Shifting Between Cognitive and Visual Distraction: The Impact of Cognitive Ability on Distraction Caused by Secondary Tasks

Similar documents
Estimation of Driver Inattention to Forward Objects Using Facial Direction with Application to Forward Collision Avoidance Systems

Effects of Visual and Cognitive Distraction on Lane Change Test Performance

Eye Movement Patterns and Driving Performance

The Danger of Incorrect Expectations In Driving: The Failure to Respond

Evaluating the Safety of Verbal Interface Use while Driving

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING

Changing Driver Behavior Through Unconscious Stereotype Activation

Stimulus-Response Compatibilitiy Effects for Warning Signals and Steering Responses

Traffic Sign Detection and Identification

Aging and the Detection of Collision Events in Fog

Multimodal Driver Displays: Potential and Limitations. Ioannis Politis

The Effects of Age and Distraction on Reaction Time in a Driving Simulator

CAN WE PREDICT STEERING CONTROL PERFORMANCE FROM A 2D SHAPE DETECTION TASK?

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

A Model for Automatic Diagnostic of Road Signs Saliency

Verbal-Spatial Cue Conflict: Implications for the Design of Collision-Avoidance Warning Systems

Characterizing Visual Attention during Driving and Non-driving Hazard Perception Tasks in a Simulated Environment

Hazard Perception and Distraction in Novice Drivers: Effects of 12 Months Driving Experience

Divided-attention task on driving simulator: comparison among three groups of drivers

OPTIC FLOW IN DRIVING SIMULATORS

CANTAB Test descriptions by function

Crash Risk Analysis of Distracted Driving Behavior: Influence of Secondary Task Engagement and Driver Characteristics

THE EFFECT OF VOICE INTERACTIONS ON DRIVERS GUIDANCE OF ATTENTION

Naturalistic Driving Performance During Secondary Tasks

Verbal Collision Avoidance Messages of Varying Perceived Urgency Reduce Crashes in High Risk Scenarios

Effects of Driver and Secondary Task Characteristics on Lane Change Test Performance

Protecting Workers with Smart E-Vests

Adjusted Crash Odds Ratio Estimates of Driver Behavior Errors: A Re-Analysis of the SHRP2 Naturalistic Driving Study Data

Supplementary Figure 1. Psychology Experiment Building Language (PEBL) screen-shots

Assessment of police subjective workload and preference for using a voice-based interface during simulated driving

DRIVER S SITUATION AWARENESS DURING SUPERVISION OF AUTOMATED CONTROL Comparison between SART and SAGAT measurement techniques

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist

Test-Retest Reliability of Simulated Driving Performance: A Pilot Study

Cognitive and Psychomotor Correlates of Self- Reported Driving Skills and Behavior

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction

Koji Sakai. Kyoto Koka Women s University, Ukyo-ku Kyoto, Japan

Iran. T. Allahyari, J. Environ. et Health. al., USEFUL Sci. Eng., FIELD 2007, OF Vol. VIEW 4, No. AND 2, RISK pp OF... processing system, i.e

THE DIMENSIONS OF DRIVER PERFORMANCE DURING SECONDARY MANUAL TASKS

A FIELD STUDY ASSESSING DRIVING PERFORMANCE, VISUAL ATTENTION, HEART RATE AND SUBJECTIVE RATINGS IN RESPONSE TO TWO TYPES OF COGNITIVE WORKLOAD

Application of ecological interface design to driver support systems

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

Phonak Target 4.3. Desktop Fitting Guide. Content. March 2016

Using the method of adjustment to enhance collision warning perception

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab

THE EFFECT OF COGNITIVE TASKS ON PREDICTING EVENTS IN TRAFFIC

This is a repository copy of Leading to Distraction: Driver distraction, lead car, and road environment.

