Example items would consist of a combination of the item I am and the item My group is. Such an indirect approach substantially reduces the cognitive complexity of the items and is more suitable for children and adolescents. This indirect approach to measure self-group distinction can also be expected to be less influenced by social desirability biases. Consequently, the indirect approach represents a more objective estimate of the perceived distinction between self and group opinion than the direct approach. Furthermore, by calculating difference scores within individuals answering biases located in the individual are controlled per item. Thus, our new approach relies on an indirect measurement. We ask adolescents separately about their own opinion and a relevant anchor group opinion. We assume that the individually calculated difference scores between these opinions represent the self-group distinction. Second, regarding the content of the items, it is crucial to define a relevant anchor group which is meaningful for the intended participants. Hui (1988) suggested several relational contexts, namely spouse, parent, kin, neighbour, friend, and co-worker which could serve as anchor groups for adult participants. So far, for adolescents meaningful relational contexts for I/C measurement have not been proposed. Because all adolescents are enrolled in schools we decided to take the class as the anchor group for developing the new measurement. Content domains for I/C found in meta-analyses (e.g., Oysermann et al. 2002) were used to formulate the concrete items. Obviously, when using the indirect approach of measurement the content of the items formulated for self and group must be identical to be able to calculate difference scores. Third, for making valid comparisons between different cultural groups, latent mean and covariance structures should be used (Little, 1997). An important precondition for this approach is to constrain factor loadings and intercepts. This corresponds to intercept or scalar measurement invariance (Chen, 2008). Intercept or scalar measurement invariance ensures that mean differences between cultural groups is attributed to latent characteristics and not to inconsistent measurement characteristics. Consequently, our new instrument should fulfil intercept or scalar invariance. The present study The main goal of the present study was to develop a new approach to measure individualism and collectivism in adolescents based on the degree of the convergence of an individual s opinion with an anchor group opinion. This new approach is called Self Group Distinction (SGD) Scale. After establishing measurement invariance, the validity of the SGD Scale was tested in comparing latent means between Japanese and Austrian adolescents. We expect higher levels of difference scores in the Austrians compared with the Japanese adolescents.

The Self Group Distinction Scale 307 Method Procedure Japanese and Austrian adolescents were chosen to participate in this research, because Austria represents a vertically oriented individualistic culture, while Japan represents a vertically oriented collectivistic culture (Singelis, et al., 1995; Triandis, 1995). Japanese adolescents were recruited from sixteen classes of one elementary and one junior high school situated in a city in Japan 5. In line with standard ethical procedures in Japan, the school principals and teachers had to accept the study before data collection. Austrian adolescents were recruited in eleven classes of one elementary and one academic secondary school situated in a city in Austria 6. In line with standard ethical procedures in Austria, the local school council, the school principals and the parents had to actively accept the study before data collection. Sample In sum, 532 Japanese students (53% boys) aged 12.33 (SD = 1.78) and 277 Austrian students (54% boys) aged 11.96 (SD = 1.81) participated in this study. Altogether, 64 records (7.91%) were incomplete. The percentage of missing values across the 14 variables varied between 1.11 and 2.72%. Missing data were imputed 20 times separately within Japanese and Austrian students based on multivariate imputation by chained equations implemented in the mice package in R (van Buuren & Groothius-Oudshoorn, 2011). The imputation model not only included all variables used in the analyses but several potential auxiliary variables (Collins, Schafer, & Kam, 2001). Instrument The Self Group Distinction (SGD) scale comprised two question blocks (perceived group opinion vs. personal opinion), each containing seven items of equivalent content. Perceived Group Opinion. The first item block measured the perceived group opinion and was administered with the following introduction: In this section, we want to know what your class thinks about various topics. Please think about the general opinion in your class. Personal Opinion. The second item block measured the personal opinion and was administered with the following introduction: 5 In Japan, elementary school comprises grades 1 to 6 and junior high school comprises grades 7 to 9. 6 In Austria, elementary school comprises grades 1 to 4. After primary school pupils can either attend a general secondary school (grade 5 to 8) or an academic secondary school (grade 5 to 12).

308 T. Yanagida, D. Strohmeier, Y. Toda & C. Spiel In this section, we want to know what you think about various topics. Please think about your own opinion. The two question blocks (perceived group opinion vs. personal opinion) comprised the same seven items: What does your class think / What do you think (1) of classmates asking for advice when they have a problem? (advice seeking) (2) of solving tasks in groups? (group activity) (3) of classmates holding a different opinion than the teacher does? (independence) (4) of classmates, who do not want to participate in group activity? (group activity) (5) of other classmates, who want to push through their own opinion? (independence) (6) of classmates solving a difficult task completely on their own? (independence) (7) of a classmate refusing to change his or her opinion, even though all the others think differently than he or she does? (independence) The contents of these seven items, advice seeking, group activity, and independence represent three main content domains of individualism and collectivism and were taken from Oyserman et al. (2002). For all items the answer options were presented on a five point Likert-Scale: I think this is very good (5), rather good (4), neither good nor bad (3), rather bad (2), and very bad (1). To measure Self Group Distinction, the difference of each pair of items was computed, i.e., the person opinion of each individual compared with his or her perceived class opinion. The absolute values of the differences represent the degree of the Self Group Distinction. Thus, the content of the Self Group Distinction scale is not of primary interest, but the degree of the assumed convergence between an adolescent s personal opinion with the class opinion. Scale scores are derived by averaging the items (absolute values of the differences). Cronbach s alpha for the Japanese and Austrian samples are α =.72 and α =.59, respectively. Table 1 shows item-total correlation and internal consistencies of the Self-Group Distinction Scale for Japanese and Austria samples. Table 1: Means, Standard Deviations, Item-Total Correlation and Internal Consistencies of the Self Group Distinction (SGD) Scale M SD Item-Total Correlation α Whole sample 0.47 0.46.40-.46.72 Austria 0.72 0.46.19-.39.59 Japan 0.33 0.39.38-.48.72

