PERCEPTION OF AUDITORY-VISUAL SIMULTANEITY CHANGES BY ILLUMINANCE AT THE EYES

Similar documents
Recalibration of temporal order perception by exposure to audio-visual asynchrony Vroomen, Jean; Keetels, Mirjam; de Gelder, Bea; Bertelson, P.

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization

Modality Differences in Timing: Testing the Pacemaker Speed Explanation

REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l

CONTRIBUTION OF DIRECTIONAL ENERGY COMPONENTS OF LATE SOUND TO LISTENER ENVELOPMENT

Some methodological aspects for measuring asynchrony detection in audio-visual stimuli

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

Audio-visual synchrony perception

Low-Level Visual Processing Speed Modulates Judgment of Audio-Visual Simultaneity

Adaptation to motor-visual and motor-auditory temporal lags transfer across modalities

The basic hearing abilities of absolute pitch possessors

Bayesian Inference Explains Perception of Unity and Ventriloquism Aftereffect: Identification of Common Sources of Audiovisual Stimuli

Keywords: time perception; illusion; empty interval; filled intervals; cluster analysis

DOES FILLED DURATION ILLUSION TAKE PLACE FOR VERY SHORT TIME INTERVALS?

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Perception of Synchrony between the Senses

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

Influence of music-induced floor vibration on impression of music in concert halls

Simultaneity constancy

Exploring a brightness-drag illusion. Author. Published. Journal Title DOI. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Griffith Research Online

An active unpleasantness control system for indoor noise based on auditory masking

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

Audio-Visual Speech Timing Sensitivity Is Enhanced in Cluttered Conditions

Effects of stimulus duration on audio-visual synchrony perception

A NOVEL HEAD-RELATED TRANSFER FUNCTION MODEL BASED ON SPECTRAL AND INTERAURAL DIFFERENCE CUES

Supporting Information

Detectability and hearing impression of additional warning sounds for electric or hybrid vehicles

Perception of tonal components contained in wind turbine noise

The influence of the sensation of rhythm on comfort and productivity

3-D SOUND IMAGE LOCALIZATION BY INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES AND THE MEDIAN PLANE HRTF. Masayuki Morimoto Motokuni Itoh Kazuhiro Iida

Tilburg University. The spatial constraint in intersensory pairing Vroomen, Jean; Keetels, Mirjam

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

Quantifying temporal ventriloquism in audio-visual synchrony perception

Sound Localization PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 31. Audition

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals

TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS

Direction of visual apparent motion driven by perceptual organization of cross-modal signals

Effects of speaker's and listener's environments on speech intelligibili annoyance. Author(s)Kubo, Rieko; Morikawa, Daisuke; Akag

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion

Visual stream segregation has been proposed as a method to measure visual

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

21/01/2013. Binaural Phenomena. Aim. To understand binaural hearing Objectives. Understand the cues used to determine the location of a sound source

Voice Pitch Control Using a Two-Dimensional Tactile Display

AUTOCORRELATION AND CROSS-CORRELARION ANALYSES OF ALPHA WAVES IN RELATION TO SUBJECTIVE PREFERENCE OF A FLICKERING LIGHT

Quantifying temporal ventriloquism in audiovisual synchrony perception

Recent studies have shown that the brain can adjust the processing

Hearing. and other senses

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities

TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION

Systems Neuroscience Oct. 16, Auditory system. http:

The role of tone duration in dichotic temporal order judgment (TOJ)

7. Sharp perception or vision 8. The process of transferring genetic material from one cell to another by a plasmid or bacteriophage

The lowest level of stimulation that a person can detect. absolute threshold. Adapting one's current understandings to incorporate new information.

Tilburg University. Auditory grouping occurs prior to intersensory pairing Keetels, Mirjam; Stekelenburg, Jeroen; Vroomen, Jean

SUBJECTIVE EVALUATION OF COMFORTABLENESS OF WET COTTON AND PAPER HAND TOWELS OSHIBORI

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction

STUDY OF THE BISECTION OPERATION

Hearing II Perceptual Aspects

Influence of Frequency on Difference Thresholds for Magnitude of Vertical Sinusoidal Whole-Body Vibration

Sound exposure and the hearing of musicians

TEMPORAL ORDER JUDGEMENTS A SENSITIVE MEASURE FOR MEASURING PERCEPTUAL LATENCY?

Report. Direction of Visual Apparent Motion Driven Solely by Timing of a Static Sound. Elliot Freeman 1,2, * and Jon Driver 2 1

Study of perceptual balance for binaural dichotic presentation

CROSSMODAL PLASTICITY IN SPECIFIC AUDITORY CORTICES UNDERLIES VISUAL COMPENSATIONS IN THE DEAF "

Vibrotactile Stimulation Can Affect Auditory Loudness: A Pilot Study

Language Speech. Speech is the preferred modality for language.

