DEVELOPMENT OF ALCOHOL DEPRIVATION EFFECT IN RATS: LACK OF CORRELATION WITH SACCHARIN DRINKING AND LOCOMOTOR ACTIVITY

Similar documents
LOW DOSE OF ETHANOL INDUCES CONDITIONED PLACE PREFERENCE IN RATS AFTER REPEATED EXPOSURES TO ETHANOL OR SALINE INJECTIONS

Effects of Repeated Alcohol Deprivations on Operant Ethanol Self-Administration by Alcohol-Preferring (P) Rats

Independence of neophobia and taste aversion learning

Conditioned taste aversion produced by the oral ingestion of ethanol in the rat

GABAergic Influences Increase Ingestion across All Taste Categories. Liz Miller. Molly McGinnis. Lindsey Richardson

ETHANOL-REINFORCED BEHAVIOUR PREDICTS ACQUISITION BUT NOT EXTINCTION OF COCAINE SELF-ADMINISTRATION IN THE RAT

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Addict Biol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 June 15.

New measurement criteria for studying alcohol drinking and relapse in rodents

Outline of the presentation

Ålder, kön och konsumtionsmönster ur ett prekliniskt perspektiv

Repeated Cycles of Binge-Like Ethanol Drinking in C57BL/6J Mice Augments Subsequent Voluntary Ethanol Intake But Not Other Dependence-Like Phenotypes

Adolescent Binge Drinking Leads to Changes in Alcohol Drinking, Anxiety, and Amygdalar Corticotropin Releasing Factor Cells in Adulthood in Male Rats

Motivation for Alcohol Becomes Resistant to Quinine Adulteration After 3 to 4 Months of Intermittent Alcohol Self-Administration

Taste quality and extinction of a conditioned taste aversion in rats

Sensitization of Salt Appetite Is Associated With Increased Wanting but Not Liking of a Salt Reward in the Sodium-Deplete Rat

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Alcohol Clin Exp Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 April 01.

Fatty acids enhance the perceived taste intensity of non-nutritive sweeteners, saccharin & sucralose, in male and female rats

nutrients ISSN

Escalation of i.v. cocaine self- administration and reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in rats bred for high and low saccharin intake.

Extended therapeutic validation of an anti-mc4 receptor antibody in an animal model of anorexia nervosa

ATTENUATION OF STRESS-INDUCED BEHAVIORAL DEFICITS BY AZADIRACHTA INDICA (NEEM): ROLE OF SEROTONIN

A learned flavor preference persists despite the extinction of conditioned hedonic reactions to the cue flavors

Adolescent Prozac Exposure Enhances Sensitivity to Cocaine in Adulthood INTRODUCTION

Supplementary Fig. 1: TBR2+ cells in different brain regions.

Selective Breeding for Divergence in Novelty-seeking Traits: Heritability and Enrichment in Spontaneous Anxiety-related Behaviors

Ethanol Consumption by Rat Dams During Gestation, Lactation and Weaning Increases Ethanol Consumption by Their Adolescent Young

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques

Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning

Hypothalamic-specific proopiomelanocortin deficiency reduces alcohol drinking in male and female mice

Perceptual Learning in Flavor Aversion: Evidence for Learned Changes in Stimulus Effectiveness

Decremental Carryover Effects of Sucrose Ingestion in the Negative Anticipatory Contrast Procedure in Rats

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure

Key words: apomorphine; dopamine; prepulse inhibition; schizophrenia; startle; strain

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Alcohol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 May 01.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

Deficits in amygdaloid camp-responsive element binding protein signaling play a role in genetic predisposition to anxiety and alcoholism

Flavor avoidance learning based on missing calories but not on palatability reduction

Difference in Blood Alcohol Levels Following Consumption of Whisky and Beer in Man

A RODENT MODEL OF PHENOTYPIC VARIATION IN SWEET PREFERENCE, ADDICTION VULNERABILITY, AND PHARMACOLOGICAL TREATMENT SENSITIVITY

ARE MOTHERS WHO DRINK HEAVILY IN PREGNANCY VICTIMS OF FAS? Maud Rouleau,BSc, Zina Levichek, MD, Gideon Koren, MD, FRCPC

Alcohol dependence induced in rats by semivoluntary intermittent intake

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning

Behavioral Pharmacology

AMIR H. REZVANI 1,2 *, DAVID H. OVERSTREET 2, GEORGE A. MASON 2, DAVID S. JANOWSKY 2, MANI HAMEDI 2, E. CLARK, Jr 2 and YING YANG 2

J.C. Crabbe et al. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2009;65: dark phase, when they are normally engaged in their highest levels of eating, drinking, and act

Ethanol regulated preference in rats

Acquisition of schedule-induced polydipsia by rats in proximity to upcoming food delivery

12µl) was implanted in the jugular vein (S1) under ketamine (100 mg/kg) /xylacine (1 mg/kg)

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

Sugar-Conditioned Flavor Preferences in Normal and Sweet Tasteless Mice

Overshadowing not potentiation of illness-based contextual conditioning by a novel taste

Advantame Sweetener Preference in C57BL/6J Mice and Sprague-Dawley Rats

How to measure rodent behavior and perform a neurological screen.

