functions grow at a higher rate than in normal{hearing subjects. In this chapter, the correlation

Similar documents
Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology. Psychoacoustics of hearing impairment

What Is the Difference between db HL and db SPL?

Reliability of Categorical Loudness Scaling and its Relation to Threshold

Audiogram+: The ReSound Proprietary Fitting Algorithm

Issues faced by people with a Sensorineural Hearing Loss

Audiogram+: GN Resound proprietary fitting rule

Representation of sound in the auditory nerve

Christopher J. Plack Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester CO4 3SQ, England

C HAPTER FOUR. Audiometric Configurations in Children. Andrea L. Pittman. Introduction. Methods

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

BINAURAL DICHOTIC PRESENTATION FOR MODERATE BILATERAL SENSORINEURAL HEARING-IMPAIRED

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Healthy Organ of Corti. Loss of OHCs. How to use and interpret the TEN(HL) test for diagnosis of Dead Regions in the cochlea

9/29/14. Amanda M. Lauer, Dept. of Otolaryngology- HNS. From Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics, Green & Swets (1966)

Characterizing individual hearing loss using narrow-band loudness compensation

ClaroTM Digital Perception ProcessingTM

Categorical loudness scaling and equal-loudness contours in listeners with normal hearing and hearing loss

Acoustics Research Institute

Psychoacoustical Models WS 2016/17

Frequency refers to how often something happens. Period refers to the time it takes something to happen.

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening

Basic Audiogram Interpretation

Auditory fmri correlates of loudness perception for monaural and diotic stimulation

Masker-signal relationships and sound level

Study of perceptual balance for binaural dichotic presentation

ABSTRACT EQUALIZING NOISE (TEN) TEST. Christine Gmitter Doctor of Audiology, 2008

Prescribe hearing aids to:

Charactering the individual ear by the Auditory Profile

Prediction of Loudness Growth in Subjects with Sensorineural Hearing Loss Using Auditory Steady State Response. Afaf A. Emara

A FRESH Approach to Pediatric Behavioral Testing

Loudness. Loudness is not simply sound intensity!

Clinical applications of otoacoustic emissions in Industry. Prof. Dr. B. Vinck, MSc, PhD University of Ghent, Belgium

FREQUENCY COMPRESSION AND FREQUENCY SHIFTING FOR THE HEARING IMPAIRED

Effects of Age and Hearing Loss on the Processing of Auditory Temporal Fine Structure

1706 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113 (3), March /2003/113(3)/1706/12/$ Acoustical Society of America

Intelligibility of narrow-band speech and its relation to auditory functions in hearing-impaired listeners

Spectrograms (revisited)

EEL 6586, Project - Hearing Aids algorithms

Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths

Hearing Lectures. Acoustics of Speech and Hearing. Auditory Lighthouse. Facts about Timbre. Analysis of Complex Sounds

Pitfalls in behavioral estimates of basilar-membrane compression in humans a)

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080

Speech intelligibility in simulated acoustic conditions for normal hearing and hearing-impaired listeners

David A. Nelson. Anna C. Schroder. and. Magdalena Wojtczak

A truly remarkable aspect of human hearing is the vast

Best Practice Protocols

Recovery from on- and off-frequency forward masking in listeners with normal and impaired hearing

Auditory model for the speech audiogram from audibility to intelligibility for words (work in progress)

Relation between speech intelligibility, temporal auditory perception, and auditory-evoked potentials

ID# Exam 2 PS 325, Fall 2003

Minimum Audible Angles Measured with Simulated Normally-Sized and Oversized Pinnas for Normal-Hearing and Hearing- Impaired Test Subjects

Estimates of compression at low and high frequencies using masking additivity in normal and impaired ears

Christopher J. Plack Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, E. Sussex BN1 9QG, England

Effects of Instantaneous Multiband Dynamic Compression on Speech Intelligibility

Determination of filtering parameters for dichotic-listening binaural hearing aids

PERIPHERAL AND CENTRAL AUDITORY ASSESSMENT

64 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING, VOL. 61, NO. 1, JANUARY 2014

The development of a modified spectral ripple test

Chapter 7 CONCLUSION AND RECOMMENDATIONS

Guidance on Identifying Non-Routine Cases of Hearing Loss

On the Interplay Between Cochlear Gain Loss and Temporal Envelope Coding Deficits