Time Order as Psychological Bias

FACE THE MUSIC: A BEFORE AND AFTER STUDY OF THE HEARING ACUITY OF CLASSICAL MUSIC STUDENTS

EFFECTS OF TEMPORAL FINE STRUCTURE ON THE LOCALIZATION OF BROADBAND SOUNDS: POTENTIAL IMPLICATIONS FOR THE DESIGN OF SPATIAL AUDIO DISPLAYS

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis

On the influence of interaural differences on onset detection in auditory object formation. 1 Introduction

How high-frequency do children hear?

Tactile Communication of Speech

Gabriel Kreiman Phone: Web site: Dates: Time: Location: Biolabs 1075

Temporal offset judgments for concurrent vowels by young, middle-aged, and older adults

Sensation and Perception. A. Sensation: awareness of simple characteristics B. Perception: making complex interpretations

Gathering and Repetition of the Elements in an Image Affect the Perception of Order and Disorder

Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Week 1. Laboratory session: Measuring thresholds

Tilburg University. Directing Spatial Attention towards the Illusory Location of a Ventriloquized Sound Vroomen, Jean; Bertelson, P.

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency

Sound and Hearing. Decibels. Frequency Coding & Localization 1. Everything is vibration. The universe is made of waves.

Attention enhances feature integration

Effects of consciousness and consistency in manual control of visual stimulus on reduction of the flash-lag effect for luminance change

3-D Sound and Spatial Audio. What do these terms mean?

Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Week 5. The peripheral auditory system: The ear as a signal processor

Illusory reversal of temporal order: the bias to report a dimmer stimulus as the first

Consonant Perception test

Psychoacoustical Models WS 2016/17

Underwater hearing in California sea lions (Zalophus californianus): Expansion and interpretation of existing data

CROSSCULTURAL EFFECTS IN AUDIO-VISUAL INTERACTIONS

Masked Perception Thresholds of Low Frequency Tones Under Background Noises and Their Estimation by Loudness Model

The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function

Evidence against response bias in temporal order tasks with attention manipulation by masked primes

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency. (Some slides are adapted from Zhiyao Duan s course slides on Computer Audition and Its Applications in Music)

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Definition Slides. Sensation. Perception. Bottom-up processing. Selective attention. Top-down processing 11/3/2013

Seeing Sound: Changing Visual Perception Through Cross-Modal Interaction. Tracey D. Berger. New York University. Department of Psychology

Perceptual congruency of audio-visual speech affects ventriloquism with bilateral visual stimuli

Transcription:

23 rd International Congress on Sound & Vibration Athens, Greece 10-14 July 2016 ICSV23 PERCEPTION OF AUDITORY-VISUAL SIMULTANEITY CHANGES BY ILLUMINANCE AT THE EYES Hiroshi Hasegawa and Shu Hatakeyama Graduate School of Engineering, Utsunomiya University, 7-1-2 Yoto, Utsunomiya-shi, Tochigi-ken, 321-8585, Japan, email: hasegawa@is.utsunomiya-u.ac.jp In this study, we measured the point of subjective simultaneity (PSS) between auditory and visual stimuli under various conditions of illuminance at the subject s eyes. The test stimuli were a pure tone of 1000 Hz at a sound pressure level of 50 db and a white light (LED). Both sound and light stimuli had a duration of 10 ms. The luminance of the LED was set at 21.2 cd/m 2, and the ambient illuminance around the LED was set at 0 lx. The illuminance at the subject s eyes was adjusted to 0, 500, or 1000 lx using a spotlight located above the subject s head. The test stimuli had a stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) of 0, ±20, ±40, ±80, or ±160 ms, where positive values indicate that the visual stimulus was presented first. Four men in their early 20s participated in the experiments. We presented the test stimuli to the subject with the nine SOAs in random order. After each presentation of the test stimuli, we asked subjects to answer which of the stimuli sound or light they perceived first. Then we evaluated the PSS of the test stimuli and considered the influence of illuminance at the eyes on the PSS. As a result, the PSS shifted toward positive direction, i.e., the perception of light precedence as the illuminance at the eyes increased. 1. Introduction Many studies have been done on the perception of auditory and visual information [1] [15]. We have been studying about the percetion of auditory-visual simultaneity [11] [14]. We investigated the point of subjective simultaneity (PSS) between auditory and visual stimuli depending on the subject s attention, and showed that the PSS relatively shifted toward the perception of sound precedence when they paid attention to the light[11]. We also showed that the luminance of the light and the ambient illuminance of the room had an influence on the perception of the simultaneity between auditory and visual stimuli [12, 14]. In this study, we focused on the illuminance at the subject s eyes and investigated the perception of simultaneity between auditory and visual stimuli when the illuminance level was changed. 2. Experiment We investigated effects of the illuminance level at the subject s eyes on the perception of simultaneity between auditory and visual stimuli. 2.1 Stimuli We used a pure tone of 1000 Hz at 50 db SPL (sound pressure level) and a white light (LED; LP-10HW3B) with a luminance of 21.2 cd/m 2. Both stimuli had a duration of 10 ms. The auditory stimulus had a linear fade-in and fade-out of 2.5 ms to reduce transient responses. 1