Distracted Driving Effects on CMV Operators

SPATIAL AUDIO VS. VERBAL DIRECTIONAL CUES: AN EXAMINATION OF SALIENCE AND DISRUPTIVENESS WITHIN A SIMULATED DRIVING CONTEXT

Detecting and Reading Text on HUDs: Effects of Driving Workload and Message Location

The role of memory on patients with mild cognitive impairment. Sophia Vardaki, PhD National Technical University of Athens

Development of a new loudness model in consideration of audio-visual interaction

Effects of Narcotic Analgesics on Driving

Identification of human factors criteria to assess the effects of level crossing safety measures

Deciphering Psychological-Physiological Mappings While Driving and Performing a Secondary Memory Task

Prevention of drowsy driving by means of warning sound

Effects of Central Nervous System Depressants on Driving

Distraction, Cognition, Behaviour and Driving Analysis of a large data set

Driver Distraction. Bryan Reimer. Auto UI Tutorials October 17, 2012

Chapter 5 Car driving

Enhancement of Application Software for Examination of Differential Magnification Methods and Magnification Interface Factors

Cue Saliency and Age as Factors Affecting Performance in a Card Sorting Task

Junko Fuse, Atsushi Ohsaga ABSTRACT

Predictors of Mind-Wandering While Driving

Effects of Cannabis on Driving

Distracted Driving. Stephanie Bonne, MD

Capturing Driver Response to In-Vehicle Human- Machine Interface Technologies Using Facial Thermography

Inhibitory Control and Peer Passengers Predict Risky Driving in Young Novice Drivers - A Simulator Study

TOPICS IN AMPLIFICATION

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

7/2/ Cell Phone Induced Failures of Visual Awareness. During Simulated Driving. David L. Strayer, Frank A. Drews, and William A.

Mitigating cognitive distraction and its effects with interface design and collision avoidance systems

optimising the value of sound quality evaluations by observing assessors driving strategies

Distracted Driving among Teens. What We Know about It and How to Prevent It May 31 st, 2017

Measuring IVIS Impact to Driver by On-road Test and Simulator Experiment Chenjiang Xie a,b, * Tong Zhu a,b Chunlin Guo a,b Yimin Zhang a a

Research Review: Multiple Resource Theory. out in multi-task environments. Specifically, multiple display layout and control design

Desktop Fitting Guide for Phonak Brio 3

Intelligent Object Group Selection

Pupillary Response Based Cognitive Workload Measurement under Luminance Changes

SEAT BELT VIBRATION AS A STIMULATING DEVICE FOR AWAKENING DRIVERS ABSTRACT

Detection of Driver s Low Vigilance Using Vehicle Steering. Information and Facial Inattention Features

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

The Context and Purpose of Executive Function Assessments: Beyond the Tripartite Model

Young Adults with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): The impact of secondary tasks on driving performance

Objective Methods for Assessments of Influence of IVIS (In-Vehicle Information Systems) on Safe Driving

Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions.

Comparison of speech intelligibility between normal headsets and bone conduction hearing devices at call center

DISTRACTION AN ACADEMIC PERSPECTIVE. Steve Reed Loughborough Design School

TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS

Encoding of Information in Auditory Displays: Initial Research on Flexibility and Processing Codes in Dual-task Scenarios

Noise-Robust Speech Recognition in a Car Environment Based on the Acoustic Features of Car Interior Noise

Journal of Emerging Trends in Computing and Information Sciences

Driver Distraction: Towards A Working Definition

Cell Phones and Driving 1. Why Do Cell Phone Conversations Interfere With Driving?

Potential Benefits of a Concurrent Verbal Task when Feeling Fatigued Due to Monotonous Driving Conditions

Resident Fatigue. A Primer For Residents

Perceptions of Risk Factors for Road Traffic Accidents

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

Predicting Older Drivers' Difficulties Using the Roadwise Review

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Transcription:

University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2013 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 18th, 12:00 AM Shifting Between Cognitive and Visual Distraction: The Impact of Cognitive Ability on Distraction Caused by Secondary Tasks Sachi Mizobuchi Vocalage Inc., Toronto, Ontario, Canada Mark Chignell Vocalage Inc., Toronto, Ontario, Canada Junko Suzuki Toyota InfoTechnology Center Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan Ko Koga Toyota InfoTechnology Center Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan Kazunari Nawa Toyota InfoTechnology Center Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan Follow this and additional works at: http://ir.uiowa.edu/drivingassessment Mizobuchi, Sachi; Chignell, Mark; Suzuki, Junko; Koga, Ko; and Nawa, Kazunari. Shifting Between Cognitive and Visual Distraction: The Impact of Cognitive Ability on Distraction Caused by Secondary Tasks. In: Proceedings of the Seventh International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design, June 17-20, 2013, Bolton Landing, New York. Iowa City, IA: Public Policy Center, University of Iowa, 2013: 142-148. https://doi.org/10.17077/drivingassessment.1480 This Event is brought to you for free and open access by the Public Policy Center at Iowa Research Online. It has been accepted for inclusion in Driving Assessment Conference by an authorized administrator of Iowa Research Online. For more information, please contact lib-ir@uiowa.edu.