Data-driven approach for auditory profiling

Spectral-peak selection in spectral-shape discrimination by normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners

Temporal Masking Contributions of Inherent Envelope Fluctuations for Listeners with Normal and Impaired Hearing

Enrique A. Lopez-Poveda Alan R. Palmer Ray Meddis Editors. The Neurophysiological Bases of Auditory Perception

HEARING AND PSYCHOACOUSTICS

Power Instruments, Power sources: Trends and Drivers. Steve Armstrong September 2015

JSLHR. Research Article

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

Even though a large body of work exists on the detrimental effects. The Effect of Hearing Loss on Identification of Asynchronous Double Vowels

Christopher J. Plack and Ray Meddis Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester CO4 3SQ, United Kingdom

SoundRecover2 the first adaptive frequency compression algorithm More audibility of high frequency sounds

Paediatric Amplification

USING THE AUDITORY STEADY-STATE RESPONSE TO DIAGNOSE DEAD REGIONS IN THE COCHLEA. A thesis submitted to The University of Manchester for the degree of

Relating binaural pitch perception to the individual listener s auditory profile

Effects of background noise level on behavioral estimates of basilar-membrane compression

Sound localization psychophysics

INTRODUCTION. Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA Electronic mail:

COM3502/4502/6502 SPEECH PROCESSING

The Devil is in the Fitting Details. What they share in common 8/23/2012 NAL NL2

EVALUATION OF SPEECH PERCEPTION IN PATIENTS WITH SKI SLOPE HEARING LOSS USING ARABIC CONSTANT SPEECH DISCRIMINATION LISTS

Hearing in Noise Test in Subjects With Conductive Hearing Loss

Effects of partial masking for vehicle sounds

Time Varying Comb Filters to Reduce Spectral and Temporal Masking in Sensorineural Hearing Impairment

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Hear Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 December 04.

The role of periodicity in the perception of masked speech with simulated and real cochlear implants

Psychoacoustics. Author: Nejc Rosenstein. Advisor: Simon irca

Brad May, PhD Johns Hopkins University

Auditory Physiology PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 29. Hearing

PSY 310: Sensory and Perceptual Processes 1

Effects of flight on auditory function in pilots

Lecture 3: Perception

Testing Digital Hearing Aids

Tuning curves and pitch matches in a listener with a unilateral, low-frequency hearing loss Florentine, Mary; Houtsma, A.J.M.

New measurement methods for signal adaptive hearing aids

Audiometric Techniques Program in Audiology and Communication Sciences Pediatric Audiology Specialization

The functional importance of age-related differences in temporal processing

Bimodal listening or bilateral CI: When and why?

Relating binaural pitch perception to the individual listener's auditory profile

Transcription:

Chapter Categorical loudness scaling in hearing{impaired listeners Abstract Most sensorineural hearing{impaired subjects show the recruitment phenomenon, i.e., loudness functions grow at a higher rate than in normal{hearing subjects. In this chapter, the correlation between audiometric threshold shift and slope of loudness function of 67 sensorineural hearing{ impaired subjects is exammed. Loudness functions are measured using a one{step categorical scaling technique with 11 categories. It is shown that on average the slopes of loudness functions increase with increasing hearing loss but no strong correlation between audiometric threshold and slope of loudness functions is observed. Subjects with a similar amount of hearing loss sometimes show very dierent slopes in their loudness functions. A similar result has been reported for other psychoacoustic tasks like measurement of auditory lter bandwidth. No strong correlation has been found between audiometric threshold and lter bandwidth. Several sources, like dierent etiology or types of hearing losses, or shortcomings of the experimental techniques, might contribute to these large intersubject dierences..1 Introduction Growth{of{loudness functions provide an important clinical tool to characterize sensorineural hearing impairment. The one{step categorical scaling technique provides a clinically applicable means to determine these curves. The reliability and accuracy of this technique has been studied intensively (Kieling et al., 199; Hellbruck, 199; Kieling et al., 199; Boretzki et al., 199; Kollmeier and Hohmann, 1995). However, it is still unresolved whether this provides more information about sensorineural hearing{impaired subjects than a pure tone audiogram. If it does not contain more information, then a strong correlation should exist between audiometric thresholds and the results of loudness scaling, e.g., the slopes of loudness functions. Hence, the slopes of the loudness functions should be predictable from the pure tone audiogram. Thus loudness scaling would be dispensable in -5-