The effects of environmental enrichment on nicotine sensitization in a rodent model of schizophrenia

EFFECT OF ILLUMINATION CONDITION ON RISK ASSESSMENT BEHAVIORS OF MICE

Enhanced operant self-administration of alcohol in Wistar rats receiving intermittent versus continuous alcohol vapor exposure

Altered Vulnerability to Acute Opiate Withdrawal Following Stress: Roles of N-Methyl-D-Aspartate and Glucocorticoid Receptors

Contribution of the Opioid System to Alcohol Aversion and Alcohol Drinking Behavior 1

SUPPLEMENTAL INFORMATION. Materials Trichostatin A (TSA) and 5-azacytidine (5-AzaC) were purchased from Sigma.

A Behavioral Economic Analysis of Concurrent Ethanol- and Water-Reinforced Responding in Different Preference Conditions

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Pharmacol Biochem Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 September 21.

Running head: EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL ON OBJECT RECOGNITION. Impairing Effects of Alcohol on Object Recognition. A Senior Honors Thesis

Naltrexone reduces ethanol- and sucrose-reinforced responding in rhesus monkeys

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Pharmacol Biochem Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 September 01.

Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change*

MACIEJ GASIOR, 1 MARIA JASZYNA, 2 JAMIE PETERS, 3 and STEVEN R. GOLDBERG

RUNNING HEAD: HPA AXIS ACTIVATION BY ETHANOL DEPENDENCE 1. HPA Axis Activation by Ethanol Dependence in Adult and Adolescent Rats.

Evaluation of Anxiolytic Effect of XXX in Guinea Pig Maternal Separation Test.

Do Rats Consume Ethanol to Regulate a Negative Emotion Induced by a Successive Negative Contrast Procedure?

TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES ABSTRACT

Serotonergic mechanisms of MDMA self-administration. Susan Schenk Victoria University of Wellington School of Psychology

Mouse Taste Preference Tests: Influence of Drinking Spout Position

Is the rodent maternal separation model a valid and effective model for studies on the early-life impact on ethanol consumption?

Jonathan Kindberg BNF Molecular Biology Through Discovery. Research Proposal

Behavioral and Biological Factors Associated With Individual Vulnerability to Psychostimulant Abuse

Reports from the Research Laboratories

LOCOMOTOR ACTIVITY FOLLOWING LATERAL FRONTAL LESIONS IN RHESUS MONKEYS 1

Female Rats show a Bimodal Preference Response to the Artificial Sweetener Sucralose

500 KAWAI AND NAKAJIMA

Rimonabant attenuates sensitization, crosssensitization and cross-reinstatement of place preference induced by nicotine and ethanol

Negative affect words prime beer consumption in young drinkers

Why we can be confident that low-calorie sweeteners are safe?

Using Neuroimaging to Explore Addiction in Animal Models

The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine

Neurobiological Correlates of Individual Differences in Novelty- Seeking Behavior in the Rat: Differential Expression of Stress- Related Molecules

Sex and lineage interact to predict behavioral effects of chronic adolescent stress in rats

and genetic influences on behaviour

Disruption of latent inhibition and perceptual learning

The Neuronal Nitric Oxide Synthase Gene Is Critically Involved in Neurobehavioral Effects of Alcohol

Drug and Alcohol Dependence and Withdrawal Models

Adolescent alcohol use is a serious public health problem and

THE ROLE OF CORTICOTROPIN-RELEASING FACTOR (CRF) SIGNALING IN THE VENTRAL TEGMENTAL AREA IN THE REGULATION OF BINGE-LIKE ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION

ADHD Explanation 5: Medications used in ADHD

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

SP850 OWNER S MANUAL

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

Transcription:

Alcohol & Alcoholism Vol. 34, No. 4, pp. 542 550, 1999 DEVELOPMENT OF ALCOHOL DEPRIVATION EFFECT IN RATS: LACK OF CORRELATION WITH SACCHARIN DRINKING AND LOCOMOTOR ACTIVITY ELIZA KOROS 1, JERZY PIASECKI 1, WOJCIECH KOSTOWSKI 1,2 and PRZEMYSLAW BIENKOWSKI 1 * 1 Department of Pharmacology and Physiology of the Nervous System, Institute of Psychiatry and Neurology, Al. Sobieskiego 1/9, PL-02957 Warsaw and 2 Department of Experimental and Clinical Pharmacology, Warsaw Medical University, ul. Krakowskie Przedmiescie 26/28, PL-00527, Warsaw, Poland (Received 2 September 1998; in revised form 9 December 1998; accepted 15 January 1999) Abstract The present study addressed the relationship between the parameters of saccharin drinking behaviour and locomotor activity in an open field environment and long-term alcohol self-administration. In a 22-day initiation phase, male Wistar rats were presented with increasing concentrations of ethanol (2 8%, v/v) in a choice with water. The rats were then given the choice between water and two ethanol solutions (8 and 16%). Every 28 days, ethanol was withdrawn for 5 days. The ethanol intake and the transient increase in ethanol consumption after each of six deprivation episodes (alcohol deprivation effect) was monitored and correlated with parameters of the subsequent saccharin drinking and open field tests. The total ethanol intake (g/kg/24 h) as well as the consumption of 16% ethanol were stable over time. However, the magnitude of the alcohol deprivation effect increased with the repeated deprivation episodes. None of the parameters measured in the open field or the saccharin drinking tests correlated with either ethanol consumption or the alcohol deprivation effect. These results suggest that (1) repeated episodes of ethanol deprivation may increase the magnitude of the alcohol deprivation effect, (2) neither saccharin drinking nor locomotor activity correlates with long-term ethanol drinking behaviour in rats. INTRODUCTION Both animal and human studies demonstrate marked individual differences in the propensity to develop alcohol (and other drug) taking behaviour (Cloninger et al., 1988; Piazza et al., 1989; Bisaga and Kostowski, 1993). Thus, it would be of particular importance to define biological markers (predictors) of the vulnerability towards alcohol/ drug self-administration. One approach involves searching for individual behavioural features that are correlated with excessive alcohol/drug taking. For example, several authors have reported that individuals with a high preference for sweet substances consume more ethanol and acquire amphetamine self-administration more quickly than those with a low preference for these substances (Piazza et al., 1989; Gosnell and Krahn, 1992; *Author to whom correspondence should be addressed. Kampov-Polevoy et al., 1996). Similarly, it has been shown that rats with higher locomotor response to novelty (high responders, HR) acquire amphetamine self-administration more readily than low responders (LR) (Piazza et al., 1990). In contrast, no consistent relationship between locomotor activity and ethanol intake has been found in genetically selected alcohol-preferring and -non-preferring rats (Badishtov et al., 1995) or in outbred Wistar rats (Bisaga and Kostowski, 1993; Samson and Chapelle, 1995). We have recently shown that parameters of saccharin drinking behaviour were highly correlated with the initial acceptance of low ethanol concentrations in Wistar rats. However, this relationship disappeared during further weeks of ethanol presentation. In contrast, locomotor activity in an open field area did not predict any subsequent ethanol consumption (Koros et al., 1998). These findings suggest that certain behavioural features may predict only initial alcohol drinking behaviour. It could also be speculated that other behavioural 542 1999 Medical Council on Alcoholism

SACCHARIN DRINKING, LOCOMOTOR ACTIVITY, AND THE ADE 543 parameters correlate with ethanol intake in later stages of alcohol self-administration (maintenance phase). Thus, one aim of the present study was to assess possible relationships between long-term ethanol self-administration and several behavioural parameters derived from the open field and the saccharin drinking tests. The alcohol deprivation effect (ADE) was first described by Sinclair and Senter (1968) as a transient increase in ethanol consumption/preference after a period of forced abstinence. In rats, the ADE was reported to occur after 18 or 21, but not 1 or 7, days of access to alcohol (Sinclair and Senter, 1968; Sinclair, 1972). More recently, Spanagel et al. (1996) have described a model of long-term free-choice alcohol self-administration with repeated deprivation episodes. In this latter study, rats showed the ADE after 2 months of access to water and three ethanol solutions (5, 10 and 20%, v/v). In contrast, we did not observe any changes in ethanol drinking after the deprivation episode, which followed a 50-day period of alcohol drinking (Koros et al., 1998). Thus, another aim of the present study was to examine the pattern of ethanol consumption after repeated episodes of deprivation in the rats used by Koros et al. (1998). MATERIALS AND METHODS Subjects Twenty-four male Wistar rats (275 350 g at the beginning of the study) served as subjects. The rats were obtained from a licensed breeder (HZL, Warsaw, Poland) 2 weeks before the start of the investigation. During this time, the animals were housed six to a cage, weighed, and handled several times. The rats were kept under standard laboratory conditions [at 22 ± 1 C, 60% relative humidity, and a 12 h light/12 h dark cycle (lights on at 07:00)]. After 2 weeks of acclimatization, the subjects were transferred to individual wire cages (20 25 28 cm, L W H) with two graduated drinking tubes mounted at the front. During the entire investigation, the subjects had free access to standard lab chow (Bacutil, Poland) and tap water. Ethanol solutions (see below) were prepared from 95% stock ethanol and tap water. All solutions were replaced completely twice a week. Fluid intake was measured daily at 10:00 and body weights were recorded once a week throughout the study. Study design Table 1 presents a general design of the study. One week after the completion of the initial Table 1. Description of experimental procedures Procedure Fluids Availability (days) Initial saccharin drinking and open field test a,b Washout Water 7 Two-bottle choice test 2 8% ethanol vs water a 22 Three-bottle choice test 8% vs. 16% ethanol vs water a 28 Three-bottle choice test 8% vs. 16% ethanol vs water 3 Washout Water 7 Saccharin drinking and open field test b a Parameters already reported and analysed by Koros et al. (1998). b For details, see Materials and methods section.

