Aesthetic preferences in the size of images of real-world objects

Similar documents
Viewpoint-dependent recognition of familiar faces

Normative Representation of Objects: Evidence for an Ecological Bias in Object Perception and Memory

Exploring a brightness-drag illusion. Author. Published. Journal Title DOI. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Griffith Research Online

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

Wobbling appearance of a face induced by doubled parts

SHORT AND SWEET When walls are no longer barriers: Perception of wall height in parkour

Look before you leap: Jumping ability affects distance perception

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Gathering and Repetition of the Elements in an Image Affect the Perception of Order and Disorder

Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces

Judging body weight from faces: The height ^ weight illusion

Integrating Episodic Memories and Prior Knowledge. at Multiple Levels of Abstraction. Pernille Hemmer. Mark Steyvers. University of California, Irvine

When facial attractiveness is only skin deep

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

THE EFFECT OF EXPECTATIONS ON VISUAL INSPECTION PERFORMANCE

Meaning-based guidance of attention in scenes as revealed by meaning maps

Supplementary Materials

Integrating episodic memories and prior knowledge at multiple levels of abstraction

Scene recognition following locomotion around a scene

Frequency Distributions

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Evaluation of CBT for increasing threat detection performance in X-ray screening

The Impact of Schemas on the Placement of Eyes While Drawing.

Evaluation of CBT for increasing threat detection performance in X-ray screening

Structure mapping in spatial reasoning

Frequency Distributions

Last but not least. Perception, 2003, volume 32, pages 249 ^ 252

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1. Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen

Numerical bias in bounded and unbounded number line tasks

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

OPTIC FLOW IN DRIVING SIMULATORS

Dimensional interaction in distance judgment

Behavioural Brain Research

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report WE SAW IT ALL ALONG: VISUAL HINDSIGHT BIAS IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS

An experimental investigation of consistency of explanation and graph representation

Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth

The Effects of Action on Perception. Andriana Tesoro. California State University, Long Beach

Effect of Choice Set on Valuation of Risky Prospects

A model of parallel time estimation

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

THE "FOUR STIMULUS - TWO CHOICE " - PARADIGM IN MULTIDIMENSIONAL PSYCHOPHYSICS: SIZE, BRIGHTNESS, AND COLOR DIMENSIONS COMBINED *

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

A Comparison of Three Measures of the Association Between a Feature and a Concept

Color Difference Equations and Their Assessment

The (Dynamic) Mind in the Cave: Representational Space of Cave Paintings and Petroglyphs

7 Grip aperture and target shape

The relationship perceived between the real body and the mirror image

The obligatory nature of holistic processing of faces in social judgments

Active and passive movements give rise to different judgments of coldness

MODELS FOR THE ADJUSTMENT OF RATING SCALES 1

Transfer of Dimensional Associability in Human Contingency Learning

Myers Psychology for AP* David G. Myers PowerPoint Presentation Slides by Kent Korek Germantown High School Worth Publishers, 2010

Memory for object position in natural scenes

The roles of encoding, retrieval, and awareness. in change detection.

REVIEW PROBLEMS FOR FIRST EXAM

Coding location in enclosed spaces: is geometry the principle?

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity

Pushing the Right Buttons: Design Characteristics of Touch Screen Buttons

..." , \ I \ ... / ~\ \ : \\ ~ ... I CD ~ \ CD> 00\ CD) CD 0. Relative frequencies of numerical responses in ratio estimation1

The glare effect does not give rise to a longer-lasting afterimage

Supplemental Material. Experiment 1: Face Matching with Unlimited Exposure Time

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

The Color of Similarity

Decline of the McCollough effect by orientation-specific post-adaptation exposure to achromatic gratings

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences

EFFECT OF SPEED AND ACCURACY INSTRUCTIONS ON CONFIDENCE JUDGMENTS FOR MAGNITUDE ESTIMATES*.