The Self Group Distinction Scale 309 Statistical analysis In order to compare latent means between cultural groups, measurement invariance needs to be established. The following steps were taken to test for MI (van de Schoot, Lugtig & Hox 2012): First, we fitted a single-factor measurement model for each cultural group separately (configural invariance). Next, we ran a model where the factor loadings were constrained to be equal across cultural groups (metric invariance). Lastly, we ran a model where the factor loadings and the intercepts were constrained to be equal (scalar invariance). After establishing measurement invariance latent means between Austrian and Japanese students were compared. In the cultural group comparison model, the latent means of Japanese students were constrained to zero. Latent variable modeling program Mplus 7 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-2012) was used to estimate and test the model. Model fit was evaluated using Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), and the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA). Bayesian information criterion (BIC) was used to compare competing models. Results Item means of difference scores First, we checked item means of difference scores between Austrian and Japanese adolescents. As shown in Figure 1, the means of the absolute values of the seven difference scores between perceived class opinion and personal opinion were smaller in the Japanese sample compared with the Austrian sample. This is in line with our hypothesis assuming that Japanese adolescents show a smaller Self-Group Distinction compared with the Austrians. Figure 1: Means of the Absolute Values of the Difference Scores

310 T. Yanagida, D. Strohmeier, Y. Toda & C. Spiel Measurement model The Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) model shown in Figure 2 was fitted for the Austrian (χ²(14) = 12.93, p =.532; CFI = 1.0; TLI = 1.0; RMSEA =.001) and the Japanese sample (χ²(14) = 32.22, p <.01; CFI =.926; TLI =.890; RMSEA =.049) separately. The results showed that the model fit was better in the Austrian sample than in the Japanese sample. Nevertheless, we continued testing for measurement invariance across these cultural groups. As shown in Table 2, the model assuming scalar invariance had the lowest BIC value and therefore the best trade-off between model fit and model complexity. Moreover, fit indices indicated an adequate fit according to CFI and TLI and a good fit according to RMSEA respectively. The latent factor means were different between Austrian and Japanese adolescents (ΔM = 0.335, p <.001). The positive values indicate that Austrian adolescents showed higher differences than Japanese adolescents, indicating higher levels of individualism in Austrian adolescents than in Japanese adolescents. This result is in line with our theoretical consideration, hence offers evidence for the validity of the instrument. To rule out possible answering biases, we re-run all analyses using both a three point scale and dichotomous items. The substantial results did not change indicating that there were no answering biases present (for more details, see Yanagida, 2010). Figure 2: Factor structure of the Self Group Distinction (SGD) Scale. Unstandardized parameter estimates for Austrian/Japanese students

The Self Group Distinction Scale 311 Table 2: Test of measurement invariance of the Self Group Distinction (SGD) Scale. χ² df p CFI TLI RMSEA BIC configural invariance 49.616 28 <.01 0.941 0.911 0.044 11776 metric invariance 58.562 35 <.01 0.935 0.922 0.041 11748 scalar invariance 67.328 40 <.01 0.925 0.921 0.041 11727 Discussion Based on a rather narrow definition of I/C we proposed a new approach to measure individualism and collectivism in adolescents. The I/C construct was characterized by the degree of an assumed convergence of an individual s opinion with an anchor group opinion. Consequently, we applied a measurement strategy which did not rely solely on an arbitrary selection of content domains but on difference scores based on items covering self and group opinion. Because our target group consisted of adolescents, we chose the class as the relevant anchor group. Seven items covering three content domains, namely advice seeking, group activity, and independence (Oysermann et al., 2002) were constructed and formulated for both the own and the class opinion. Thus, the new measurement resulted in 7 pairs of corresponding items. First, we explored the mean level of the difference scores between self and group. Thus, for each pair of corresponding items we inspected the means of the absolute values of the differences between self and class separately for the Austrian and Japanese sample. In line with our theoretical considerations, there were smaller differences in any of the seven item pairs in Japanese adolescents compared with the Austrian adolescents. This is an important finding, because it supports our theoretical considerations regarding I/C and the definition of I/C as group orientation characterized by the degree of the assumed convergence of an individual s opinion with an anchor group opinion. Furthermore, we regard this result as important evidence for the validity of the newly developed measurement approach. In a second step, we compared latent means after constraining factor form, factor loadings and intercepts across Japanese and Austrian adolescents via a multiple group CFA model. The constraint model showed good fit indicating strong measurement invariance (Chen, 2008). Thus, the newly developed Self Group Distinction Scale (SGD scale) validly measures I/C across Japanese and Austrian adolescents. In line with our theoretical considerations, the latent mean level of the Japanese adolescents was lower in comparison with the Austrian adolescents. This indicates that the distinction between self and group is smaller in Japanese adolescents compared with Austrian adolescents. This finding further illustrates the validity of the Self Group Distinction Scale.