544 E. KOROS et al. saccharin drinking and the open field tests, the ethanol drinking tests began. For the first 22 days, the animals were exposed to increasing concentrations of ethanol (2 8%, v/v) and tap water in a twobottle choice situation. Then, for the next 28 days, the animals were presented with two different ethanol solutions (8 and 16%, v/v) and tap water (a three-bottle choice test). The drinking tubes were rotated daily to prevent position preference. After 50 days of continuous access to alcohol and water the rats were deprived of alcohol for 5 days. During the deprivation period water was available in all three bottles. After the 5-day deprivation period, both alcohol solutions were presented again along with water for the next 28 days. [The correlations between the initial behavioural tests, ethanol self-administration during the first 50 days and the ADE after the first ethanol deprivation were described in detail by Koros et al. (1998).] After the completion of six three-bottle ethanol drinking/deprivation cycles, saccharin drinking and the open field tests were performed again (Table 1). To assess the ADEs, i.e. changes in the ethanol intake after the deprivation episodes, ethanol drinking was measured for 3 days before and 3 days after each deprivation (Koros et al., 1998). The ADE was separately calculated using the ethanol consumption from the first 24 (ADE 24 h ) or 72 h (ADE 72 h ) after each deprivation. Positive and negative values of the ADE reflected increases or decreases in ethanol consumption after deprivation, respectively. Saccharin drinking test During the 48-h saccharin drinking test, the animals were given the choice between a saccharin solution (0.1% w/v; saccharin sodium salt, Aldrich, Gillingham, UK) and tap water. The position of bottles was changed after 24 h to prevent place preference. For each fluid, an intake measure was obtained, averaged across 2 days of choice and corrected for body weight of the subject (ml/kg/24 h). It has been reported that the increase in total fluid intake (TFI) when saccharin is offered is a better predictor of subsequent ethanol intake than absolute saccharin consumption (Kampov-Polevoy et al., 1995). Thus, the increase in the TFI in the presence of saccharin was calculated as the percentage difference between the TFI when saccharin was available and a control TFI when only water was available (Kampov-Polevoy et al., 1995). The mean water intake (ml/kg/24 h) during the 2 days preceding the saccharin drinking test was treated as the control TFI. Open field test One week after the completion of the saccharin drinking test, the rats were used in the open field test. The open field apparatus consisted of four identical, computer-controlled cages (60 60 40 cm, L W H; COTM, Bialystok, Poland). Each cage was transected by two perpendicular, co-planar arrays of 16 infrared photocells which were intended to measure forward locomotion by determining the rat s position every 0.1 s (Bienkowski et al., 1997a; Koros et al., 1998). The forward locomotion was defined as the distance (in in.) travelled by the rat during the 20-min test session. Another set of photocells located 15 cm above the cage floor measured the number of rearings. After the initial habituation of 20 min to the test room, each rat was introduced to the test cage for another 20 min. The cages were cleaned carefully between the recordings. The test sessions were conducted between 10:00 and 14:00 to avoid errors attributable to the variation in motor activity of the day activity cycle (File and Day, 1972). Statistical analysis Absolute changes (increases or decreases) in ethanol consumption ( g/kg) after repeated deprivation episodes were compared by a one-way ANOVA. In addition, the average ethanol drinking from pre- and post-deprivation days was analysed with a two-way ANOVA (ADE episode). The Newman Keuls test was used for post-hoc comparison. Multiple regression analysis was employed for testing the relationship between behavioural variables and ethanol intake. Due to the risk of a Type I error a probability below 0.01 was considered significant. RESULTS Ethanol drinking behaviour Figure 1 presents the total and the 16% ethanol intake in the six 28-day access periods. One-way ANOVA showed that total ethanol intake remained stable over time [F(5,138) = 2.05, P = 0.11]. Similarly, ANOVA did not reveal any significant