Rapid fear detection relies on high spatial frequencies

A model of the interaction between mood and memory

Configural information is processed differently in perception and recognition of faces

Non-categorical approaches to property induction with uncertain categories

HARRISON ASSESSMENTS DEBRIEF GUIDE 1. OVERVIEW OF HARRISON ASSESSMENT

Grouping by similarity is mediated by feature selection: evidence from the failure of cue combination

The path of visual attention

Comment on W.S. Cleveland, A Model for Studying Display Methods of Statistical Graphics. Leland Wilkinson SYSTAT, Inc. and Northwestern University

Are Faces Special? A Visual Object Recognition Study: Faces vs. Letters. Qiong Wu St. Bayside, NY Stuyvesant High School

Developmental Changes in the Use of Absolute and Relative Information: The Case of Spatial Extent

A Bayesian Account of Reconstructive Memory

Characterizing Visual Attention during Driving and Non-driving Hazard Perception Tasks in a Simulated Environment

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Task timeline for Solo and Info trials.

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

The dependence of motion repulsion and rivalry on the distance between moving elements

SHORT REPORT Facial features influence the categorization of female sexual orientation

The Role of Color and Attention in Fast Natural Scene Recognition

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

Threat modulates perception of looming visual stimuli

CHANGES IN VISUAL SPATIAL ORGANIZATION: RESPONSE FREQUENCY EQUALIZATION VERSUS ADAPTATION LEVEL

Learning Deterministic Causal Networks from Observational Data

Validating the Visual Saliency Model

Aesthetic Response to Color Combinations: Preference, Harmony, and Similarity. Supplementary Material. Karen B. Schloss and Stephen E.

Subjective randomness and natural scene statistics

MEDIA KIT THE ANGLE ORTHODONTIST EDITOR-IN-CHIEF STEVEN J. LINDAUER, DMD, MDSC ANGLE.ORG. Volume 87 (6)

JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION NORMAN H. ANDERSON

Transcription:

Perception, 2011, volume 40, pages 291 ^ 298 doi:10.1068/p6835 Aesthetic preferences in the size of images of real-world objects Sarah Linsen, Mieke H R Leyssen, Jonathan Sammartinoô, Stephen E Palmerô Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Tiensestraat 102, 3000 Leuven, Belgium; e-mail: sarah.linsen@live.com; ô Department of Psychology, University of California at Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA Received 24 September 2010, in revised form 27 January 2011; published online 12 April 2011 Abstract. Konkle and Oliva (in press, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance) found that the preferred (`canonical') visual size of a picture of an object within a frame is proportional to the logarithm of its known physical size. They used within-participants designs on several tasks, including having participants adjust the object's size to `look best'. We examined visual size preference in 2AFC tasks with explicit aesthetic instructions to choose: ``which of each pair you like best''. We also used both within- and between-participants conditions to investigate the possible role of demand characteristics. In experiments 1 and 2, participants saw all possible image pairs depicting the same object at six different sizes for twelve real-world objects that varied in physical size. Significant effects of known physical size were present, regardless of whether participants made judgments about a single object (the between-participants design) or about all objects intermixed (the within-participants design). Experiment 3 showed a reduced effect when the amount of image detail present at different visual sizes was kept constant by posterizing the images. The results are discussed in terms of ecological biases on aesthetic preferences. 1 Introduction In earlier research, Konkle and Oliva (in press) found that the preferred visual size of a picture of an object is proportional to the logarithm of its known physical size. They showed that, when viewing pictures of objects of different physical sizes within a frame, smaller sizes within the frame were preferred for smaller objects in the real world (eg strawberries or a key), whereas larger sizes in the frame were preferred for larger real-world objects (eg a piano or chair). They called these effects `canonical size' in analogy with Palmer et al's (1981) `canonical perspective' effects, showing that people systematically prefer some perspective views of objects over others. Konkle and Oliva employed within-participants designs using multiple objects of differing physical sizes in several tasks, including a perceptual-preference task, in which participants were asked to view images of real-world objects on the computer monitor and to adjust the image size so that the object `looked best'. They found that, when observers could freely resize objects on the screen, the preferred visual size of the object was proportional to the logarithm of its known physical size. Thus they concluded that knowledge about the physical size of objects systematically influences the visual size at which objects are preferentially viewed. In a similar vein, Bertamini et al (in press) recently analyzed images from two corpora of artistic images of diverse sizes of animals and found reliable positive correlations between the physical sizes of the animals depicted and the sizes of their drawn or painted images in both. Unfortunately, neither of these findings settles the question we address here, namely whether there is an aesthetic bias for the sizes of images of objects to be systematically related to the physical sizes of those objects. Konkle and Oliva's instructions could be interpreted in two rather different ways: participants might choose the image that `looks best' to be the one at which it `looks most like the object it depicts' (analogous to Palmer et al's explicit instructions in their research on canonical perspective), or they might choose the image that they found to be `most aesthetically pleasing'.