SHIFTING BETWEEN COGNITIVE AND VISUAL DISTRACTION: THE IMPACT OF COGNITIVE ABILITY ON DISTRACTION CAUSED BY SECONDARY TASKS Sachi Mizobuchi 1&2, Mark Chignell 1&2, Junko Suzuki 3, Ko Koga 3 & Kazunari Nawa 3 1 Vocalage Inc., Toronto, Ontario, Canada 2 University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada 3 Toyota InfoTechnology Center Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan Email: sachi@vocalage.com Summary: We conducted an experiment in order to investigate impacts of central executive (CE) functions and modality of secondary task presentation in a dualtask experiment. We found that shifting ability, out of three major CE functions (inhibition, shifting, and updating) was particularly important in determining whether primary (pedal-tracking) task performance was better in the presence of auditory, vs. visual, presentation of the secondary task. INTRODUCTION New in-vehicle applications have potential impacts on driving safety. More tasks compete for the driver s attention, distracting even as they provide benefits (e.g., navigation support). Do individual differences in central executive functions (major components of cognitive ability) moderate the impacts of distraction and also influence the decision as to which modality should be used to present secondary tasks while driving? The multiple attentional resource theory (Wickens, 2002) approach might suggest using auditory presentation of secondary tasks to avoid overloading visual attentional resources. However, auditory presentation of secondary task information may lead to cognitive distraction by creating a greater load on working memory, since auditory information has to be integrated over time to infer meaning. Thus there is a design tradeoff, with auditory presentation of secondary tasks tending to create cognitive distraction, and visual presentation tending to create visual distraction. Cognitive and visual distraction created by new interactive technologies and applications in vehicles may lead to inattention to the primary driving task, and inattention is known to increase the risk of accidents (Klauer et al., 2006). In this research, we consider three main central executive (CE) functions in working memory: "inhibition", "shifting", and "updating" (cf. Miyake et al., 2000). Mizobuchi et al. (2011) found that inhibition, updating, and shifting were all involved when carrying out device operation during simulated driving. These CE functions are presumably also involved in the driving task (e.g., updating one s knowledge of where the vehicle is relative to the roadway; switching attention between the roadway ahead and views in mirrors; inhibiting routine driving modes when faced with traffic signals, road signs, or a stopped school bus). How do differences in cognitive ability affect ability to drive while performing cognitively loading secondary tasks? Baumann et al. (2008) assessed the effect of CE load on driving performance (time to collision and driving speed). They found that participants received less benefit from a warning signal when they had to perform a running memory task. They concluded that the central executive component of working memory is strongly involved in the construction of situation awareness. Mäntylä et al. (2009) also examined the relationship between CE function and driving 142

