Reading Based IQ Estimates and Actual Premorbid Cognitive Performance: Discrepancies in a College Athlete Sample

Similar documents
The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of the Liberal Arts

Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition

Neuropsychological test performance of Hawaii high school athletes: Hawaii ImPACT normative data

Version of record first published: 25 Apr 2012.

Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples

Neurocognitive Assessment of Sports-Related Concussion

Abstract. To assess the effects of two sports-related concussions on neuropsychological functioning

NEUROCOGNITIVE ASSESSMENT OF SPORTS-RELATED CONCUSSION

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of the Liberal Arts PREDICTORS OF POST-CONCUSSION SYMPTOMS IN COLLEGIATE ATHLETES

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. Department of Psychology

Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised*

Concussion and neuropsychological testing (literature search) Dr Ryan Kohler December 2011

One-Month Test Retest Reliability of the ImPACT Test Battery

Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores

THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY

Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan

Serial 7s and Alphabet Backwards as Brief Measures of Information Processing Speed

Plenary Session 2 Psychometric Assessment. Ralph H B Benedict, PhD, ABPP-CN Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry SUNY Buffalo

Comparison of Predicted-difference, Simple-difference, and Premorbid-estimation methodologies for evaluating IQ and memory score discrepancies

Baseline Concussion Testing: The Effects of Learning Disabilities and Sleep

Minimizing Misdiagnosis: Psychometric Criteria for Possible or Probable Memory Impairment

Two-year Test Retest Reliability of ImPACT in High School Athletes

An Initial Validation of Virtual Human Administered Neuropsychological Assessments

Detecting neurocognitive impairment in HIV-infected youth: Are we focusing on the wrong factors?

(Received 30 March 1990)

Surgical Neurology International

Baseline Neurocognitive Performance and Clearance for Athletes to Return to Contact

Concussion Facts & Stats

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Performance in Above Average and Superior School Children: Relationship to Intelligence and Age

An empirical analysis of the BASC Frontal Lobe/Executive Control scale with a clinical sample

Lambros Messinis PhD. Neuropsychology Section, Department of Neurology, University of Patras Medical School

Robust Cognitive Change

The relation between post concussion symptoms and neurocognitive performance in concussed athletes

Concussion: Pressure to Play in Sports: Can We Trust What the Athlete Tells Us? How I Manage Concussion: A Neuropsychologist s Perspective

Examination of the Construct Validity of ImPACT Computerized Test, Traditional, and Experimental Neuropsychological Measures

Sports Neuropsychology: Definition, Qualifications, and Training Guidelines

Rapid Mass Neuropsychological Testing

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of the Liberal Arts THE IMPACT OF CONCUSSION ON PROCESSING SPEED AND INDIVIDUAL

10/5/2015. Advances in Pediatric Neuropsychology Test Interpretation Part I: Importance of Considering Normal Variability. Financial Disclosures

Summary of evidence-based guideline update: Evaluation and management of concussion in sports

The Construct Validity of Memory Span as a Measure of Intelligence

A review of the validity of computerized neurocognitive assessment tools in mild traumatic brain injury assessment

Test Retest Reliability of Computerized Neurocognitive Testing in Youth Ice Hockey Players

The Impact of Mild Traumatic Brain Injury on Cognitive Functioning Following Co-occurring Spinal Cord Injury

International Forum on HIV and Rehabilitation Research

PPMI Cognitive-Behavioral Working Group. Daniel Weintraub, MD

Neuropsychological Performance in Cannabis Users and Non-Users Following Motivation Manipulation

Psychometric Properties and Reference Values for the ImPACT Neurocognitive Test Battery in a Sample of Elite Youth Ice Hockey Players

PEDIATRIC SPORTS RELATED CONCUSSIONS

Clinical Policy Title: Immediate post-concussion assessment and cognitive testing (ImPACT)

Pre and Post Concussion Management

New Study Confirms the Clinical Usefulness of the ImPACT Baseline ImPACT Testing Model VOL. 2 - FALL 2014

INTRODUCTION. Abstract

A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE DIVISION OF THE UNIVERSITY OF HAWAI I AT MĀNOA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF

APPENDIX A TASK DEVELOPMENT AND NORMATIVE DATA

The Need for Baseline Concussion Testing. Brandie Messer, DNP, RN DNP Coordinator Assistant Professor Saint Anthony College of Nursing

