Model-free detection of synchrony in neuronal spike trains, with an application to primate somatosensory cortex

Similar documents
1 Introduction Synchronous ring of action potentials amongst multiple neurons is a phenomenon that has been observed in a wide range of neural systems

Synchrony and the attentional state

Input-speci"c adaptation in complex cells through synaptic depression

Adaptive leaky integrator models of cerebellar Purkinje cells can learn the clustering of temporal patterns

Beyond bumps: Spiking networks that store sets of functions

Thalamocortical Feedback and Coupled Oscillators

Long-term depression and recognition of parallel "bre patterns in a multi-compartmental model of a cerebellar Purkinje cell

Precise Spike Timing and Reliability in Neural Encoding of Low-Level Sensory Stimuli and Sequences

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Temporally asymmetric Hebbian learning and neuronal response variability

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System

On the tracking of dynamic functional relations in monkey cerebral cortex

Processing and Plasticity in Rodent Somatosensory Cortex

Supplemental Material

Control of Selective Visual Attention: Modeling the "Where" Pathway

Transitions between dierent synchronous ring modes using synaptic depression

Electrophysiological and firing properties of neurons: categorizing soloists and choristers in primary visual cortex

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex

Supplementary Motor Area exerts Proactive and Reactive Control of Arm Movements

In M.I. Jordan, M.J. Kearns & S.A. Solla (Eds.). (1998). Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 10, MIT Press.

A U'Cortical Substrate of Haptic Representation"

Modeling the interplay of short-term memory and the basal ganglia in sequence processing

Relationships between Spike Train Spatiotemporal Structure and Cortical Synchronization

Spike-timing-dependent synaptic plasticity can form zero lag links for cortical oscillations.

Spatiotemporal clustering of synchronized bursting events in neuronal networks

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

Neurobiology of Hearing (Salamanca, 2012) Auditory Cortex (2) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang

Group Redundancy Measures Reveal Redundancy Reduction in the Auditory Pathway

From single-trial EEG to brain area dynamics

Synfire chains with conductance-based neurons: internal timing and coordination with timed input

A developmental learning rule for coincidence. tuning in the barn owl auditory system. Wulfram Gerstner, Richard Kempter J.

Different inhibitory effects by dopaminergic modulation and global suppression of activity

Structure and Function of the Auditory and Vestibular Systems (Fall 2014) Auditory Cortex (3) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang

Neurons of the cerebral cortex exhibit precise inter spike timing in correspondence to behavior

From feedforward vision to natural vision: The impact of free viewing and clutter on monkey inferior temporal object representations

Jude Francis Mitchell, Ph.D. Curriculum Vitae July 22, 2013

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010

Abstract A neural network model called LISSOM for the cooperative self-organization of

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields.

Spikes that count: rethinking spikiness in neurally embedded systems

A dendritic model of coincidence detection in the avian brainstem

A Novel Account in Neural Terms. Gal Chechik Isaac Meilijson and Eytan Ruppin. Schools of Medicine and Mathematical Sciences

Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch!

Reading Neuronal Synchrony with Depressing Synapses

Timothy J. Buschman Princeton University Assistant Professor, Princeton Neuroscience Institute and Department of Psychology

Sparse Coding in Sparse Winner Networks

Synchrony Generation in Recurrent Networks with Frequency-Dependent Synapses

Consciousness The final frontier!

Neural Representations of Temporally Asymmetric Stimuli in the Auditory Cortex of Awake Primates

LEAH KRUBITZER RESEARCH GROUP LAB PUBLICATIONS WHAT WE DO LINKS CONTACTS

Emanuel Todorov, Athanassios Siapas and David Somers. Dept. of Brain and Cognitive Sciences. E25-526, MIT, Cambridge, MA 02139

Self-Organization and Segmentation with Laterally Connected Spiking Neurons

Measuring the Accuracy of the Neural Code

Attention, intention and salience in the posterior parietal cortex

Beyond Vanilla LTP. Spike-timing-dependent-plasticity or STDP

Deconstructing the Receptive Field: Information Coding in Macaque area MST

Linear filtering. Center-surround differences and normalization. Linear combinations. Inhibition of return. Winner-take-all.

Investigation of Physiological Mechanism For Linking Field Synapses

Supplemental Information. Gamma and the Coordination of Spiking. Activity in Early Visual Cortex. Supplemental Information Inventory:

Behavioral considerations suggest an average reward TD model of the dopamine system

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Elias B Issa, PhD. Curriculum Vitae 10/2015

The individual animals, the basic design of the experiments and the electrophysiological

Brain-computer interface to transform cortical activity to control signals for prosthetic arm

Analysis and Models in Neurophysiology

Power and phase properties of oscillatory neural responses in the presence of background activity

Katharina Anton-Erxleben

Why is our capacity of working memory so large?