292 S Linsen, M H R Leyssen, J Sammartino, S E Palmer These tasks may or may not produce the same results. The findings of Bertamini et al (in press) may be interpreted as implying that the artists found the images of animals to be more aesthetically pleasing when their sizes were consistent with the sizes of the depicted animals, but we do not know that this is why they drew or painted them in this way, and we do not know whether viewers might also see them as more aesthetically pleasing when a canonical size bias is present in the images. Because we are particularly interested in the aesthetic question (cf Palmer et al 2008; Sammartino and Palmer, submitted), we repeated Konkle and Oliva's experiment using explicit aesthetic instructions, asking participants to choose the picture that they `liked best' (rather than the one that `looks best'). We were also concerned about possible demand characteristics that might influence size preference judgments. Konkle and Oliva showed the same participant many objects of different sizes, which might lead him/her to feel some implicit pressure to make responses consistent with the objects' known physical sizes. They replicated their effects with more restricted ranges of object sizes, but each participant still saw several objects of different sizes. To rule out the possible influence of demand characteristics, we employed both a fully between-participants design, in which each person saw only one object, and a fully within-participants design, in which each person saw objects of all different sizes. In previous research on aesthetic response to spatial composition of pictures containing a single object, preferences for the vertical position of single objects within a rectangular frame were examined (Sammartino and Palmer, submitted). Sammartino and Palmer found an ecological bias in which preferences were driven by the typical viewer-relative position of the object in the world. Objects that are typically positioned above the viewer in the world (eg a flying eagle or a ceiling-mounted light fixture) are preferred higher in the frame than objects that are typically below the viewer (eg a swimming stingray or a bowl on a table). This effect, which might be termed `canonical height', is consistent with their general principle of representational fit, according to which viewers are biased toward more positive aesthetic responses when the spatial characteristics of objects in the world are reflected in corresponding spatial characteristics of their images within the frame (Gardner and Palmer 2009, 2010; Palmer et al 2011). The present experiments can thus be understood as testing for an analogous ecological bias in the size dimension. 2 Experiment 1. Canonical-size effects on aesthetic judgments of multiple objects In the first experiment, we repeated Konkle and Oliva's within-participants experiment using explicitly aesthetic instructions and a different set of objects and images. Rather than asking participants to judge images in terms of which `looked best', we asked them to judge them in terms of which they `liked best'. We also used a 2AFC method to avoid memory effects that may have been present in the adjustment procedure used by Konkle and Oliva. 2.1 Method 2.1.1 Participants. Twenty-three participants took part in the experiment. Fifteen were students at the University of California, Berkeley, who received credit as partial fulfilment of their course requirements. The other eight received compensation for participating. Their mean age was 20.7 years, ranging from 18 to 29 years. All were naive to the purpose of the experiment and gave informed consent in accord with the policies of the University of California, Berkeley, Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects, which approved the experimental protocol. 2.1.2 Design. Each trial consisted of two images of the same object that differed only in size. There were thirty paired comparisons per object, resulting from all possible comparisons of six sizes of each object, balanced for left/right screen position. Six animals and six artifacts (see figure 1a) were used in the experiment, resulting in a total of 360 trials.