SACCHARIN DRINKING, LOCOMOTOR ACTIVITY, AND THE ADE 545 changes in the 16% ethanol consumption [F(5,138) = 1.51, P = 0.19]. The initial acceptance of low ethanol concentrations (2 6%, v/v; see Koros et al., 1998) did not correlate with ethanol intake in the three-bottle choice situation and the subsequent ADE values. The baseline ethanol consumption did not predict the magnitude of the ADE (data not shown). The only exception was the ADE after the first deprivation episode, which was negatively correlated with the baseline ethanol consumption (Koros et al., 1998). Fig. 1. Voluntary ethanol consumption in the threebottle choice test. Mean (± SEM) consumption in g/kg/24 h in the three-bottle choice test (8% ethanol vs 16% ethanol vs water) (n = 24 rats)., Total ethanol intake in subsequent 28-day periods of ethanol availability,, intake of 16% ethanol in subsequent 28-day periods of ethanol availability. The ADE calculated for changes in total ethanol intake Figures 2A and B show the ADE expressed as the change in the total ethanol intake. One-way ANOVA indicated a significant difference between the subsequent ADEs [ADE 24 h, F(5,23) = 3.52, Fig. 2. Mean (± SEM) increase in total (A,B) or 16% (C,D) ethanol consumption after subsequent periods of ethanol deprivation (alcohol deprivation effect, ADE). The ADE was calculated for the first 24 h (ADE 24 h ; left panel) or 72 h (ADE 72 h ; right panel) of ethanol access after deprivation. The data points marked with different letters differ significantly with P less than 0.01. For other details, see Fig. 1.

546 E. KOROS et al. Table 2. Parameters for saccharin drinking and open field test performed 1 week after completion of ethanol drinking procedures Parameter Mean ± SEM Minimum Maximum Saccharin drinking test Baseline water drinking (ml/kg/day) 63±2 50 84 Saccharin drinking (ml/kg/day) 148±9 62 233 Saccharin preference (%) 95±2 56 100 TFI (%) 127±15 4 274 Open field test Forward locomotion (in.) 1072±130 242 2523 Rearings 19±3 2 52 TFI = Total fluid intake; TFI = increase in TFI in the presence of saccharin. No. of rats tested was 24. Table 3. Correlations between saccharin drinking parameters and mean total ethanol intake (g/kg/day) in subsequent 28-day access periods (1 6) Saccharin drinking TFI Drinking period r F P r F P 1 +0.01 0.03 0.95 +0.14 0.45 0.50 2 0.27 1.74 0.19 0.12 0.33 0.57 3 0.29 2.02 0.16 0.13 0.37 0.54 4 0.28 1.82 0.18 0.13 0.38 0.54 5 0.27 1.76 0.19 0.12 0.36 0.56 6 0.36 3.42 0.07 +0.23 1.33 0.26 Mean total ethanol intake = mean 8% ethanol intake (g/kg/day) + mean 16% ethanol intake (g/kg/day); TFI = increase in total fluid intake in the presence of saccharin. No. of rats tested was 24. P < 0.01; ADE 72 h, F(5,23) = 3.37, P < 0.01; see Figs 2A and B, respectively]. Visual inspection of the data and the post-hoc analysis revealed that both the ADE 24 h and the ADE 72 h values increased with repeated deprivation episodes. The above results were partially confirmed by two-way ANOVA, which revealed a trend towards significant ADE episode interaction for the ADE 72 h [F(5,230) = 2.78; 0.01 < P < 0.05]. The ADE calculated for changes in 16% ethanol intake One-way ANOVA for the ADEs did not reveal any significant differences (F < 1.46, P > 0.2) (ADE 24 h, Fig. 2C; ADE 72 h, Fig. 2D). Two-way ANOVA did not indicate any significant effects. It is clear, however, that the ADE calculated for the total ethanol intake was mainly the result of the increase in the 16% ethanol intake. Correlational analysis The mean (± SEM) weight of the rats after the completion of all ethanol self-administration procedures was 559 ± 16 g. The results of the saccharin drinking and the open field tests are summarized in Table 2. Parameters of saccharin drinking did not correlate with the open field behaviours (r values ranged from 0.18 to +0.10; P > 0.35). As shown in Table 3, total ethanol intake correlated neither with the saccharin drinking (left) nor with the increase in the TFI in the presence of saccharin (right). Similarly, there were no significant correlations between total ethanol intake and the open field parameters (Table 4).