Robust Neural Encoding of Speech in Human Auditory Cortex

Elias B Issa, PhD. Curriculum Vitae 08/2016. RESEARCH INTEREST The neural mechanisms and computational principles underlying high-level vision

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation

Peter N. Steinmetz, M.D., Ph.D.

Dendritic compartmentalization could underlie competition and attentional biasing of simultaneous visual stimuli

Computational cognitive neuroscience: 2. Neuron. Lubica Beňušková Centre for Cognitive Science, FMFI Comenius University in Bratislava

Neural Correlates of High-Gamma Oscillations ( Hz) in Macaque Local Field Potentials and Their Potential Implications in Electrocorticography

Temporal Factors in Tactile Spatial Acuity: Evidence for RA Interference in Fine Spatial Processing

Context-Dependent Changes in Functional Circuitry in Visual Area MT

Ch.20 Dynamic Cue Combination in Distributional Population Code Networks. Ka Yeon Kim Biopsychology

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Computational Cognitive Neuroscience (CCN)

Neural Coding Mechanisms Underlying Perceived Roughness of Finely Textured Surfaces

Classification of EEG signals in an Object Recognition task

EDGE DETECTION. Edge Detectors. ICS 280: Visual Perception

PHY3111 Mid-Semester Test Study. Lecture 2: The hierarchical organisation of vision

Oscillations: From Neuron to MEG

Error Detection based on neural signals

Deep Neural Networks Rival the Representation of Primate IT Cortex for Core Visual Object Recognition

Validating the Visual Saliency Model

International Journal of Scientific & Engineering Research Volume 4, Issue 2, February ISSN THINKING CIRCUIT

Information processing at single neuron level*

A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of Medicine

Convergence of Submodality-Specific Input Onto Neurons in Primary Somatosensory Cortex

Resonant synchronization of heterogeneous inhibitory networks

Spatial and Feature-Based Attention in a Layered Cortical Microcircuit Model

Pattern Recognition of Functional Neuroimage Data of the Human Sensorimotor System after Stroke

Temporal patterning of neural synchrony in the basal ganglia in Parkinson s disease

Theme 2: Cellular mechanisms in the Cochlear Nucleus

Neuron, Volume 63 Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in Macaque area V4.

Transcription:

Neurocomputing 32}33 (2000) 1103}1108 Model-free detection of synchrony in neuronal spike trains, with an application to primate somatosensory cortex A. Roy, P.N. Steinmetz, K.O. Johnson, E. Niebur* Krieger Mind/Brain Institute, Johns Hopkins University, 3400N Charles St. Baltimore, MD 21218, USA Accepted 13 January 2000 Abstract Synchronized neuronal "ring has been reported in many neural systems and may play a role in the representation of sensory stimuli and the modi"cation of sensory representations by both experience and attention. In this report we describe a bootstrap procedure for computing the statistical signi"cance of changes in the degree of synchrony and apply it to recordings from the second somatosensory (SII) cortex of Macaques performing tactile and visual discrimination tasks. A majority (68%) of neuron pairs in SII "re synchronously in response to a tactile stimulus. In a fraction of those pairs (17.5%), the degree of synchrony covaries with the focus of attention. 2000 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Synchrony; Bootstrap; Somatosensory cortex 1. Introduction Synchronous "ring of action potentials amongst multiple neurons is a phenomenon that has been observed in a wide range of neural systems. Temporal structures of similar nature have been proposed to play a functional role in representing sensory information, as possible representations of internal behavioral states, and in motor planning [1}4,7,11,10]]. One of the challenges in the "eld is the development of statistical methods suitable for characterizing the signi"cance of synchronous "ring. In the present report, we are concerned with the question whether the degree of synchrony observed changes signi"cantly with the behavioral state of the animal. To this purpose, we develop a model-free analysis of the cross-correlogram [6]. * Corresponding author. Presently at the California Institute of Technology, CA, USA. 0925-2312/00/$ - see front matter 2000 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. PII: S 0 9 2 5-2 3 1 2 ( 0 0 ) 0 0 2 8 4-8