ImPACT Concussion Management Program

Clinical Policy Title: Immediate post-concussion assessment and cognitive testing (ImPACT)

New Developments in the Management of Concussions. David Marshall, MD Medical Director Sports Medicine Program Children s Healthcare of Atlanta

Optimizing Concussion Recovery: The Role of Education and Expectancy Effects

Cross-Validation of Measures Used for Computer-Based Assessment of Concussion

Are people with Intellectual disabilities getting more or less intelligent II: US data. Simon Whitaker

Multiple prior concussions are associated with symptoms in high school athletes

Clinical Utility of Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised and Predicted IQ Discrepancies in Closed Head Injury

Psychometric and Measurement Properties of Concussion Assessment Tools in Youth Sports

Case reports. Computerised cognitive assessment of concussed Australian Rules footballers

Role of Neuropsychology Concussion Management. Aimee Custer, PsyD Clinical Sports Neuropsychologist

A Review of ImPACT Testing Used for the Diagnosis of Concussion and the Influence of Language of Test Administration

Concussion: The Basics. Bill Meehan, MD Micheli Center for Sports Injury Prevention Sports Concussion Clinic, Boston Children s Hospital

Criterion validity of the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury

THE CHALLENGES OF CHANGE

Concussion Baseline Testing: Preexisting Factors, Symptoms, and Neurocognitive Performance

The Association Between Comorbidities and Neurocognitive Impairment in Aging Veterans with HIV

The Role of Cardiovascular Risk and Aging in Memory Performance in a Sample of Veterans with HIV

Current Concepts in Sports Concussion: Opportunities for the Physical Therapist. Concussion in Sport

Forney ISD Protocol and Procedures for the Management of the Sports-Related Concussion

Clinical Practice of a Vision Based Concussion Tool MATT FRANTZ, M.ED., LAT, ATC LAFAYETTE COLLEGE

Using contextual analysis to investigate the nature of spatial memory

The Short NART: Cross-validation, relationship to IQ and some practical considerations

IT S ALL IN YOUR HEAD!

Validity of Immediate Post-Concussion Assessment and Cognitive Testing (ImPACT)

Sandbagging Baseline Test Performance on ImPACT, Without Detection, Is More Difficult than It Appears

Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)


Neuropsychological Assessment of Sport- Related Concussion

Concussions in Soccer: Assessment and Management. Ruben J. Echemendía, Ph.D.

ImPACT Normative Data of Ethnically Diverse Adolescent Athletes. William T. Tsushima, Ph.D. Straub Medical Center Honolulu, Hawaii

Concussion in Adventure Athletes Epidemiology and Current Guidelines. Aaron Provance, MD Medical Director

Head Injury Testing & Management Protocol Boston University Athletic Training Services

Relationship Between Post-Concussion Headache and Neuropsychological Test Performance in High School Athletes

Baseline Concussion Testing: A Comparison between Collision, Contact, and Non-Contact Sports

Measurement Issues in Concussion Testing

Scoring Sway Balance. Scoring System and BESS Comparison. October 2 nd, Copyright 2013 Sway Medical LLC

Finnish practices: Cognitive assessment and interventions

I n the United States alone, an estimated sports

Impairments in cognitive abilities are among the. Promising New Approaches to Assess Cognitive Functioning in People with Multiple Sclerosis

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT S A R A H R A S K I N, P H D, A B P P S A R A H B U L L A R D, P H D, A B P P

Diagnostic Tools. Objectives UPDATES AND ADVANCES IN THE DIAGNOSIS AND MANAGEMENT OF SPORTS RELATED CONCUSSION

Virtual Reality Cognitive Performance Assessment Test

Transcription:

Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society (2012), 18, 139 143. Copyright E INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2011. doi:10.1017/s1355617711001275 BRIEF COMMUNICATION Reading Based IQ Estimates and Actual Premorbid Cognitive Performance: Discrepancies in a College Athlete Sample Amanda R. Rabinowitz, 1 AND Peter A. Arnett 1 1 Psychology Department, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania (RECEIVED October 11, 2010; FINAL REVISION August 21, 2011; ACCEPTED August 22, 2011) Abstract The present study sought to evaluate the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) Full-Scale IQ (FSIQ) estimate as an index of premorbid ability in a sample 574 of healthy college athletes participating in a sports concussion management program. We compared baseline neuropsychological test performance with the WTAR FSIQ estimate obtained at baseline. Results revealed that the discrepancy between actual neuropsychological test scores and the WTAR FSIQ estimate was greatest for those with estimated FSIQs greater than 107. The clinical implication of this finding was evaluated in the 51 participants who went on to sustain a concussion. For individuals with higher IQ estimates, the WTAR estimate obtained post-concussion suggested greater post-concussion decline than that indicated by comparison with actual baseline neuropsychological performance. (JINS, 2012, 18, 139 143) Keywords: Intelligence, Cerebral concussion, Neuropsychological tests, Traumatic brain injury, Sports injuries, Intelligence tests INTRODUCTION Comparing neuropsychological test performance with a readingbased IQ-estimate is one method that neuropsychologists use to infer impairments in cognitive functioning. This approach has been supported by research demonstrating that estimates of intellectual functioning do indeed predict neuropsychological test performance across a range of ability levels (Diaz-Asper, Schretlen, & Pearlson, 2004). However, merely demonstrating a relationship between premorbid IQ estimates and cognitive abilities (e.g., attention, memory, etc.) does not directly speak to the accuracy of these instruments for predicting performance in these other neurocognitive domains. For example, Schretlen, Buffington, Meyer, and Pearlson (2005) demonstrated that, although correlations between National Adult Reading Test- Revised (NART-R) and concurrent Verbal and Full-Scale IQ (FSIQ) were robust, the NART-R s correlations with concurrent functioning in other cognitive domains were significantly weaker (Schretlen et al., 2005). Correspondence and reprint requests to: Amanda R. Rabinowitz, Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, 422 Moore Building, University Park, PA 16802-3106. E-mail: arr200@psu.edu Discrepancies between predicted and actual performance could result in classifying individuals as impaired, when they are not in-fact experiencing cognitive decline, or vice versa. In fact, a study of base rate data for IQ-Memory Score discrepancies across IQ-strata demonstrated that memory performance was likely to exceed FSIQ in those with FSIQs below average, whereas the opposite was true in those with FSIQs in the High Average or Superior range. The authors suggest that clinicians who are unaware of this significant interaction between IQ and IQ-Memory Score discrepancy may be at risk of interpreting a normative performance pattern as indicative of memory decline (Hawkins & Tulsky, 2001). The present study sought to replicate and extend these findings, by evaluating the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) as an index of premorbid ability in healthy college athletes participating in a sports-concussion management program. Participating athletes complete a comprehensive neuropsychological battery, including the WTAR, as a baseline measurement of their cognitive functioning. This provides the opportunity to compare the WTAR estimate with actual premorbid cognitive performance across a variety of domains and specifically to quantify any possible discrepancy 139