312 T. Yanagida, D. Strohmeier, Y. Toda & C. Spiel Strengths and limitations A need for a tighter operationalization of I/C has been claimed in the literature (Oyserman et al, 2002, p. 43), but new measurement approaches in this direction have rarely been developed. This study is the first that conceptualized I/C rather narrowly in terms of a Self-Group Distinction and applied an innovative measurement approach. Consequently, there are several limitations. To begin with, we did not explore associations between the Self Group Distinction Scale and other I/C measures. This would be important to gain a better understanding of several aspects of the I/C construct. However, such a comparison is only possible if both measures prove strong measurement invariance. Unfortunately, strong measurement invariance was only rarely demonstrated for I/C measures in the literature (Fischer, et al., 2009). In fact, we collected data with the Collectivism Scale (Yamaguchi, 1994) which however failed strong measurement invariance in our data (Yanagida, 2010). Therefore, we were not able to consider this scale in the current analyses. Second, we only used class as the anchor group to estimate the distinction between self and group opinion. It would be worthwhile to apply our approach to other relevant anchor groups as well, e.g. to family, friends, or peer groups outside school to be able to generalize our findings to different relational contexts. Third, we chose only one representative country for an individualistic and one representative for a collectivistic culture, namely Austria and Japan. Although these two representatives were chosen for a good reason, because both are considered to be vertically oriented (Triandis, 1995), it would be nice to examine the SGD scale in other individualistic and collectivistic countries as well. Overall, cross-validation of the SGD scale is needed. In conclusion, the SGD scale based on our new measurement approach proved to be a promising new measure for the I/C construct which is worth further development. Author note We are grateful to all the schools, teachers and students who participated in this study. The great support and help from Mr. M. Miki made the data collection in the Japanese high school possible. Also, we wish to thank Anja Lampert and Eva-Maria Schiller for their help with data collection and data input and Hanna Steiner who helped the German research team to develop the questionnaire. References Chen, F. F. (2008). What happens if we compare chopsticks with forks? The impact of making inappropriate comparisons in cross-cultural research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1005-1018. Collins, L. M., Schafer, J. L., & Kam, C.-M. (2001). A comparison of inclusive and restrictive strategies in missing data procedures. Psychological Methods, 6, 330-351.

The Self Group Distinction Scale 313 Fischer, R., Ferreira, M. C., Assmar, E., Redford, P., Harb, C., Glazer, S., et al. (2009). Individualism-collectivism as descriptive norms: Development of a subjective norm approach to culture measurement. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40, 187-213. Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture's Consequences. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Hui, C. H. (1988). Measurement of individualism - collectivism. Journal of Research in Personality, 22, 17-36. Kim, U., Triandis, H. C., Kagitcibasi, C., Choi, S. C., & Yoon, G. (1994). Individualism and collectivism: Theory, method, and application. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Little, T. D. (1997). Mean and Covariance Structures (MACS) Analyses of Cross-Cultural Data: Practical and theoretical issues. Multivariate Behavioural Research, 32(1), 53-76. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Pyschological Review, 98, 224-253. Muthén, L.K. and Muthén, B.O. (1998-2012). Mplus User s Guide. Seventh Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3-72. Singelis, T. M., Triandis, H. C., Bhawuk, D. P., & Gelfand, M. J. (1995). Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: A theoretical and measurement refinement. Cross-Cultural Research, 29, 240-275. Takano, Y., & Sogon, S. (2008). Are Japanese more collectivistic than Americans? Examining conformity in in-groups and the reference-group effect. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39(3), 237-250. Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism & Collectivism. Boulder: Westview Press. van Buuren, S. & Groothius-Oudshoorn, K. (2011). mice: Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45, 1-67. van de Schoot, R., Lugtig, P., & Hox. J. (2012). A checklist for testing measurement invariance. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 486-492. Yamaguchi, S. (1994). Collectivism among the Japanese: A perspective from the self. In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, C. Kagitcibasi, S. C. Choi & G. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and collectivism: Theory, method, and application (pp. 175-188). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Yanagida, T. (2010). Individualismus und Kollektivismus. Äquivalenzprüfung zweier Messinstrumente für Kinder und Jugendliche [Individualism and Collectivism: Testing Equivalence of two Measures for Children and Adolescents]. Saarbrücken: Verlag Dr. Müller.