Aesthetic preferences in the size of images of real-world objects 293 (a) (b) Figure 1. [In color online, see http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p6835] Examples of images used in experiment 1. Images of the animals and artifacts (a), and the full set of sizes for the butterfly image relative to the frame (b). 2.1.3 Displays. The estimated physical size of the objects along their longest dimension ranged from 1 inch to 12 feet for the animals and from 1 inch to 10 feet for the artifacts. High-resolution images were obtained from the Internet and their backgrounds were deleted. The images were presented in six different sizes constructed by starting with the high-resolution image at the largest size that would fill the frame without cropping and then reducing it to the smaller sizes using the bicubic resampling function in Photoshop. The sizes were reduced in equal intervals by area (ie bounding box area was 1:6, 1:3, 1:2, 2:3, and 5:6). For an overview of the different stimuli and sizes see figures 1a and 1b. The monitor measured 20 inches diagonally, and had a resolution of 12806768 pixels. Each image was presented in a frame that measured 6006450 pixels. The frames were presented side-by-side, with the center of the frames presented 125 pixels vertically and 200 pixels horizontally from the center of the screen. 2.1.4 Procedure. Participants sat in a darkened booth and were told that they would be presented with a series of image pairs that they were to judge for aesthetic preference. They were presented with a slide listing all objects that would appear in the experiment. They were also shown a slide of an object that did not appear in the experiment itself to indicate the different image sizes that would be presented in the experiment. In the 2AFC task participants were shown two centered, framed images of the same object in two different sizes relative to the frame. All object pairs were randomly intermixed. Participants were instructed to indicate which picture of each pair they liked better. A 2 s delay was enforced before participants could make their response to ensure that they really looked at the image pairs. After the 2 s delay, the statement ``Please make your response'' appeared at the bottom of the screen, at which time the participant had to press the left-arrow or right-arrow key, after which the images disappeared. All pairs were presented in a random order. 2.2 Results and discussion The sizes of the images relative to the frame were represented as the integers from 1 (smallest) to 6 (largest). For each object and each participant, the average size chosen was calculated over all possible pairs. The averages of these data are plotted as a function of the logarithm of each object's physical size in figure 2. The correlation between the logarithm of the physical size and the average size chosen for the image of that object was 0:84 (t 10 ˆ 4:9, p 5 0:001). The best-fitting linear regression line predicting the average size chosen from the logarithm of the object's actual size had a positive slope of 0.30. We assessed the significance of this effect by computing the slope of the regression lines separately for each participant and found that the resulting slopes were reliably greater than zero (t 22 ˆ 4:98, p 5 0:001).