SACCHARIN DRINKING, LOCOMOTOR ACTIVITY, AND THE ADE 547 Table 4. Correlations between open field parameters and mean total ethanol intake (g/kg/day) in subsequent 28-day access periods (1 6) Forward locomotion Rearings Drinking period r F P r F P 1 +0.28 0.15 0.23 +0.04 0.08 0.87 2 +0.09 0.18 0.67 0.03 0.02 0.91 3 +0.14 0.45 0.50 +0.09 0.20 0.65 4 +0.08 0.17 0.68 +0.02 0.01 0.91 5 +0.11 0.29 0.59 +0.03 0.02 0.90 6 +0.13 0.39 0.53 +0.03 0.03 0.88 Mean total ethanol intake = mean 8% ethanol intake (g/kg/day) + mean 16% ethanol intake (g/kg/day). No. of rats tested was 24. Table 5. Correlations between saccharin drinking parameters and the alcohol-deprivation effect (ADE) calculated for the first 24 h of ethanol drinking after subsequent deprivation episodes (1 6) Saccharin drinking TFI ADE 24 h r F P r F P 1 0.21 1.04 0.31 0.08 0.14 0.71 2 +0.16 0.60 0.44 0.08 0.14 0.71 3 0.38 3.93 0.06 0.39 4.07 0.06 4 0.47 6.53 0.02 +0.04 0.05 0.89 5 +0.01 0.01 0.98 0.15 0.48 0.48 6 0.10 0.26 0.61 0.14 0.46 0.50 TFI = increase in total fluid intake in the presence of saccharin. No. of rats tested was 24. Table 6. Correlations between open field parameters and the alcohol-deprivation effect (ADE) calculated for the first 24 h of ethanol drinking after subsequent deprivation episodes (1 6) Forward locomotion Rearings ADE 24 h r F P r F P 1 0.01 0.01 0.98 0.06 0.08 0.77 2 +0.01 0.01 0.99 0.15 0.57 0.45 3 0.05 0.07 0.79 0.02 0.02 0.90 4 0.27 1.79 0.19 0.06 0.10 0.75 5 0.18 0.79 0.38 0.20 0.92 0.34 6 0.13 0.43 0.51 0.10 0.23 0.63 No. of rats tested was 24. As shown in Tables 5 and 6, the ADE 24 h values did not correlate with the parameters derived from the behavioural tests. In line with the above, the ADE 72 h values were correlated neither with saccharin drinking nor with open field test parameters (data not shown). In addition, the analysis of the relationships between the parameters from the initial behavioural tests (reported by Koros et al., 1998) and ethanol drinking in the subsequent access/deprivation cycles did not reveal any significant correlations (data not shown).

548 E. KOROS et al. DISCUSSION One aim of the present experiments was to evaluate the pattern of long-term ethanol selfadministration with repeated deprivation periods. The total ethanol intake in the three-bottle choice situation typically ranged between 3 and 4 g/kg/24 h and remained stable across several months of drinking. Notably, the mean ethanol consumption in the first (4.0 ± 0.41 g/kg/24 h) and the sixth (3.6 ± 0.39 g/kg/24 h) 28-day access did not differ significantly. The consumption of 16% ethanol did not substantially change over time. Similar values for total ethanol intake have been found by Spanagel et al. (1996) in the four-bottle model of long-term ethanol self-administration. Generally, in agreement with previous reports (Sinclair, 1972, 1979), baseline ethanol consumption did not predict the ADE in our rats. The ADE has been repeatedly shown after relatively short ethanol presentations in different rat strains (Sinclair and Senter, 1968; Sinclair, 1972; Sinclair and Tiihonen, 1988; Sinclair and Li, 1989). The initial ADEs in the present study were relatively weak in comparison with the results cited above. Sinclair and Senter (1968), using Long- Evans rats, have shown that 1 and 7 days of ethanol (7%, v/v) access did not result in any significant ADE, whereas that for 21 days did. Similarly, Spanagel et al. (1996), using Wistar rats, have reported a significant ADE after a 3-day deprivation episode which followed 2 months of continuous access to water and three ethanol concentrations. In a more recent study from the same laboratory, Hölter et al. (1998), using the drinkometer system, did not find the ADE in Wistar rats pre-exposed to ethanol for 24 weeks. However, the baseline drinking in this latter study was relatively high (6.79 g/kg/24 h) and a ceiling effect might have occurred. When the ADEs after repeated deprivation episodes were compared, a clear-cut difference between the initial and the last ADE was found. The last ADE 24 h exceeded 1.5 g/kg and was fully comparable with the results reported previously by others (Sinclair, 1979; Spanagel et al., 1996). Based on the above findings, one could hypothesize that the ADE in the present study increased with repeated deprivation episodes. However, at least two other possible explanations should be mentioned. First, the duration of ethanol exposure rather than the deprivation episodes might have been critical for the increase in the ADE magnitude. Second, the possibility exists that aged rats are more prone to increased ethanol drinking after deprivation. This latter hypothesis seems to be less likely, as Sinclair (1972) reported that young and old Wistar rats (aged 3 and > 6 months, respectively) showed a similar ADE after 18 days of access to water and ethanol solution (5%, v/v). Obviously, future studies with additional control groups are needed to clarify the above hypotheses. The second purpose of the present study was to evaluate correlations between long-term ethanol intake and parameters from the open field and saccharin drinking tests. Neither saccharin drinking nor the increase in the TFI in the presence of saccharin correlated with ethanol consumption or the ADE. This finding indicates that saccharin drinking is not a good predictor of ethanol intake in the maintenance phase of ethanol self-administration. Previously, Koros et al. (1998), using the same rats, have shown that the association between saccharin drinking and ethanol intake was limited to initial acceptance of low ethanol concentrations (2 6%, v/v). The correlation between saccharin and ethanol intake was also strongest for initial ethanol drinking in the study of Kampov-Polevoy et al. (1996). In our previous report (Koros et al., 1998), we used parameters of the saccharin drinking test performed before the start of the ethanol presentation (not shown here). In the present paper, we used these parameters again but, as expected, the results of the correlational analysis were negative. Thus, the parameters of the saccharin drinking test performed either before or after the period of ethanol presentation did not predict ethanol selfadministration behaviour. The obvious limitation of the present study is that only one strain of rats (Wistar) was used. It is possible that this rat strain does not possess the genetic diversity connecting long-term ethanol drinking and saccharin intake. As mentioned above, robust correlations between initial ethanol acceptance and saccharin intake have been found in the first part of the study (Koros et al., 1998). This finding was in agreement with the results of other short-term ethanol drinking experiments, including those done on genetically selected rats. Thus, it remains to be established if saccharin drinking might also predict long-term ethanol drinking behaviour in other rat strains.