1104 A. Roy et al. / Neurocomputing 32}33 (2000) 1103}1108 Signi"cance was tested by bootstrap procedures at two levels using appropriate null hypothesis. 2. Experimental methods The activity of a total of 553 pairs of neurons in SII from two awake behaving monkeys was recorded using seven extracellular electrodes driven individually [9]. Physiological methods were similar to those described by Hsiao et al. [8]. The experimental protocol required both monkeys to alternate between performing a tactile discrimination task (considered the `attentivea task for recordings in somatosensory cortex) and a visual (`nonattentivea) task. 3. Statistical methods 3.1. Test for signixcance of the degree of synchrony The spike trains of two neurons α and β recorded simultaneously are denoted as S (t) and S (t). Here, n"12n indexes the trial number, for a particular behavioral state m and stimulus s, and t"12¹/b the bin number where ¹ is the length of the spike trains and b the bin width. The raw cross-correlogram C (τ) averaged over N trials is C (τ)" 1 S (t#τ)s (t). (1) N In order to subtract the contribution of stimulus locked mean rate e!ects from the raw cross-correlogram, the shift predictor (or shu%e predictor) is subtracted, yielding the covariogram ζ (τ) ζ (τ)"c (τ)! 1 N (N!1) S (t#τ)s (t). (2) The stimulus-averaged covariogram ζ (τ) for a particular behavioral state m is computed as the average of Eq. (2) over all S stimuli in this state. Our "rst objective is to test whether the covariogram for small time shifts (around τ"0) for a speci"c neuron pair is signi"cantly above noise. We de"ne the deviation of the covariogram from zero (i.e. the degree of synchrony) as the sum-squared area under the covariogram in a 50 ms time window around τ"0 and refer to it as to our statistic S. In particular, let us denote the observed statistic S[ζ (τ)] as Z. The signi"cance of the number of excess coincidences was tested using a bootstrap method [5]. We use the null hypothesis H that the two neurons α and β are More precisely, a Fisher permutation test; results are similar with bootstrapping.

independent. Let Π denote a permutation of N trials for stimulus s and behavioral state m. We generate for each iteration S such permutations and use them to compute a bootstrapped covariogram ζ Π (τ) for each stimulus. The covariogram ζ Π (τ) for a particular iteration under H is computed by averaging over all S such covariograms. The signi"cance level of the observed statistic Z is then tested against the distribution of ZI (S[ζΠ (τ)]) computed from all S iterations to determine the probability that the data could have arisen assuming H is true. No a priori assumptions are required about the underlying nature of the random process generating the spike trains, particularly the independence of "ring in neighboring bins of the binned spike trains or the distribution of the test statistics, contrary to what is assumed in many statistical models. 3.2. Signixcance of attentional ewect A. Roy et al. / Neurocomputing 32}33 (2000) 1103}1108 1105 Our second objective is to determine whether attention has a signi"cant e!ect on synchronous "ring. Only pairs of neurons which possess a signi"cant degree of synchrony according to the test in Section 3.1 were included. Single trial covariograms for each stimulus and behavioral type were computed as in Section 3.1. An appropriate null hypothesis for this test is that the degree of synchrony is independent of behavioral state or presented stimulus. Again, our measure of the degree of change in synchrony, our statistic D, was the sum-squared deviations between the averaged covariograms in a 50 ms time window centered around zero time shift. In order to test the di!erence in synchrony as a function of one of the behavioral states (attended vs. unattended), indexed as m"1 or 2, we compute the observed statistic as Z "D[C (τ)!c (τ)]. (3) If the null hypothesis is true, covariograms obtained by averaging over trials during the tactile task should not be di!erent, except for random variations, from those obtained in the baseline condition, the visual task. Let Π denote a set of n trials drawn with replacement from the set of visual trials and C Π (τ), the averaged covariogram over this set. We use a Monte-Carlo simulation to estimate the distribution of the bootstrapped test statistic, de"ned as ZI (N, N )"D[CΠ (τ)!cπ (τ)] (4) with N " N and N " N. The signi"cance level of Z is then tested against the distribution of ZI (N, N ). 4. Results We applied the above statistical procedures to spike data recorded from macaque area SII while the animals were performing attentional tasks. Table 1, Column 2 shows the number of neuron pairs that showed a signi"cant peak in the covariogram (all signi"cances reported at p(0.05). Overall, 68% of neuron pairs in SII showed