. Method routine audiometry and the selection and tting of hearing aids. In addition, sensorineural hearing loss could be modelled in a straightforward way with few parameters. To test this hypothesis, the correlation between audiometric threshold and slope of loudness function is investigated in the following section.. Method The loudness functions of 67 sensorineural hearing{impaired subjects have been measured by V. Hohmann and I. Holube. Parts of these results have been presented elsewhere (Hohmann, 199; Kollmeier and Hohmann, 1995). However, the correlation between audiometric threshold and slope of loudness function has not yet been investigated for the whole data base. Subjects In the experiments, 67 sensorineural hearing{impaired subjects participated voluntarily. Sensorineural hearing impairment was diagnosed by routine audiometry in the ENT Department of the University Clinic of Gottingen. The air{bone gap of the hearing{impaired listeners was always below 6 db. Stimuli Narrowband frozen noises centered at the audiological frequencies, 5, 5, 1,, and 6 Hz, are employed as stimuli. Their bandwidth was one third octave. The signals were generated o{line. Initally, a Gaussian white noise (duration s) was generated at a sampling rate of 5 khz. It was Fourier{transformed and bandpass ltered at the respective center frequency and with the respective bandwidth. After transforming these signals back to the time domain, they were windowed with a rectangular window ( s duration including 5 ms cosine{ramps). Procedure During the experiments the subjects were seated in a sound{attenuating chamber. The signals were presented monaurally via headphones (BeyerDynamic DT 8) to the subjects. Their task was to judge perceived loudness in a one{step procedure using a categorical scale with 11 categories. Figure 5. shows the dierent categories available to the subjects during the loudness scaling procedure. Subjects' responses were recorded using a handheld touch screen (Epson ETH{1) connected to a PC via a serial port (RS). This screen has a touch{sensitive display on which written instructions or dierent scales can be presented to the subjects. The categories were displayed on the screen after each presentation of the stimulus. The column at the right side of Fig. 5. shows the numbers assigned to the dierent categories for evaluating the data. These numbers were not displayed on the screen during the presentation of the categories. Each noise band was presented at eight levels equispaced on a db scale covering the entire dynamic range dened by audiometric threshold and uncomfortable loudness level (UCL). -6-

. Categorical loudness scaling in hearing{impaired listeners In a rst experiment, audiometric thresholds and individual UCLs were determined by presenting each stimulus to the subjects at ascending levels starting 1 db below standard pure tone thresholds. Subjects had to indicate when they heard the stimulus for the rst time, dening threshold, and when the loudness of the stimulus fell in the category \too loud", dening UCL. In the second experiment, the stimuli were presented in random order (with a restricted stepsize). Each stimulus was presented twice during one run. The experimental parameters were chosen according to the results of a joint research project (Kollmeier and Hohmann, 1995; Kieling et al., 199; Kieling et al., 199; Hohmann, 199).. Results and discussion Linear functions with the parameters slope m and intermediate loudness level 1 L 5 were tted to the measured loudness functions. Thus a linear relationship of the form CU = m (L, L 5 )5 (.1) was obtained between loudness in categorical units (CU) and sound pressure level L on a db scale. Figure.1 shows atypical loudness function for normal{hearing listeners and a function for one hearing{impaired subject. The loudness function, i.e., loudness in categorical units versus level, is plotted for six dierent center frequencies. For the loudness curves of normal{hearing subjects, linear functions tted to the average data of 9 subjects are plotted. Obviously, linear functions provide a good description of the loudness functions for hearing{impaired subjects when applying this scaling technique. Furthermore, the well known recruitment phenomenon can be observed: In regions with normal audiometric thresholds (5 and 5 Hz) growth of loudness is normal, while at frequencies with increased threshold (1 { 6 Hz), an increase in rate of growth of loudness is observed. In Fig.. the slopes of the loudness functions of all dierent frequencies of all 67 hearing{ impaired subjects are plotted as a function of hearing loss. On average, the slope of the loudness function increases with increasing hearing loss, but a large intersubject variabilityis observed. Surprisingly,even for a large amount of audiometric threshold shift, quite shallow loudness functions sometimes occur. A similar result has been reported by Kieling et al. (199). For moderate to severe hearing losses, they also reported either no further increase in slope with increasing hearing losses above a certain amount of hearing loss or even quite shallow loudness functions. Since only subjects with sloping high frequency hearing losses participated in their study, shallow loudness functions combined with signicant hearing loss occured mainly at high frequencies. Kieling et al. speculated that a neural component of hearing loss could be the origin of these results. Two further explanations could be proposed to explain both our ndings and the results of Kieling et al. Firstly, recruitment could be explained as being caused by abnormal growth 1 The level of the category \intermediate" = 5 in categorical units. -7-

