University of Warwick institutional repository: This paper is made available online in accordance with publisher

Similar documents
Empathizing and systemizing cognitive traits in the sciences and humanities

The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) in Japan: A Cross-Cultural Comparison

Cognitive styles sex the brain, compete neurally, and quantify deficits in autism

4 Detailed Information Processing in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome: The Usefulness of Neuropsychological Tests and Self-reports

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Which assessment tool is most useful to diagnose adult autism spectrum disorder?

Supporting Online Material for

Foetal testosterone and autistic traits

JDiBrief Security Visual Inspection with X-rays: SUMMARY (1 of 5)

Local and global processing in savant artists with autism. Linda Pring, Nicola Ryder, Laura Crane and Beate Hermelin

University of Warwick institutional repository:

Cognitive style predicts entry into physical sciences and humanities: Questionnaire and performance tests of empathy and systemizing

HOW AUTISTIC ARE ANORECTIC FEMALES? SIMILARITIES AND DIFFERENCES BETWEEN ANOREXIA NERVOSA AND AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS

PATTERNS OF INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN REDUCTION: IMPLICATIONS FOR LISTENER-ORIENTED THEORIES

Loneliness, social relationships, and a broader autism phenotype in college students

The short forms of the Empathy Quotient (EQ-short) and the Systemizing Quotient (SQ-short)

High-Functioning Autism & Asperger Syndrome in Girls. Prevalence of ASD. Gender Gap. Late Diagnosis

Individual Differences in (Non-Visual) Processing Style Predict the Face Inversion Effect

Brief Report: Development of the Adolescent Empathy and Systemizing Quotients

Are people with Intellectual disabilities getting more or less intelligent II: US data. Simon Whitaker

5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome

The broader language phenotype of autism: a comparison with specific language impairment

An exploration of autistic children s ability to recognise emotions in single word and sentence language conditions

Does central coherence relate to the cognitive performance of children with autism in dynamic assessments?

Grace Iarocci Ph.D., R. Psych., Associate Professor of Psychology Simon Fraser University

Effects of autistic traits and context use on social cognitions a mediation analysis.

The merits of mental age as an additional measure of intellectual ability in the low ability range. Simon Whitaker

Lecture 6: The Whorfian Hypothesis (contd.); autism spectrum disorders and language

Central Coherence and Theory of Mind in children and adolescents. with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Original Article. Broader Autism Phenotype in Iranian Parents of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders vs. Normal Children

Social Cognition: What Is It and What Does It Tell Us About How to Teach? Kari Dunn Buron.

WISC-IV Profiles in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Karen Stack, Dr. Raegan Murphy, Paula Prendeville and Dr.

To Match or Not To Match? Methodological Issues in Autism-Related Research

Supplementary Information. Enhancing studies of the connectome in autism using the Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange II

Effects of severe depression on TOMM performance among disability-seeking outpatients

Autism Spectrum Disorder. Aims for Session 3 03/04/2018. Neurodevelopmental Disorders Professor Pamela Heaton. Session 3

DOWNLOAD OR READ : THE AUTISM SPECTRUM GUIDE TO SEXUALITY AND RELATIONSHIPS PDF EBOOK EPUB MOBI

A Comparison of Autistic Like Traits in the Relatives of Patients with Autism and Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorder

ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES ( ) PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology

Enhanced Visual Search for a Conjunctive Target in Autism: A Research Note

URL: < >

Introduction to Psychology. Lecture 34

3 CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATIONS OF STATISTICS

Children s Communication Checklist - 2: a validation study

The effect of social stories on reduction of challenging behaviours in autistic children

(Received 30 March 1990)

The links between Autistic traits, Theory of Mind and Moral Judgement.

Eye-tracking brings focus to 'theory of mind'

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

MODULE SPECIFICATION. Part 1: Basic Data. Module Title Autism Spectrum and Communication Skills Module Code UTTGT Level 2 Version 1

The effect of little bird program in decreasing problem behaviors of autistic children

Supplementary Online Content 2

Misunderstood Girls: A look at gender differences in Autism

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

(This is a sample cover image for this issue. The actual cover is not yet available at this time.)

WHAT IS AUTISM? Chapter One

The Effect of Perspective on Misconceptions in Psychology: A Test of Conceptual Change Theory 1. Eric Amsel. Department of Psychology

COGNITIVE FACTORS IN EPILEPSY BY MARGARET DAVIES-EYSENCK

RESEARCH PROJECT SUBJECT TITLE:

Delusions of Gender: The Real Science Behind Sex Differences

Temporal Context and the Recognition of Emotion from Facial Expression

Spatial cognition and emotion recognition as a function of ASD

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children

AUTISM: THEORY OF MIND. Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD

Perspectives on Autism and Sexuality. University of British Columbia. Research Article Summaries. Tina Gunn

Analysis of Confidence Rating Pilot Data: Executive Summary for the UKCAT Board

Analysis of new diagnostic criteria for autism sparks debate

Forty-year Secular Trends in Cognitive Abilities

Reliability and Validity of the Divided

Abstract. Methods. Participants. Authors. Alexander Milovanov, 1 Melissa Paquette-Smith, 1 Yona Lunsky, 1 Jonathan Weiss 2. Correspondence.

Psychometric Properties and Overlap of the GSQ and AQ among Japanese University Students

University of Huddersfield Repository

COGMED CLINICAL EVALUATION SERIES

Ambiguous Data Result in Ambiguous Conclusions: A Reply to Charles T. Tart

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755

Autism Spectrum Disorders. A general overview

Is Empathizing intuitive and Systemizing deliberative? Konstantina Antoniadou, Maastricht University, 6200 MD Maastricht, Netherlands.