performance. In their experiment, high school students completed a simulated driving task and six experimental tasks that tapped the three CE functions of inhibition, shifting, and updating. Their results showed that updating ability was a significant predictor of performance on a Lane Change Task (LCT) while doing simulated driving. Thus earlier research has established a role of CE functions in moderating driving performance distracted by secondary tasks. Our research sought to extend these results to incorporate the impact of secondary task modality. METHOD We screened 102 people on three tasks, a Stroop task (inhibition), a colour monitoring task (updating), and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; shifting). Thirty-four of these people then participated in the main experiment. People were chosen so as to cover a variety of different profiles in terms of shifting, updating, and inhibition scores. The selected participants consisted of 20 males and 14 females, aged from 17 to 64 years old (M=42.9, SD=13.2). Design The experiment was a within-participant design with 12 conditions (2 primary tasks x 3 CE functions x 2 modalities). Only results for the pedal tracking task will be reported here. Results for the second primary task (a target detection task) will be reported elsewhere. Pedal tracking task This task was designed to mimic driving behind a lead vehicle. The task was based on a pedaltracking task used by Uno and Nakamura (2010). A target rectangle in blue (corresponds to a car in front) and a frame-shaped area in yellow were displayed on the main display (Figure 1). Figure 1. Pedal tracking task The participants' goal was to keep the outer edge of the target rectangle inside the yellow area by controlling a foot pedal. To simulate adjust inter-vehicle distance controlling an acceleration pedal, the size (side length) of the target rectangle (D) was defined by the equation (1). D = D 0 + ( V 0 + ( S f L t )dt)dt (1) Initially D 0 was equal to half the width of the acceptable area (yellow area), V 0 equaled 0 km/h and dt was 0.1sec. S f represented the fluctuating signal while L t was a percentage of the first order lag of the throttle opening. D was the second-order integral of the difference between the fluctuating signal (corresponding to the acceleration of the car in front) and the control signal 143

(corresponding to the acceleration of one s own car; the first order lag of the throttle opening %). The fluctuation signal was generated from a mixture of four sine waves. Secondary tasks (CE tasks) We developed the following inhibition, shifting, and updating tasks in both visual and audio conditions. The secondary task was presented in the top left quadrant of the secondary display. (1) Visual Inhibition (VI). We used the Stroop task from the screening test as a visual inhibition task. (2) Audio Inhibition (AS). We used a modified auditory Stroop task based on previous research (Hamers et al., 1972; Morgan, A., 1989). Three different words ("High", "Day", "Low") were presented individually in two pitches (High pitch: 290Hz, Low pitch: 110 Hz) with semi-random word-pitch combinations (the numbers of each combination of word-pitch was balanced). The participant's task was to indicate the pitch by pressing a corresponding key (low = left arrow, high = right arrow). (3) Visual Shifting (VS). A single digit was presented on a display with three single digit numbers underneath it. Participants were expected to apply one of the rules (-1, +0, +1) to the target number, and then choose the answer from the three options provided. Participants were instructed to find the expected rule as quickly as possible and to apply the same rule until it changed (ex., -1, -1, -1, -1,..., +0, +0, +0, +0,... ). After eight consecutive correct responses, the program changed the rule. (4) Audio Shifting (AS). The procedure in this task was equivalent to the VS task except that all the stimuli were presented in audio; A single digit number (1-8; the target number) was presented in a high-pitched voice (290Hz) followed by three single digit number (0-9) in lowpitch voice (110Hz) as options. Feedback to an incorrect response was given using a beep sound. The three option numbers corresponded to the left, down and right arrow keys, in that order. (5) Visual Updating (VU). Participants were shown blue, yellow and red circles (8cm in diameter) one at a time for 500ms in randomized order with an inter-stimulus interval of 2500ms. The task was to respond when the third instance of each circle color was presented (e.g., after seeing the third blue circle, or the third yellow circle), which required participants to monitor and keep track of the number of times each color had been presented. For example, if the sequence was blue, red, yellow, yellow, red, blue, yellow, blue, red then the participant should have responded to the third blue, yellow and red circle (italicized). In order for momentary mental lapses to have less impact on task performance, the circle count for each color was automatically reset to 0 if the participant made a key press for that color, and participants were informed of this feature before starting the task. Prior to completing the trial blocks, participants received a practice session, which continued until they made 3 correct responses. (6) Audio Updating (AU). The procedure in this task was equivalent to the VU task except that all the stimuli were presented in audio; instead of the colored circles, participants were presented with high-pitched tones (880Hz) and low-pitched tones (220Hz) for 500ms, with an inter- 144