1106 A. Roy et al. / Neurocomputing 32}33 (2000) 1103}1108 Table 1 The second column indicates the fraction of cell pairs which showed signi"cant synchrony (p'0.05) for each monkey. The third column shows the fraction of those which showed a signi"cant change in the synchrony (p(0.5) with the attentional state. Finally, the fourth column indicates the percentage in which synchrony in SII increased with attention directed on the tactile task Monkey Synchrony Change Increase Monkey 1 113/145 (80%) 41/113 (36%) 37/41 (90%) Monkey 2 264/408 (65%) 25/264 (9%) 17/25 (68%) Total 377/553 (68%) 66/377 (17.5%) 54/66 (82%) a signi"cant degree of synchronous "ring. Column 3 shows the number of pairs that had a signi"cant change in the degree of synchronous "ring between the attended and unattended tasks. Overall, 17% of the pairs selected in the "rst test show a signi"cant change in the degree of synchrony with the amount of attention focused on the tactile stimulus. Furthermore, we found that of all pairs with a signi"cant change, 82% had a greater degree of synchronous "ring during performance of the attended tactile detection task than during the visual task (Column 4). 5. Discussion In complex nervous systems, behavior is not only in#uenced by the immediate sensory input but also by the internal cognitive or perceptive state of the animal. It has been suggested on theoretical grounds that the temporal structure of spike trains plays a role in the coding of such states and that the correlational structure of spike trains varies with the attentional state of the animal [10]. We test this prediction by analyzing spike trains recorded from the cortex of an awake behaving monkey working in a task under attentional control. We show that statistically signi"cant increases in the synchrony of neurons in somatosensory are positively correlated with the level of attention devoted to tactile stimuli. These changes in synchronization are evidence for a functional role of synchronous "ring in processing sensory information [12]. Acknowledgements We thank Steve Hsiao and Paul Fitzgerald for the data used in this work and acknowledge support by NSF and the Sloan Foundation. References [1] M. Abeles, Role of the cortical neuron: integrator or coincidence detector? Israel J. Med. Sci. 18 (1982) 83}92.

A. Roy et al. / Neurocomputing 32}33 (2000) 1103}1108 1107 [2] M. Abeles, Corticonics * Neural Circuits of the Cerebral Cortex, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, MA, 1991. [3] J.E. Dayho!, G.L. Gerstein, Favored patterns in spike trains. I. Detection, J. Neurophysiol. 49 (1983) 1334}1348. [4] J.E. Dayho!, G.L. Gerstein, Favored patterns in spike trains. II. Application, J. Neurophysiol. 49 (1983) 1349}1363. [5] B. Efron, R.J. Tibshirani, An Introduction to the Bootstrap, Chapman & Hall, New York, NY, 1993. [6] J.J. Eggermont, The Correlative Brain: Theory and Experiment in Neural Interaction, Springer, Berlin, 1990. [7] G.L. Gerstein, W.A. Clark, Simultaneous studies of "ring patterns in several neurons, Science 143 (1964) 1325}1327. [8] S.S. Hsiao, D.M. O'Shaughnessy, K.O. Johnson, E!ects of selective attention on spatial form processing in monkey primary and secondary somatosensory cortex, J. Neurophysiol. 70 (1) (1993) 444}447. [9] V.B. Mountcastle, H.J. Reitboeck, G.F. Poggio, M.A. Steinmetz, Adaptation of the Reitboeck method of multiple microelectrode recording to the neocortex of the waking monkey, J. Neurosci. Meth. 36 (1991) 77}84. [10] E. Niebur, C. Koch, A model for the neuronal implementation of selective visual attention based on temporal correlation among neurons, J. Comput. Neurosci. 1 (1) (1994) 141}158. [11] E. Niebur, C. Koch, C. Rosin, An oscillation-based model for the neural basis of attention, Vision Res. 33 (1993) 2789}2802. [12] P.N. Steinmetz, A. Roy, P.J. Fitzgerald, S.S. Hsiao, K.O. Johnson, E. Niebur, Attention modulates synchronized neuronal "ring in primate somatosensory cortex, Nature 404 (2000) 187}190. Arup Roy got a B. Tech. in Instrumentation Engineering from I.I.T Kharagpur (India), and then a M.S. in Biomedical Engineering from Virginia Commonwealth University. Currently, he is a Ph.D. candidate for Biomedical Engineering at the Johns Hopkins University. His interests lie in the "eld of computational neuroscience. Peter N. Steinmetz is a Computational Neuroscientist at Caltech. He is a postdoctoral scholar in the Division of Biology and the Computation and Neural Systems Program. Dr. Steinmetz received his M.D. and Ph.D. in Biomedical Engineering from the Johns Hopkins University. He is interested in the statistics of neural coding and the biophysics of computation. His recent work involves determining how accurately pyramidal cells can compute given their intrinsic noise sources.

1108 A. Roy et al. / Neurocomputing 32}33 (2000) 1103}1108 Ken Johnson got a B.S. in Electrical Engineering in 1961 and then worked in the space program for several years. In 1970 he got a Ph.D. in Biomedical Engineering from the Johns Hopkins University where his thesis research consisted of reconstruction of the neural population activity evoked by a vibratory stimulus on the skin. Since then he has worked on the neural mechanisms of tactile perception. The work has involved neurophysiological, psychophysical, and computational investigations in equal measure. Ernst Niebur was trained as a physicist at the Universities of Dortmund (W. Germany; Diplom Physiker 1982) and Lausanne (Switzerland; Dr. ès sciences 1988). He then joined the Computation and Neural Systems group at Caltech where he was a Research Fellow and Senior Research Fellow. He is presently an Assistant Professor in the Department of Neuroscience and the Krieger Mind/Brain Institute at Johns Hopkins University.