294 S Linsen, M H R Leyssen, J Sammartino, S E Palmer 5.0 4.5 maximum y ˆ 0:30x 3:39 animals artifacts Average size chosen 4.0 3.5 3.0 2.5 r ˆ 0:84 p 5 0:001 minimum Animals Artifacts 2.0 2.5 25 50 125 250 500 Physical size=cm (logarithmic scale) Figure 2. [In color online] Results of experiment 1. The average chosen size over all possible pairs for each animal (blue) and artifact (orange), plotted as a function of the logarithm of its physical size. When objects have a smaller physical size, images of them tend to be preferred aesthetically when they are smaller relative to the frame and, when objects have a larger physical size, they tend to be preferred aesthetically when they are larger relative to the frame. The present results show that canonical-size effects are indeed present in aesthetic judgments, replicating Konkle and Oliva's findings for this aspect of canonical size. This result excludes the possibility that it is only present in evaluating images in terms of how much they look like the objects depicted. It is possible, however, that these effects were due to demand characteristics. In experiment 2, we addressed this issue by using a purely between-participants design. 3 Experiment 2. Canonical-size effects on aesthetic judgments of single objects In experiment 2, we replicated experiment 1 using a between-participants design in which each participant saw only one of the animals from experiment 1. Because no participant knew that any other sizes of objects were being studied with other participants, the results should indicate whether the size effects in experiment 1 could have arisen from explicit or implicit consideration of the relative sizes of the objects they were judging. If so, the size effects in experiment 1 should disappear or be measurably reduced. 3.1 Method 3.1.1 Participants. Eighty-four participants took part in the experiment. Seventy-two of them were students at the University of California, Berkeley, who received partial course credit in an undergraduate psychology course for participating. The other twelve received compensation for participating. The mean age of the participants was 20.1 years, ranging from 18 to 29 years. All were naive to the purpose of the experiment and gave informed consent in accord with the policies of the University of California, Berkeley, Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects, which approved the experimental protocol. 3.1.2 Design. Only the animal series from experiment 1 was used. In this betweenparticipants design, each participant saw only one of the objects instead of all objects intermixed. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the six groups, each of which saw one of the 6 animals at all different sizes (see figure 1).

Aesthetic preferences in the size of images of real-world objects 295 3.1.3 Displays. The displays were the same as those of experiment 1 except that each participant saw only pairs of pictures of a single animal. 3.1.4 Procedure. The apparatus and procedures were identical to those of experiment 1 except that each participant saw only pairs of pictures of a single animal. 3.2 Results and discussion As in experiment 1, we calculated the average size chosen over all possible pairs for each object and each participant. The pattern of results replicated that in experiment 1, as indicated by a correlation of 0:94 ( p 5 0:01) between the average sizes chosen in experiment 1 and the average sizes chosen for the same object in experiment 2. We also analyzed the data in the same way as for experiment 1. The correlation between the average sizes chosen and the logarithm of physical sizes was 0:76 (t 4 ˆ 2:34, p 5 0:05, one-tailed). We then calculated a regression line to predict the average sizes chosen as a function of the logarithm of the objects' physical sizes (see figure 3). The results show a positive slope (0.40), which is not significantly different from the slope of the regression line from experiment 1 just for the animals (0.36). Because each participant saw only one size of object, within-participants regression lines could not be computed and the statistical significance of the positive slope could not be assessed. Average size chosen 5.0 4.5 4.0 3.5 3.0 2.5 maximum y ˆ 0:40x 3:34 r ˆ 0:76 minimum Animals 2.0 2.5 25 50 125 250 500 Physical size=cm (logarithmic scale) Figure 3. [In color online] Results of experiment 2. The average chosen size over all possible pairs for each animal, plotted as a function of the logarithm of its physical size. The canonical-size effect is clearly still present when participants saw only a single object. People thus prefer pictures of smaller objects to be smaller in the frame and pictures of larger objects to be larger in the frame. We conclude that the effect of canonical size on aesthetic judgments is not driven by demand characteristics but by the relation between the viewer's knowledge of object sizes and the sizes of their images within a surrounding frame. 4 Experiment 3. Effects of detail: Posterized images The amount of spatial detail necessarily covaries with image size in high-resolution digital photographs when displayed on a fixed resolution monitor. It is therefore possible that the canonical-size effect reported above arises from people having a preferred amount of detail for different objects rather than a preferred overall size. If people know more about the visual details of larger objects than smaller ones, for example, and if they prefer the smallest image in which the requisite spatial detail is present, then canonical-size preference effects