. Results and discussion 5 Hz, right 5 1 1 Hz, right 5 1 Hz, right 5 1 5 Hz, right 5 1 Hz, right 5 1 6 Hz, right 5 1 Fig..1: Comparison of the loudness scaling results for normal{hearing (average of nine subjects, ) and one hearing{impaired () subject (JC). For the loudness functions of the normal{hearing subject the tted linear functions are employed. At frequencies where the hearing{impaired subject has normal thresholds (5 { 5 Hz) the growth of loudness function is also normal, while at frequencies with increased threshold (1 { 6 Hz) the slope of the growth of loudness functions increase with increasing threshold, i.e., recruitment occurs. The loudness curves are measured using the one{step categorical scaling technique with 11 categories. -8-

. Categorical loudness scaling in hearing{impaired listeners of spread of excitation (Evans, 1975). Thus, by restricting spread of excitation, the growth of loudness with level should be reduced. Hellman (199) measured growth of loudness in subjects with a very steeply sloping hearing loss. At the \cut{o" frequency, i.e., the frequency where hearing loss begins, measured loudness functions increased at a lower rate than at frequencies with normal thresholds. She explained the results as being caused by a restricted increase in spread of excitation due to the very steeply increasing hearing loss. Similarily, at high frequencies and in cases of highly elevated thresholds, spread of excitation could also be restricted, yielding either shallow loudness functions or at least no further increase in slope of loudness function with increasing hearing loss..5 Slope.5 1.5 1.5 6 8 1 Audiometric Threshold / db HL Fig..: Slopes of loudness functions of 67 sensorineural hearing{impaired subjects as a function of audiometric threshold. The slopes were measured with categorial scaling technique. All subjects suered from a sensorineural hearing loss. Note that, even for large hearing losses, quite shallow loudness functions occur. Dierent symbols represent dierent center frequencies of the narrowband noises. The solid curve represents the regression curve. :.5 khz, :.5 khz, : 1 khz, : khz, : khz. The second explanation is based on dierent types of damage to the Organ of Corti and altered basilar membrane mechanics as was described in chapter. If the hearing loss exceeds about 6 db, most OHCs are probably damaged. Thus, further increases in audiometric threshold would be due to damage to IHCs only, yielding no further steepening of loudness functions. If the hearing loss is further increased, large parts of the IHC population are -9-

. Results and discussion probably damaged, yielding shallow loudness functions. Furthermore, strong damage to IHCs yields much less input via aerent bers to higher stages of the auditory pathway resulting in a degeneration of these stages and thus perhaps causing a neural component of hearing loss as was suggested by Kieling et al. (199). In order to help clarify this point, the question has to be addressed whether a large audiometric threshold shift combined with a shallow loudness function only occurs at high frequencies. Therefore, the data given in Fig.. are plotted with dierent symbols for dierent center frequencies. Obviously, the lack of correlation between hearing loss and slope of loudness functions the same for all dierent center frequencies, since the variability is similar, regardless of frequency and even at very low frequencies. The correlation coecient across all frequencies between the audiometric thresholds and the slopes of loudness functions was.65, p<:1 (Spearman's rank correlation coecient (Sachs, 199)). Thus, a highly signicant but only moderate correlation exists. The intersubject variability also increases strongly with increasing absolute threshold causing a larger scatter for severe hearing losses than for low to medium hearing losses. Thus, due to the restriction of the slopes to \normal" values (i.e., lower boundary) and the increase in variance with increasing absolute threshold, the distribution of slopes expands solely towards greater values yielding an increase in the mean slope. A similar relation between audiometric threshold and an auditory parameter can also be observed in a number of other psychoacoustical experiments. Kinkel (199), Kollmeier (199) and Holube (199) have demonstrated this for a battery of auditory tests including monaural and binaural temporal and spectral resolution as well as dierent speech tests in quiet and noise. Stone (199), Moore (1995) and Hetu and Tran{Quoc (1995) also reported that no strong correlation between audiometric threshold and auditory lter bandwidth exists. In Fig.. auditory lter bandwidth is plotted as a function of audiometric threshold. The lter bandwidths have been measured by Moore, Peters and Stone (Stone et al., 199; Stone, 199; Moore, 1995) with a notched{noise technique. Obviously the scatter in this gure is similar to that of Fig... Again, the variability of lter bandwidth is about the same for all frequencies. A similar nding was also reported byhetu and Tran{Quoc (1995). In summary, no strong correlation between audiometric threshold shift and alteration in dierent psychoacoustic tasks can be found. Why is the psychoacoustic performance of sensorineural hearing{impaired listeners so variable for a given amount of hearing loss This variability could be caused by several factors: -- 1. It could be simply due to \measurement errors". However this is highly improbable because the same scatter occurs for all kinds of measurement techniques and psychoacoustical tasks: Categorical scaling (this study, Kieling et al. (199)) and magnitude estimation (Hellman (199) Fig. 1.6 and Hellman and Meiselman (199) Fig. 8) for loudness measurement; notched{noise (Stone, 199; Hetu and Tran-Quoc, 1995), rippled{noise and psychoacoustic tuning curves (Tyler, 1986; Moore, 1995) for the measurement of auditory lter bandwidth; monaural and binaural temporal resolution and speech tests (Kinkel, 199; Kollmeier, 199; Holube, 199).