2.2 Apparatus We carried out the experiments in a soundproof room (D-30; Fig. 1). We played the auditory stimuli on a CD player (XL-V1-N, Victor) with amplification through an amplifier (CR-D2, Onkyo), and presented them to each subject via headphones (SRH840, Shure). The luminance of the LED light was set to 21.2 cd/m 2 with a lighting controller (LMD01, Fitdesign). We adjusted the illuminance at the subject s eyes to 0, 500, or 1000 lx using a spotlight located above the subject s head as shown in Fig. 2). The timing of the light emission was controlled by a trigger signal sent from the CD player to the controller. The subject sat on a chair in front of the LED light. The testing space was closed off with a blackout curtain to eliminate all other light. Soundproof room (D-30) Subject Blackout curtain Amplifier (CR-D2, Onkyo) Experimenter Headphones (SRH840, Shure) LED Trigger signal CD player (XL-V1-N, Victor) Lighting controller (LMD01, Fitdesign) Light luminance control voltage signal DC power supply (PMC18-2, Kikusui) Figure 1: Experimental apparatus. The sound stimuli were presented via headphones to the subject. The visual stimuli were presented using a LED light controlled by a light controller. 2.3 Procedure We used a method of constant stimuli to determine the PSS between the sound and light stimuli. These stimuli were presented at offsets (stimulus onset asynchronies: SOAs) of 0, ±20, ±40, ±80, and ±160 ms, where positive values indicate that the visual stimulus was presented first. We presented the test stimuli with the nine SOAs in random order, every 5 s (Fig. 3). The PSS between the two stimuli was obtained by fitting a psychometric function to the results as the 50% point of the answer rate with respect to the SOA. After each presentation, the subject answered: The sound was perceived first, or The light was perceived first. 2.4 Subjects and trials Four men in their early 20s participated in the experiments. All had normal hearing acuity and normal or corrected-to-normal vision. We conducted 45 trials (9 SOAs 5 iterations) per session and performed 20 sessions in each illuminance level of 0, 500, and 1000 lx, in which the presentation order was different in every session. In total, we performed 10, 800 trials (45 trials 20 sessions 3 illuminance levels 4 subjects). 2 ICSV23, Athens (Greece), 10-14 July 2016

Spotlight (EL-S1000L/W, Mitsubishi Electric) LED (LP-10HW3B) Blackout curtain Illuminance at the eyes: 0, 500, or 1000 lx Headphones (SRH840, Shure) Subject Lighting controller (LMD01, Fitdesign) Soundproof room (D-30) Figure 2: Schematic figure of the experiment. Illuminance at the subject s eyes was adjusted to 0, 500, or 1000 lx using a spotlight located above the subject s head. 3. Results Figures 4 (a) (c) show the experimental results in the condtions that the illuminance level at the eyes were 0, 500, and 1000 lx, respectively. The vertical and horizontal axes denote the answer rate and the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between the sound and light, respectively. The white and dark bars indicate the answer rates sound was perceived first and light was perceived first, respectively. In all the figures, the perception of sound precedence decreased and that of light precedence increased as the SOA increased. At an SOA of 0 ms, the perception of light precedence was greater in all the conditions. These results mean that the point of subjective simultaneity (PSS) shifted to sound precedence. Sound stimulus (50 db, 1 khz, 10 ms) Light stimulus (21.2 cd/m 2, 10 ms) 160 ~160 ms Lag 5 s Interstimulus interval 160 ~160 ms Lag Figure 3: Presentation timing of the sound and light stimuli. ICSV23, Athens (Greece), 10-14 July 2016 3