SACCHARIN DRINKING, LOCOMOTOR ACTIVITY, AND THE ADE 549 In a recent clinical study, Kampov-Polevoy et al. (1997) have shown that a majority (80%) of their alcoholic patients were sweet likers, i.e. preferring high sucrose concentrations. In contrast, only 41% of the control group of non-alcoholic subjects preferred highly-concentrated sucrose solutions. Thus, it should be noted that our paradigm differs substantially from the clinical study mentioned above, as we have used only one concentration of saccharin. Interestingly, Sinclair et al. (1992) have reported that alcohol-preferring lines of rats accepted much higher sweet concentrations than their alcohol-non-preferring partners. The concentration of saccharin used by us (0.1%, w/v) is much preferred by naive rats (Kampov-Polevoy et al., 1996; Bienkowski et al., 1997b). However, the study of Kampov-Polevoy et al. (1997) suggests that, in future preclinical research, even higher concentrations of saccharin should be used. Possibly, the positive association between saccharin and ethanol consumption exists only when drinking relatively high (possibly even aversive for some rats) concentrations of the sweetener is considered. In line with many previous papers (Bisaga and Kostowski, 1993; Badishtov et al., 1995; Fahlke et al., 1995; Samson and Chappelle, 1995; Nadal et al., 1996), spontaneous locomotor activity during a single exposure to a novel environment failed to predict the ethanol intake. Interestingly, Gingras and Cools (1995) have even shown that their HR rats drank significantly less alcohol than LR rats. Moreover, no major differences in locomotor stimulant effects of dexamphetamine in the HR and the LR rats have been reported by the same authors (Gingras and Cools, 1997). It is noteworthy that, in the study of Goeders and Guerin (1996), no association between the locomotor response to a novel environment and self-administration of a 0.125 mg dose of cocaine has been found. In conclusion, the results of the present study indicate that neither saccharin consumption nor locomotor activity predicts long-term ethanol selfadministration behaviour in Wistar rats. In addition, our findings suggest that the magnitude of the ADE may increase with repeated deprivation episodes. Acknowledgements This study was supported in part by the Institute of Psychiatry and Neurology (grant 12/97 and 53/98) and PARPA (grant 1/98). REFERENCES Badishtov, B. A., Overstreet, D. H., Kashevskaya, O. P., Viglinskaya, I. V., Kampov-Polevoy, A., Seredenin, S. B. and Halikas, J. A. (1995) To drink or not to drink: open field behavior in alcohol-preferring and -nonpreferring rat strains. Physiology and Behavior 57, 585 589. Bienkowski, P., Koros, E., Piasecki, J. and Kostowski, W. (1997a) Interactions of ethanol with nicotine, dizocilpine, CGP 40116, and 1-(m-chlorophenyl)-biguanide in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 58, 1159 1165. Bienkowski, P., Kuca, P., Piasecki, J. and Kostowski, W. (1997b) 5-HT 3 receptor antagonist, tropisetron, does not influence ethanol-induced conditioned taste aversion and conditioned place aversion. Alcohol 14, 63 69. Bisaga, A. and Kostowski, W. (1993) Individual behavioral differences and ethanol consumption in Wistar rats. Physiology and Behavior 54, 1125 1131. Cloninger, C. R., Sigvardsson, S. and Bohman, M. (1988) Childhood personality predicts alcohol abuse in young adults. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 12, 494 505. Fahlke, C., Hard, E., Eriksson, C. J. P., Engel, J. A. and Hansen, S. (1995) Amphetamine-induced hyperactivity: differences between rats with high or low preference for ethanol. Alcohol 12, 363 367. File, S. E. and Day, S. (1972) Effects of time of day and food deprivation on exploratory activity in the rat. Animal Behaviour 20, 758 762. Gingras, M. A. and Cools, A. R. (1995) Differential ethanol intake in high and low responders to novelty. Behavioural Pharmacology 6, 718 723. Gingras, M. A. and Cools, A. R. (1997) No major differences in locomotor responses to dexamphetamine in High and Low Responders to novelty: a study in Wistar rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 4, 857 862. Goeders, N. E. and Guerin, G. F. (1996) Role of corticosterone in intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats. Neuroendocrinology 64, 337 348. Gosnell, B. A. and Krahn, D. D. (1992) The relationship between saccharin and alcohol intake in rats. Alcohol 9, 201 206. Hölter, S., Engelmann, M., Kirschke, C., Liebsch, G., Landgraf, R. and Spanagel, R. (1998) Long-term ethanol self-administration with repeated ethanol deprivation episodes changes ethanol drinking pattern and increases anxiety-related behaviour in rats. Behavioural Pharmacology 9, 41 48. Kampov-Polevoy, A. B., Overstreet, D. H., Rezvani, A. H. and Janovsky, D. S. (1995) Saccharin-induced increase in daily fluid intake as a predictor of voluntary alcohol intake in alcohol-preferring rats. Physiology and Behavior 57, 791 795. Kampov-Polevoy, A. B., Kashevskaya, O. P., Overstreet, D. H., Rezvani, A. H., Viglinskaya, I. V., Badistov, B. A., Seredenin, S. B., Kalikas, J. A. and Sinclair, J. D. (1996) Pain sensitivity and saccharin intake in alcoholpreferring and alcohol-nonpreferring rat strains. Physiology and Behavior 59, 683 688.