. Categorical loudness scaling in hearing{impaired listeners 5 f Imp =f Norm 1 1 5 6 7 Audiometric Threshold / db HL Fig..: Correlation between bandwidth of auditory lters and audiometric threshold. Filter bandwidths f are plotted as the ratio of impaired f Imp and normal f Norm bandwidth. The solid line represents the increase of auditory lters bandwidth with level in normal{hearing subjects. Filter bandwidths were measured with a notched{noise technique. All subjects suered from a pure sensorineural hearing loss. Note that even for large amount of hearing losses quite normal lter bandwidths occur. Dierent symbols represent dierent frequencies. Adapted from Stone (199), his Fig. 11.8. :. khz, :.5 khz,.8 khz, : 1. khz :.khz,:. khz. By courtesy of M. Stone.. Another source of variability could be a dierent etiology of hearing loss, i.e., dierent origins of hearing loss like noise, ototoxic drugs or hereditary hearing losses. However, the same scatter occurs if subjects have a similar etiology (for instance noise{induced hearing loss): Hellman and Meiselman (199) for loudness perception; Laroche et al. (199), Hetu and Tran{Quoc (1995), for the measurement of auditory lter bandwidth.. The diagnostic procedure for sensorineural hearing impairment allows for a dierence between air conduction and bone conduction thresholds of up to 1 db for diagnosing a pure sensorineural hearing loss. However, the dierence between air conduction and bone conduction thresholds of the subjects employed in this study was always below 6 db.. In chapter another source of variability has been discussed: dierent damage pattern in the Organ of Corti. Psychoacoustical performance might strongly depend on which cells are damaged in the Organ of Corti. Loss of IHCs is assumed to solely cause a -1-

. Conclusions reduced sensitivity while loss of OHCs probably causes both, a reduced sensitivity and a loss of the nonlinear cochlear processing. Thus, two dierent components of sensorineural hearing loss might be distinguished: a reduction in sensitivity (\sensitivity loss") and a reduction in compressive processing (\compression loss") Unfortunately, at present there are no diagnostic techniques for distinguishing between these two components of cochlear damage. Therefore, probably there is not one single cause of the large variability observed, but it is a combination of the above mentioned dierent causes. However, the dierent mechanical properties discussed as the last cause appears to explain the eect of sensorineural hearing loss in an appealing and promising way.. Conclusions The results indicate that usually the slopes of loudness functions increase with increasing audiometric threshold, but large intersubject dierences are observed. These dierences also increase, regardless of frequency, with increasing hearing loss. A similar scatter is observed for other psychoacoustical tasks like measurement of auditory lter bandwidth or temporal resolution. Neither dierent etiology nor shortcomings of the experimental techniques appear to account for the variability. Among the possible causes the most appealing is that the variability commonly observed might be due to dierent contributions of two dierent components of sensorineural hearing impairment, i.e., loss of sensitivity and loss of compression. --