100 : Sound precedence : Light precedence 80 Answer rate (%) 60 40 20 0 160 80 40 20 0 20 40 80 160 Sound first Stimulus onset asynchrony (ms) Light first (a) Illuminance level: 0 lx 100 : Sound precedence : Light precedence 80 Answer rate (%) 60 40 20 0 160 80 40 20 0 20 40 80 160 Sound first Stimulus onset asynchrony (ms) Light first (b) Illuminance level: 500 lx 100 : Sound precedence : Light precedence 80 Answer rate (%) 60 40 20 0 160 80 40 20 0 20 40 80 160 Sound first Stimulus onset asynchrony (ms) Light first (c) Illuminance level: 1000 lx Figure 4: Experimental results in the conditions of the illuminance level of 0, 500, and 1000 lx at the eyes. The vertical and horizontal axes denote the answer rate and the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between the sound and light, respectively. The white and dark bars indicate the perception of sound and light precedence, respectively. 4 ICSV23, Athens (Greece), 10-14 July 2016

4. Discussion We fitted a psychometric function [11] to the result of each subject in each condtion, and obtained the PSS as the 50% point of the answer rate with respect to the SOA. Table 1 shows the average values of PSS of all subjects in the conditions that the illuminance at the eyes was 0, 500, and 1000 lx. Table 1: Averaged PSS values in each illuminance level of 0, 500, and 1000 lx at the eyes. Illuminance level at the eyes (lx) 0 500 1000 PSS (ms) 33.5 30.0 26.7 In Table 1, all the PSS values were negative, i.e., the PSS shifted toward the perception of sound precedence. In comparing the PSS values among the illuminance levels, the PSS shifted toward positive direction, i.e., the perception of light precedence as the illuminance level increased. This tendency agreed with the our previous reports [14], that is, by raising the illuminance level at the eyes, relative attention to the LED light decreased, and thus attention to the sound relatively increased. Paying attention to the sound increased the speed of transmission of auditory information, so the sound and light were perceived as simultaneous when the light was presented earlier. The PSS then shifted toward the perception of light precedence. 5. Conclusion The point of subjective simultaneity (PSS) between the auditory and visual stimuli shifted toward the perception of light precedence as the illuminance level at the eyes increased. Further experiments would be required to ensure this result by increasing the number of subjects. References 1. Hershenson, M., Reaction time as a measure of intersensory facilitation, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63 (3), 289 293 (1962). 2. Aschersleben, G. and Müsseler, J., Dissociations in the timing of stationary and moving stimuli, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25 (6), 1709 1720, (1999). 3. Stone, J. V., Hunkin, N. M., Porrill, J., Wood, R., Keeler, V., Beanl, M., Port, M., and Porter, N. R., When is now? Perception of simultaneity, Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Science, 268, 31 38, (2001). 4. Sugita, Y. and Suzuki, Y., Implicit estimation of sound-arrival time, Nature, 421, No. 27, 911, (2003). 5. Neumann O. and Niepel M., Timing of perception and perception of time, Psychophysics beyond sensation: laws and invariants of human cognition, 245 269, (2003). 6. Fujisaki, W., Shimojo, S., Kashino, M., and Nishida, S., Recalibration of audiovisual simultaneity, Nature Neuroscience, 7, 773 778, (2004). 7. Lewald, J. and Guski, R., Auditory-visual temporal integration as a function of distance: no compensation for sound-transmission time in human perception, Neuroscience Letters, 357 (2), 119 122, (2004). ICSV23, Athens (Greece), 10-14 July 2016 5

8. Arnold, D. H., Johnston, A., and Nishida, S., Timing sight and sound, Vision Research, 45 (10), 1275 1284, (2005). 9. Zampini, M., Shore, D. I., and Spence, C., Audiovisual prior entry, Neuroscience Letters, 381 (3), 217 222, (2005). 10. Zampini, M., Guest, S., Shore, D. I., and Spence, C., Audio-visual simultaneity judgments, Perception & Psychophysics, 67 (3), 531 544, (2005). 11. Hasegawa, H., Yoshida, J., and Kasuga, M., Effect of attention to preceding stimuli on the simultaneity perception between an auditory and visual stimulus, Proceedings of 20th International Congress on Acoustics, 236, (2010). 12. Hasegawa, H., Kasuga, M., and Yoshida, J., Influence of light luminance on auditory-visual simultaneity perception, Proceedings of Forum Acusticum 2011, 000204, (2011). 13. Hasegawa, H., Yamamoto, Y., Saito, W., and Yoshida, J., Learning changes the perception of the equivalence of strengths between auditory and visual stimuli, Proceedings of 20th International Congress on Sound and Vibration, 341, (2013). 14. Hasegawa, H., Ishiwatari, K., and Yoshida, J., Ambient illuminance alters perception of auditoryvisual simultaneity, Proceedings of Forum Acusticum 2014, R17_12, (2014). 15. H. Hasegawa and W. Saito: Learning of the equality between an auditory and a visual stimulus alters the perception of auditory-visual simultaneity, Proceedings of 22nd International Congress on Sound and Vibration 2015, 676, (2015). 6 ICSV23, Athens (Greece), 10-14 July 2016