stimulus interval of 2500ms. They were instructed to respond to the second occurrence of tones. This was the modified procedure (Miyake et al., 2000) of the Mental Counters task (Larson et al. 1988). Apparatus The main and CE task programs were run on the same computer (Hardware: Gigabyte Technology Co., Ltd./ X58A-UD3R, OS: Microsoft Windows XP Professional), and were shown on the main display (Acer 23"/58cm Wide LCD Monitor, S231HL) and secondary display (Dell E177FPf TFT, E177FPf) correspondingly. Participants' input was given with a foot pedal (Logitech/ Driving Force GT) for the primary tasks, and with a keyboard for the secondary tasks. Experimental equipment was set up as shown in Figure 2. Procedure Figure 2. Experiment settings All experimental sessions were conducted at University of Toronto from January through March 2012. Participants participated in the experiment individually. The participants first performed the pedal tracking task as a single task. They then performed the pedal tracking task and CE tasks as dual tasks. The CE tasks consisted of the six CE function conditions (AI, VI, AS, VS, AU, and VU). The order of the CE task was varied among participants. Each participant was exposed to all 6 dual task conditions (one CE task at a time), with approximately two minutes' worth of trials per condition. Ordering of conditions was counterbalanced between participants. Participants were instructed to respond as quickly and accurately as they could, and to perform as well as they could on both of the tasks. Participants were paid for their participation and signed a consent form before participating, in accordance with a research protocol that was approved by the University of Toronto Ethics Review Board. Results Based on eye tracking data, we calculated the proportion of the time that participants viewed the main vs. the secondary task display (main display gaze rate). Due to missing data, eye gaze data for 22 of the participants was used in this analysis. Figure 3 shows the main display gaze rate (line) and pedal tracking task error rate (bars) across the six secondary task conditions. Error bars shown in this and following figures are standard errors. The main effect of modality on gaze rate to the main display was higher in the audio vs. visual secondary task conditions (F[1, 21]=11.41, p<.005), i.e., there was less visual distraction in the audio conditions. However, the VU condition showed a higher gaze rate on the main display (lower visual distraction) as compared 145

to the other visual conditions. This may be because the VU task could be performed using peripheral vision, since the stimuli (colored circle of 8cm in diameter) were large enough, as was reported by some of the participants. Main display gaze rate (%) 100 80 60 40 20 0 Tracking error rate AI AS AU VI VS VU Condition Figure 3. Main display gaze rate and the primary task performance Analysis of covariance was run with the three CE function abilities (inhibition, shifting, updating) as covariates, the three CE loading tasks as one within subjects factor, and the modality of secondary task presentation (visual vs. auditory) as a second within subjects factor. There was a borderline significant three way interaction between shifting ability and the two other factors (CE loading tasks, modality) (F[1.63, 49]=2.72, p<.10), with degrees of freedom in the analysis adjusted using the Greenhouse-Geiser criterion to account for non-sphericity. After viewed the histogram of shifting ability (as measured by number of completed rules in the WCST), we defined high shifting ability as those people who completed seven or more rules (with low ability assigned for fewer than seven completed rules). We plotted the three way interaction between the high vs. low measure of shifting ability, CE loading tasks and modality. From visual inspection there seemed to be an interaction between shifting ability and modality for the inhibition secondary task, but only a shifting ability main effect for the updating and shifting secondary tasks. We then carried out two follow up analyses: a) analysis of the suspected two way interaction between shifting ability and modality for the inhibition secondary task; b) analysis of the three way interaction between shifting ability, modality and CE loading task (with updating and shifting only). As expected, there was a significant interaction between modality and shifting ability for the inhibition secondary task (F[1.32]=7.27, p<.05). As shown in Figure 4 (left), people with high shifting ability generally had a lower pedal tracking error rate in the presence of the inhibition secondary task. In addition, people with higher shifting ability were not affected by presentation modality of the secondary task, while people with lower shifting ability did better with auditory (vs. visual) presentation of the secondary task. In the second analysis (looking at the three way interaction, but with only the shifting and updating loading secondary tasks), the only significant effect (there were no significant interactions) was the main effect of shifting ability (F[1,31]=7.73, p<.01). Figure 4 (right) shows the corresponding plot for the data pooled across the shifting and updating secondary tasks. Primary Pedal Tracking (PPT) Performance was much better for people with high shifting ability. In order to demonstrate that the lower error rate in visual conditions for people with high shifting ability was due to visual switching between primary and secondary tasks, we plotted relevant eye gaze data (Figure 5). As expected, for the inhibition and updating tasks there were significantly more switches in gaze between the monitors for people with high shifting ability. For the shifting 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 Tracking Error Rate 146