296 S Linsen, M H R Leyssen, J Sammartino, S E Palmer could result from the amount of perceived detail. The images in experiments 1 and 2 were constructed by starting with a high-resolution image at a large size within the frame and reducing it (in Photoshop) to the smaller sizes (see figure 1b). This procedure necessarily reduces the amount of detail present as the size of the image decreases. In the present experiment we therefore attempted to equate the amount of image detail across image sizes by posterizing the smallest image using relatively few colors and then enlarging it without changing the spatial resolution of the vectorized image (see figure 4). If the size effects observed in experiments 1 and 2 are influenced by the amount of image detail, they should be reduced or eliminated in the present experiment. Figure 4. [In color online] Example images from experiment 3: The posterized butterfly and tea cup. 4.1 Method 4.1.1 Participants. All twenty-eight participants were students at the University of California, Berkeley, who received partial course credit in their undergraduate psychology course for participating. Their mean age was 19.9 years, ranging from 18 years to 25 years. All were naive to the purpose of the experiment and gave informed consent in accord with the policies of the University of California, Berkeley, Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects, which approved the experimental protocol. 4.1.2 Design. The stimuli used in this experiment were posterized versions of the animals and artifacts used in experiment 1. Reducing the size of a high-resolution image reduces the amount of visual detail present in that image. More technically, a large image of a cup contains more pixels than a small image of that same cup, allowing for higherresolution details to be visible in larger images. We therefore posterized the images in Photoshop to equate the amount of information in the different images sizes. Posterization removes all gradients of lightness and color, and replaces them with homogeneous areas of color. As part of the posterization process, the number of different colors in the resulting image can be chosen, thus affecting the amount of gradient compression (see figure 4 for two examples). We posterized the smallest version of each image using 5 different colors. These posterized versions were then converted to vector graphic images (to eliminate edge aliasing) and were scaled up to the larger sizes. As before, each trial consisted of two images of an object in different sizes. Different sizes of the same image of an object were shown in all possible paired comparisons. 4.1.3 Displays. The displays were identical to those of experiment 1 except that each participant saw posterized versions of the animals and artifacts. 4.1.4 Procedure. The apparatus and procedure were the same as in experiment 1. As before, we used a 2AFC task in which we instructed participants to indicate which picture they `liked best'. 4.2 Results and discussion We analyzed the current data in the same way as in experiment 1. Figure 5 shows the average chosen size over all possible pairs for each object, plotted as a function of the logarithm of its physical size. The correlation between the logarithm of the physical

Aesthetic preferences in the size of images of real-world objects 297 5.0 4.5 maximum not owning pets owning pets Average size chosen 4.0 3.5 3.0 2.5 y ˆ 0:18x 3:44 r ˆ 0:34 p 5 0:001 minimum Pets 2.0 2.5 12.5 25 37.5 50 62.5 Physical size=cm (logarithmic scale) Figure 5. [In color online] Results of experiment 3. The average chosen size over all pairs for each posterized object plotted as a function of the logarithm of its physical size. size of the object and the average size chosen for the image of that object is 0:58 (t 10 ˆ 2:25, p 5 0:05). As before, we calculated a regression line on the group data, which shows a positive slope of 0.17. We assessed the significance of this effect by computing the slope of the regression line separately for each participant, and the resulting slopes were reliably greater than zero (t 27 ˆ 3:84, p 5 0:001). The slope appears to be reduced relative to the slope of the corresponding regression line in experiment 1 (0.30), however. We then tested the hypothesis that the slopes of the individual participants' regression lines in experiment 3 were smaller than those in experiment 1 and found that the betweengroups difference was significant (t 49 ˆ 1:72, p 5 0:05). The present results demonstrate that, even when the amount of image detail is equated across sizes, objects that are smaller in physical size are preferred to be smaller in the frame and objects that are larger in physical size are preferred to be larger in a frame. This indicates that the canonical-size effects obtained in experiment 1 and 2 are not solely due to artifacts of image detail. Nevertheless, the magnitude of the size effect is reduced somewhat when the amount of detail is objectively equated. This reduction suggests that there may be some effect of the amount of image detail present in the results of experiments 1 and 2. It is also possible that the present reduction in the slope of the size function occurs because people tend not to like posterized images when they are large enough that they begin to look like cartoons. If so, the present results with posterized images may arise from the combination of two opposite effects: a canonical-size effect with a positive slope as a function of image size and a `cartoon' effect with a negative slope for posterized images. This question is the subject of ongoing research. 5 General discussion In experiment 1 we asked whether canonical-size effects are present in explicit judgments of aesthetic preference. In a 2AFC task with pictures of the same object presented in different sizes relative to the frame, we found clear evidence for canonical-size effects analogous to those reported by Konkle and Oliva (in press). This finding cannot be attributed to demand characteristics related to participants considering objects of multiple sizes because it was replicated in experiment 2 in a fully between-participants