140 A.R. Rabinowitz and P.A. Arnett between actual and predicted performance. The present study sought to examine IQ-performance discrepancies across a range of cognitive domains, and to compare these discrepancies across ability levels. To evaluate the clinical implications of IQ-performance discrepancies, post-concussion test results were compared with baseline neuropsychological performance and a post-injury WTAR FSIQ estimate. METHOD Participants The sample consisted of 574 college athletes (430 males and 144 females) participating in a concussion management program at a large state university. Athletes represented eight varsity athletic programs: football (31.4%), men s soccer (11.3%), women s soccer (10.6%), men s wrestling (2.1%), women s lacrosse (9.4%), men s lacrosse (14.3%), women s basketball (4.9%), and men s basketball (6.3%); and one nonvarsity sports program, that is, men s ice hockey (9.4%). The average age of players at baseline was 18.5. The majority of the sample identified as Caucasian American (75%), 18% identified as African American, and 1% identified as Asian American. All participants were native English speakers. Thirty-eight percent of participants reported a history of concussion before study-inclusion. Fifty-one of these athletes (40 males and 11 females) went on to sustain a concussion during their study participation, and were tested then as well. Procedure Participants were administered a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests at baseline, before the start of their participation in team activities. Tests were administered by graduate students and undergraduate research assistants, under the supervision of a licensed psychologist and clinical neuropsychologist (P.A.). Athletes who went on to sustain a concussion were tested approximately 48 hr post-injury, using alternative forms of the test-battery. Concussions were identified at the time of injury by athletic trainers or coaching staff, and athletes were subsequently referred by one of the team physicians for testing. All injuries included in the present study met criteria for at least grade II concussions according to the American Academy of Neurology (AAN) Guidelines. Data were obtained in compliance with the standards of the university s Institutional Review Board. Measures The test battery consisted of several measures that assess cognitive functioning, including: the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R; Benedict, Schretlen, Groninger, & Brandt, 1998), the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R; Benedict, 1997), the Symbol-Digit Modalities Test (SDMT; Smith, 1982), the Digit Span Test (Wechsler, 1997), the PSU Cancellation Task, and the Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT; Trenerry, Crosson, DeBoe, & Leber, 1989), and the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR; The Psychological Corporation, 2001). For athletes who underwent multiple evaluations, alternate forms of the HVLT-R, BVMT-R, SDMT, and PSU Cancellation Task were used (see Benedict et al., 1998; Benedict, 1997; Smith, 1982, for alternate form reliability information). With the exception of the WTAR, tests have demonstrated sensitivity to traumatic brain injury (Bailey, Echemendia, & Arnett, 2005; Bohnen, Twijnstra, & Jolles, 1992; Bruce & Echemendia, 2003; Ponsford & Kinsella, 1992). The Immediate Post-Concussion Assessment and Cognitive Testing computerized battery (ImPACT; Lovell, Collins, Podell, Powell, & Maroon, 2000) was administered along with paper-and-pencil tests at both time-points. Although complete validity data for the use of this test battery in sports-related concussion are yet unpublished, it assesses cognitive domains typically affected following such injury. The ImPACT (Lovell et al., 2000) is a computerized test battery that was designed as a time-effective standardized method for collecting data to assist in concussion assessment and management. Six tests are included, designed to target attention, memory, processing speed, and reaction time. From these tests, five composite scores are derived: Verbal Memory, Visual Memory, Visuomotor Speed, Reaction Time, and Impulse Control. Studies in high school and college athletes have demonstrated that ImPACT performance is correlated with performance on similar paper-and-pencil neuropsychological tests (Iverson, Lovell, & Collins, 2005), and is sensitive to the acute effects of concussion (Schatz, Pardini, Lovell, Collins, & Podell, 2006). The WTAR is a test of reading recognition that was designed for premorbid IQ estimation. The WTAR was developed and co-normed with the WAIS-III in both the US and the United Kingdom using the same large, representative sample of normally functioning adults. Using the normative data from the co-norming sample, WTAR scores can be converted to Full Scale IQ (FSIQ) estimates. The WTAR has strong correlations (.70.80) with WAIS-III Full Scale IQ scores for a wide age range of WTAR scores, and WTAR performance is relatively resistant to the effects of traumatic brain injury (The Psychological Corporation, 2001). WTAR scores are highly correlated with other accepted premorbid measures the American National Adult Reading Test (.90), National Adult Reading Test (.78), and the Wide Range Achievement Test Revised Reading Test (.73). Test retest reliability for the instrument is.92 for the 16 29 age group (The Psychological Corporation, 2001). Approach to Data Analysis To assess the discrepancy between WTAR FSIQ estimates and actual premorbid cognitive performance, all neuropsychological test indices were transformed into standard score (SS) units, using the athletes at baseline as a reference hence, putting all test indices on the same metric as the FSIQ score. Each SS-transformed test index was subtracted from the WTAR FSIQ estimate to create an IQ-performance discrepancy score. Positive discrepancy scores indicate that the WTAR FSIQ score was higher than actual performance on the neuropsychological