secondary task there was a tendency for high shifting ability people to have more shifts in gaze, but this difference was not statistically significant. 0.4 Error rate of PPT 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 audio visual Modality of secondary task (Inhibition) Sihfting abilitiy low high Figure 4. PPT error rate by secondary task modality and shifting ability (left: for Inhibition task only, right: for Updating and Shifting task only) Number of monitor change 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Shifting ability (Number of completed rules ) low high VI VS VU Secondary task condition Figure 5. Number of switches in gaze between monitors by shifting ability and secondary task (visual only) condition DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION People with high shifting ability compensated for the effects of visual distraction by switching visual attention more frequently between primary and secondary tasks. This greater visual switching was most pronounced in the inhibition and updating secondary conditions. The reduced switching advantage for high shifting participants in the shifting loading secondary condition likely occurred because the secondary task was consuming CE shifting resources, reducing availability of shifting resources for switching attention between tasks. Overall, we found that shifting ability was the most important of the cognitive abilities relating to CE function, moderating the relationship between modality and secondary task loading when pedal-tracking was the primary task. People with higher shifting ability generally performed better on the pedal-tracking task (with fewer errors) across all modalities and CE loading secondary tasks. That general pattern of superiority for those with high shifting ability was also true with tasks that loaded on the shifting, and updating, CE functions. However, for the inhibition loading secondary task we observed an interaction between shifting ability and modality such that, for people with high shifting ability, visual presentation led to better 147

performance (lower pedal tracking errors) whereas for people with low shifting ability auditory presentation led to better performance. Further research is needed to confirm these results, particularly in driving simulators or vehicles. However, one implication of this work is that type of secondary display should depend on the shifting ability. Low shifting ability drivers should benefit from auditory display of secondary tasks, while high shifting ability drivers should perform better with visual display of those tasks. It remains to be seen if, given the choice, will people choose a mode of presentation (visual or auditory) for a secondary task that correctly matches their level of shifting ability. ACKNOWLEDGMENT We would like to thank Chorong Lee, Andrea Jovanovic, Rie Toriyama, Ryan Kealey and Phil Lam who helped run the experiments, and Pierre Duez who helped in software development. We would also like to thank Professor Moshe Eizenman, Kai Fok and Sahar Javaherhaghighi for their help in eye tracking system, and David Canella for his help in revising this paper. REFERENCES Baumann, M. R. K, Petzoldt, T., Hogema, J. & Krems, J. F. (2008). The effect of cognitive tasks on predicting events in traffic. In Proceedings of the European Conference on Human Centred Design for Intelligent Transport Systems. Lyon, France, 2008, 3-11. Hamers, J. E, & Lambert, W. E. (1972). Bilingual interdependencies in auditory perception. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 303-310. Klauer, S. G., Dingus, T. A., Neale, V. L., Sudweeks, J. D., & Ramsey, D. J. (2006). The Impact of Driver Inattention on Near-Crash/Crash Risk: An Analysis Using the 100-Car Naturalistic Driving Study Data. DO HS 810594. U.S. Department of Transportation NHTSA. Larson, G. E., Merritt,C. R. & Williams, S. E. (1988). Information processing and intelligence: Some implications of task complexity, Intelligence, Volume 12(2), 131 147. Mäntylä, T., Karlsson, M., & Marklund, M. (2009). Executive control functions in simulated driving. Applied Neuropsychology, 16, 11-18. Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H. & Howerter A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex Frontal Lobe tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49 100. Mizobuchi, S., Chignell, M., Suzuki, J., Koga, K. & Nawa, K. (2011) Central Executive Functions Likely Mediate the Impact of Device Operation When Driving, Proceeding of third conference on automotive UI and interactive vehicular applications, 129-136. Morgan, A. (1989) An auditory Stroop effect for pitch, loudness, and time, Brain and Language, 36 (4), 592-603. Uno, H., and Nakamura, Y. (2010) Study on surrogate laboratory techniques to assess driver workload induced from voice-input interface. Proceedings of the JASE annual congress (Fall), No.99-10, 1-6. (in Japanese) Wickens, C.D. (2002): Multiple resources and performance prediction, Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 3:2, 159-177. 148