298 S Linsen, M H R Leyssen, J Sammartino, S E Palmer experiment in which participants saw only one object. In a third experiment we manipulated the amount of visual detail present in the images by using posterized images and found good evidence for canonical-size effects, albeit with somewhat smaller slopes. It is not yet clear whether this reduction is due to there being a component of the canonical-size effects arising from preferences for specific amounts of detail or to people disliking cartoonish large images that contain little detail. Overall, the findings support a clear bias toward canonical size in aesthetic preferences for framed 2-D images. This bias seems to be conceptually related to another ecological bias reported by Sammartino and Palmer (submitted) for objects that are characteristically located above the viewer in the world to be located high in the picture frame (eg ceiling-mounted light fixtures and flying eagles) and for objects that are characteristically located below the viewer in the world to be located lower in the picture frame (eg bowls on tables and swimming stingrays). We call these effects `ecological' because they appear to be driven by people preferring images in which the spatial properties of the image of the depicted object within its frame fit the ecological properties of the physical object relative to the viewer. Canonical-size effects on aesthetic judgments thus indicate that people tend to prefer images in which the size of the object's image within its frame fits their knowledge of its actual physical size. Acknowledgments. We wish to thank Rosa Poggesi, Eli Strauss, Karen Schloss, and Will Griscom for their support and assistance with the collection and analyses of data. The material described in this article is based on work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant 0 745 820 and a Google Gift to SEP. References Bertamini M, Bennet K M, Bode C, in press ``The anterior bias in visual art: The case of images of animals'' Laterality Gardner J S, Palmer S E, 2009 ``Representational fit in aesthetic judgments of spatial composition'' paper presented at the 9th Annual Meeting of the Vision Sciences Society, Naples, FL, May 2009 Gardner J S, Palmer S E, 2010 ``Representation fit in position and perspective: a unified aesthetic account'' paper presented at the 10th Annual Meeting of the Vision Sciences Society, Naples, FL, May 2010 Konkle T, Oliva A, in press ``Canonical visual size for real-world objects'' Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Palmer S E, Gardner J S, Wickens T D, 2008 `Àesthetic issues in spatial composition: Effects of position and direction on framing single objects'' Spatial Vision 21 421 ^ 449 Palmer S E, Rosch E, Chase P, 1981 ``Canonical perspective and the perception of objects'', in Attention and Performance, IX Eds J Long, A Baddeley (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates) pp 135 ^ 151 Palmer S E, Schloss K B, Sammartino J, 2011 ``Hidden knowledge in aesthetic judgments: Preferences for color and spatial composition'', in Aesthetic Science: Connecting Minds, Brains, and Experience Eds A P Shimamura, S E Palmer (Oxford: Oxford University Press) Sammartino J, Palmer S E, submitted `Àesthetic issues in spatial composition: Effects of vertical position and perspective on framing single objects'' Perception Sammartino J, Palmer S E, in preparation ``Aesthetic issues in spatial composition: Representational fit and the role of context in preference for positiion and perspective'' ß 2011 a Pion publication

ISSN 0301-0066 (print) ISSN 1468-4233 (electronic) www.perceptionweb.com Conditions of use. This article may be downloaded from the Perception website for personal research by members of subscribing organisations. Authors are entitled to distribute their own article (in printed form or by e-mail) to up to 50 people. This PDF may not be placed on any website (or other online distribution system) without permission of the publisher.