fmri in pediatric MTBI 141 indices (i.e., the WTAR FSIQ estimate over-estimates actual performance); negative scores indicate that the FSIQ score was lower than these indices (i.e., the WTAR FSIQ estimate under-estimates actual performance). These discrepancy scores were used in subsequent analyses. To examine the interaction between WTAR derived FSIQ and IQ-performance discrepancy, athletes were divided into high, mid-range, and low IQ groups, based on the first and third quartiles of the distribution of WTAR FSIQ estimates in the baseline sample. For the purpose of the post-concussion analysis, an FSIQ estimate derived from the post-injury WTAR was used. As with baseline test-scores, all neuropsychological test indices were transformed into SS units using athletes at baseline as a reference. Post-concussion scores were each subtracted from the WTAR FSIQ estimate to create difference scores, representing post-concussion cognitive decline on a given task, as determined using the FSIQ estimate as the comparison standard. These scores will be referred to subsequently as WTAR-decline scores. The intra-individual mean of WTAR-decline scores across tasks was calculated to create a mean WTAR-decline score for each individual. To create baseline-decline scores, post-concussion SS-transformed scores were subtracted from the baseline SS-transformed score for each test index. The intra-individual mean of baseline-decline scores across tasks was calculated to create a mean baseline-decline score for each individual. The number of scores to decline 15 SS points or greater was calculated to create indices of the total number of scores for which the athlete exhibited clinically significant decline; separate indices were calculated using WTAR- and baseline-decline scores. RESULTS Baseline Analysis The reliability of IQ-performance discrepancies scores for individual test-indices ranged from.45 to.88; reliability coefficients were higher than.60 for 11 of 14 test-indices. Athletes were divided into high, mid-range, and low groups, based on the distribution of WTAR FSIQ estimates in the baseline sample 162 athletes had FSIQ scores that fell in the low quartile group (range 5 81 100; M 5 96), 149 had estimates falling in the high quartile group (range 5 107 117; M 5 109.5), and 217 had estimates in the mid-range (range 5 100 107; M 5 104). Multivariate analysis of variance (MAN- OVA) revealed a significant multivariate effect of IQ group on IQ-performance discrepancies for individual neuropsychological test indices (Pillai s Trace F 5 5.12; p,.001). Statistical significance of univariate tests was established using the Hochberg method correcting for multiple comparisons (Hochberg & Rom, 1995). Univariate results also supported a significant effect of IQ group on discrepancy scores; tests were significant for 11 of 14 neuropsychological test indices. These 11 significant effects all exhibited the same pattern: the WTAR FSIQ score was higher (over-estimation) than actual neuropsychological test performance for members of the high IQ group and, to a lesser extent, the mid-range IQ group (See Table 1). Post-concussion Analysis To evaluate the stability of the WTAR in response to injury, test retest reliability and reliable change indices for the WTAR FSIQ estimate in the present sample were consulted (Jacobson & Truax, 1991). The correlation between baseline and post-concussion WTAR FSIQ estimate suggested good reliability (R xx 5.87), and only one of 51 participants exhibited any reliable change (a decline, using the 95% confidence interval). To evaluate the effect of comparison standard (post-injury WTAR vs. baseline neuropsychological test performance), a repeated measures general linear model (GLM) was constructed using method (WTAR-decline vs. baseline-decline) as a within subjects factor, and the post-injury Table 1. Discrepancy between baseline test performance and WTAR FSIQ index across IQ groups Test index Low IQ Mid-range IQ High IQ F p value Adj. a ImPACT Verbal Memory 21.15 4.10 5.82 8.9.000.006* ImPACT Visual Memory 0.51 2.40 6.70 6.7.001.008* ImPACT Visual Motor Speed 1.74 3.35 5.02 1.9.155.050 ImPACT Impulse Control 21.49 2.88 8.42 15.7.000.006* ImPACT Reaction Time 20.61 3.90 6.63 8.7.000.005* BVMT Total Recall 0.36 2.88 6.68 7.1.001.007* BVMT Delayed Recall 20.67 3.03 7.40 11.0.000.005* HVLT Total Recall 0.58 3.29 6.10 5.4.005.013* HVLT Delayed Recall 20.73 3.80 6.39 8.6.000.004* SDMT 0.53 2.82 4.85 3.2.044.025 Digit Span 2.26 1.42 5.24 3.3.037.017 Stroop-W 21.33 4.01 6.10 11.9.000.004* Stroop-CW 0.41 3.09 6.10 5.4.005.010* PSU Cancellation Task 23.84 3.66 8.83 25.2.000.004* Note. *The p value is significant according to the Hochberg & Rom (1995) method correcting for multiple comparisons. All scores are in standard score units. The Low IQ group is comprised of participants with WTAR FSIQ estimates,100. The Mid-Range IQ group contains participants with WTAR FSIQ estimates between 100 and 107. The High IQ group contains individuals with estimates.107. Positive values indicate overestimation of actual neuropsychological test performance by WTAR FSIQ, whereas negative values indicate underestimation.