550 E. KOROS et al. Kampov-Polevoy, A. B., Garbutt, J. C. and Janovsky, D. S. (1997) Evidence of preference for a high-concentration sucrose solution in alcoholic men. American Journal of Psychiatry 154, 269 270. Koros, E., Piasecki, J., Kostowski, W. and Bienkowski, P. (1998) Saccharin drinking rather than open field behaviour predicts initial ethanol acceptance in Wistar rats. Alcohol and Alcoholism 33, 131 140. Nadal, R. A., Pallares, M. A. and Ferre, N. S. (1996) Oral intake of sweetened or un-sweetened alcoholic beverages and open field behavior. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 54, 739 743. Piazza, P. V., Deminiere, J. M., Le Moal, M. and Simon, H. (1989) Factors that predict individual vulnerability to amphetamine self-administration. Science 29, 1511 1513. Piazza, P. V., Deminiere, J. M., Maccari, S., Mormede, P., Le Moal, M. and Simon, H. (1990) Individual reactivity to novelty predicts probability of amphetamine selfadministration. Behavioural Pharmacology 1, 339 345. Samson, H. H. and Chappelle, A. M. (1995) Home-cage ethanol consumption and motor activity: Lack of relation to either initial activity or amphetamine-induced locomotion. Alcohol 12, 37 42. Sinclair, J. D. (1972) The alcohol-deprivation effect. Influence of various factors. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol 33, 769 782. Sinclair, J. D. (1979) Alcohol-deprivation effect in rats genetically selected for their ethanol preference. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 10, 597 602. Sinclair, J. D. and Li, T.-K. (1989) Long and short alcohol deprivation effects on AA and P alcohol-preferring rats. Alcohol 6, 505 509. Sinclair, J. D. and Senter, R. J. (1968) Development of an alcohol-deprivation effect in rats. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol 29, 863 867. Sinclair, J. D. and Tiihonen (1988) Lack of alcoholdeprivation effect in AA rats. Alcohol 5, 85 87. Sinclair, J. D., Kampov-Polevoy, A., Stewart, E. and Li, T.-K. (1992) Taste preferences in rat lines selected for high and low ethanol consumption. Alcohol 9, 155 160. Spanagel, R., Hölter, S., Allingham, K., Langraf, R. and Zieglgansberger, W. (1996) Acamprosate and alcohol: 1. Effects on alcohol intake following alcohol deprivation in the rat. European Journal of Pharmacology 305, 39 44.