142 A.R. Rabinowitz and P.A. Arnett -10 3.5-8 3 WTAR-mean decline -6 2.5 WTAR-decline scores -4-2 0 High IQ Low IQ 2 1.5 1 BL-mean decline BL-decline scores 2 0.5 4 0 Fig. 1. Repeated measures GLM results for method by WTAR FSIQ interaction on suggested post-concussion decline, operationalized as both mean-decline, and the number of declined scores. Method is a two-level within subjects factor, WTAR-decline versus Baseline-decline. WTAR FSIQ score is a continuous random factor. To illustrate the nature of the interaction, participants were divided into High IQ (those participants with FSIQ estimates falling above the 3 rd quartile) and Low IQ (those participants with FSIQ estimates falling below the 1 st quartile) groups, based on the distribution of WTAR FSIQ scores in athletes assessed post-concussion. Bars represent mean post-concussion decline in SS points (axis on left). The dark gray bar represents mean decline using WTAR FSIQ score as a comparison standard; the light gray bar represents mean decline using baseline test-performance as a comparison standard. Lines represent the mean number of scores to decline by 15 SS points or greater (axis on right). The dark gray line represents the mean number of declined scores, using the WTAR FSIQ score as a comparison standard; the light gray line represents the mean number of declined scores, using baseline test-performance as a comparison standard. WTAR FSIQ estimate as a covariate. GLM results revealed a significant main effect of method on the magnitude of suggested post-concussion decline (F 5 6.7; p,.05), and a significant method by FSIQ estimate interaction (F 5 8.2; p,.01). To test the possible clinical significance of this finding, a separate repeated measures GLM was constructed evaluating the total number of post-concussion scores to decline by 15 SS points or more. Results revealed a significant main effect of method (F 5 4.1; p,.05), and a significant method by FSIQ estimate interaction (F 5 4.8; p,.05). There was no main effect of FSIQ estimate on post-concussion decline (F 5 1.3; p 5.26), or the total number of declined scores post concussion (F 5.01; p 5.92). The nature of these significant interactions is illustrated in Figure 1 for those with higher FSIQ estimates, the WTAR comparison standard suggested greater post-concussion decline, and a greater number of declined scores than when baseline test results were used as the comparison standard. DISCUSSION The primary purpose of the present study was to evaluate the validity of a reading based premorbid IQ estimator as an indicator of premorbid cognitive performance across ability levels. Athletes performance on a neuropsychological test battery at baseline was compared with their predicted performance based on the WTAR FSIQ estimate. Results suggest that, although the WTAR FSIQ estimate may be a reasonable estimate of premorbid performance for college athletes with FSIQ estimates below 100, in individuals with FSIQ estimates above 107, WTAR estimates were significantly higher than athletes performance on neuropsychological tests (i.e., the FSIQ estimate over-estimated actual performance) by an average of 6.3 SS points. This result is consistent with the findings reported by Hawkins and Tulsky (2001), who reported that significant IQ-Memory Score discrepancies were normative in individuals with High Average and Superior FSIQ scores. To evaluate the possible clinical implications of these discrepancies, post-concussion decline from post-injury WTAR FSIQ was compared with decline from actual baseline test-performance. Results suggest that the choice of comparison standard baseline performance or WTAR FSIQ estimate did not make a difference for individuals with lower IQ estimates. However, for individuals with higher IQ estimates, choice of comparison standard had a significant impact on the magnitude of suggested concussion-related cognitive decline, with the WTAR FSIQ estimate suggesting greater post-concussion decline than that indicated by comparison with actual baseline performance. These findings suggest that choice of comparison standard the WTAR FSIQ estimate, or actual premorbid performance could influence clinical decision making for individuals with higher FSIQ estimates. It is possible that this effect is in part due to the influence of practice effects all of the athletes that were assessed post-concussion had previous exposure to alternate forms of the same tests at baseline. However, the results of the baseline analyses suggest that the WTAR FSIQ estimate overestimated premorbid ability in these individuals, which likely contributed to the discrepancy between WTAR- and baseline-decline scores. There are limitations of the present study that bear noting. The dependent variables used in the current study are difference-scores, and there are psychometric limitations of such scores that may threaten the validity of these variables. Specifically, the difference between two variables will be less

fmri in pediatric MTBI 143 reliable than either of its component scores, particularly when component scores are correlated with one another (Williams & Zimmerman, 1977). As reported, reliability of IQ-performance discrepancy scores was reasonably high, and so this issue may not have been problematic in the present study. Additionally, the present sample is not representative of large normative samples, or many patients who seek neuropsychological assessment. Healthy college athletes represent a restricted range of intellectual functioning (WTAR FSIQ M 5 103; SD 5 6). All participants in the present study had IQ estimates broadly in the average range (between Low and High Average according to WAIS descriptors), as would mostly be expected in a group of healthy college students. Hence, the extent to which these findings may generalize to other populations remains an empirical question. Lastly, it is important to note that the present findings do not address the validity of using an actual measure of premorbid IQ (i.e., the WAIS-IV) as a comparison standard. Despite these limitations, these findings are consistent with other work (Dodrill, 1997; Hawkins & Tulsky, 2001; Schretlen et al., 2005) suggesting that a premorbid IQ estimate may be a misleading benchmark for determining whether a given neuropsychological test score indicates impaired functioning. Neuropsychologists should estimate premorbid IQ in their patients with good reason this information provides a context for test-interpretation. However, the present results suggest that premorbid estimates should be used as a comparison standard with caution, particularly for individuals with higher IQ estimates. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS These data, in part, were presented at the 30th annual meeting of the National Academy of Neuropsychology, Vancouver, BC. No financial or other relationships exist that could be interpreted as a conflict of interest affecting this manuscript. The authors thank the following individuals for their help with data coding and/or running participants in the study: Fiona Barwick, Ph.D., Patrick Kelly, Michael Yacovelli, Katherine Robinett, Andrew Lynn, Madeline Martinez, Kristina Krecko, Kristina Rapuano, Jeremy Robinson, Victoria Quimpo, Alicia Evans, Kaitlyn Longhren, and Shannon Hitchcock. The authors also thank Drs. Douglas Aukerman and Wayne Sebastinelli of Penn State University Sports Medicine for their support of our research. REFERENCES Bailey, C.M., Echemendia, R.J., & Arnett, P.A. (2005). Visual memory predicts concussion status independent of verbal memory and post-mild traumatic brain injury symptoms in college athletes. Paper presented at the 33rd annual meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society. Benedict, R.H. (1997). Brief Visuospatial Memory Test Revised: Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Benedict, R.H., Schretlen, D., Groninger, L., & Brandt, J. (1998). Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised: Normative data and analysis of inter-form and test-retest reliability. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12(1), 43 55. Bohnen, N., Twijnstra, A., & Jolles, J. (1992). Performance in the Stroop color word test in relationship to the persistence of symptoms following mild head injury. Acta Neurologica Scancdinavica, 85, 116 121. Bruce, J.M., & Echemendia, R.J. (2003). Delayed-onset deficits in verbal encoding strategies among patients with mild traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 17(4), 622 629. Diaz-Asper, C.M., Schretlen, D.J., & Pearlson, G.D. (2004). How well does IQ predict neuropsychological test performance in normal adults? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(1), 82 90. Dodrill, C.B. (1997). Myths of neuropsychology. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 11(1), 1 17. Hawkins, K.A., & Tulsky, D.S. (2001). The influence of IQ stratification on WAIS-III/WMS-III FSIQ-General Memory Index discrepancy base-rates in the standardization sample. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7(7), 875 880. Hochberg, R., & Rom, D. (1995). Extensions of multiple testing procedures based on Simes test. Journal of Statistical Planning and Inference, 48(2), 141 152. Iverson, G.L., Lovell, M.R., & Collins, M.W. (2005). Validity of ImPACT for measuring processing speed following sportsrelated concussion. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27(6), 683 689. Jacobson, N.S., & Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: A statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59(1), 12 19. Lovell, M.R., Collins, M.W., Podell, K., Powell, J., & Maroon, J. (2000). ImPACT: Immediate post-concussion assessment and cognitive testing. Computer software. Pittsburgh, PA: Neuro- Health Systems, LLC. Ponsford, J., & Kinsella, G. (1992). Attentional deficits following closed-head injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14(5), 822 838. Schatz, P., Pardini, J.E., Lovell, M.R., Collins, M.W., & Podell, K. (2006). Sensitivity and specificity of the ImPACT Test Battery for concussion in athletes. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21(1), 91 99. Schretlen, D.J., Buffington, A.L., Meyer, S.M., & Pearlson, G.D. (2005). The use of word-reading to estimate premorbid ability in cognitive domains other than intelligence. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11(6), 784 787. Smith, A. (1982). Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) Manual (Revised). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. The Psychological Corporation. (2001). The Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR):Test manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Trenerry, M.R., Crosson, B., DeBoe, J., & Leber, W.R. (1989). Stroop neuropsychological screening test. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS- III). New York: Psychological Corporation. Williams, R.H., & Zimmerman, D.W. (1977). The reliability of difference scores when errors are correlated. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 37, 679 689.