Brief Report: Schema consistent misinformation effects in eyewitnesses with. autism spectrum disorder. Katie Maras.

Extraversion and Neuroticism in Contact Athletes, No Contact Athletes and Non-athletes: A Research Note

Brief Report: Prompted Pretend Play in Autism1

Views of autistic adults on assessment in the early years

University of Southampton. Doctoral Programme in Educational Psychology

Broader Autism Phenotype and Nonverbal Sensitivity: Evidence for an Association in the General Population

Empirical approaches to family assessment. Journal of Family Therapy, 22 (2):

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION

The Short NART: Cross-validation, relationship to IQ and some practical considerations

Everything DiSC Manual

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions

Modality and Contextual Differences in Computer Based Non-verbal Communication Training

Pathways to Inflated Responsibility Beliefs in Adolescent Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: A Preliminary Investigation

Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Comprehension and Story Retelling. Abilities among Children with ASD. Compared to Children with Typical. Development

Personality and Individual Differences

Intelligence What is intelligence? Intelligence Tests and Testing

Intelligence. PSYCHOLOGY (8th Edition) David Myers. Intelligence. Chapter 11. What is Intelligence?

Peer Perception in Autism. Kathryn McVicar, MD Assistant Professor Clinical Pediatrics and Neurology Albert Einstien College of Medicine

Running head: INFLUENCE OF LABELS ON JUDGMENTS OF PERFORMANCE

VALIDITY STUDIES WITH A TEST OF HIGH-LEVEL REASONING

Processing demands in belief-desire reasoning: inhibition or general difficulty?

Transcription:

University of Warwick institutional repository: http://go.warwick.ac.uk/wrap This paper is made available online in accordance with publisher policies. Please scroll down to view the document itself. Please refer to the repository record for this item and our policy information available from the repository home page for further information. To see the final version of this paper please visit the publisher s website. Access to the published version may require a subscription. Author(s): Julia M. Carroll and Kin Yung Chiew Article Title: Sex and Discipline Differences in Empathising, Systemising and Autistic Symptomatology: Evidence from a Student Population Year of publication: 2006 Link to published version: http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1007/s10803-006-0127-9 Publisher statement: The original publication is available at www.springerlink.com

Sex and discipline differences 1 Running Head: Sex differences in autistic like characteristics Sex and discipline differences in empathising, systemising and autistic symptomatology: Evidence from a student population Julia M. Carroll and Chiew Kin Yung University of York, York, UK Author Note: Julia M. Carroll, Department of Psychology, University of York, York, YO10 5DD, UK; Chiew Kin Yung, Department of Psychology, University of York, York, YO10 5DD, UK. Julia M. Carroll is now at Department of Psychology, University of Warwick, Coventry, CV4 7AL, UK. Chiew Kin Yung is now at Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol, 8 Woodland Road, Bristol, BS8 1TN. We thank Professor Maggie Snowling for her helpful comments on an earlier draft of this paper, and the students who took part in the research. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Julia M. Carroll, Department of Psychology, University of Warwick, Coventry, CV4 7AL, UK. Tel: 02476 523 613; Email: J.M.Carroll@warwick.ac.uk; Fax: 02476 524 225.

Sex and discipline differences 2 Abstract Baron Cohenʹs (2002) theory of autism suggests that deficits in theory of mind and weak central coherence in autism can be explained as features of an extreme male brain in which empathising is weak and systemising is strong. The two studies presented investigate this theory by examining the relationships between theory of mind, central coherence, empathising, systemising and autistic like symptomatology in a sample of undergraduates. Study 1 used 48 undergraduates in four equal groups of male and female science and humanities students. Consistent with the theory, there were sex differences in the expected directions on all tasks in the first study. Differences according to discipline were found only on the Block Design task. Individuals with the male brain profile also tended to show higher levels of autistic symptomatology. There was no evidence of a link between empathising and social skills on one hand and systemising and central coherence on the other. In the second study, performance on the Mechanical Reasoning and the Social Skills Inventory tasks was compared with performance on the Baron Cohen Empathising and Systemising Quotients in a sub sample of 20 students from Study 1. Moderately significant correlations were found between the Systemising Quotient and the Mechanical Reasoning task and between the Empathising Quotient and the Social Skills Inventory. Findings are largely consistent with the distinction between empathising and systemising but raise some questions concerning the tasks used to measure these abilities.

Sex and discipline differences 3 Sex and discipline differences in empathising, systemising and autistic symptomatology: Evidence from a student population For many years, there have been two major theoretical explanations for the cognitive deficits shown in autism. The Theory of Mind explanation suggests that individuals with autism have reduced understanding of the mental states of others, leading to delayed use of language, social withdrawal and a lack of pretence in play (Baron Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 1985; Frith, 2003). In contrast, the Weak Central Coherence theory suggests that individuals have a cognitive bias towards processing information in a piecemeal, rather than a global manner (Frith & Happe, 1994). This would lead to strengths on tasks in which the gestalt of a picture has to be ignored, as in the block design task used in many intelligence tests. More recently, (Baron Cohen, 2002, 2003) has suggested that deficits in both of these areas could be explained as an extreme variation of the sex differences shown in processing in the general population. This is known as the Extreme Male Brain theory of autism. This theory is an extension of Baron Cohen s empathising systemising model. According to this model females are, on average, more likely to show high social awareness or empathising, and low understanding of systems or systemising, while males are more likely to show the reverse pattern. According to Baron Cohen (2003), these differences can largely be attributed to biological processes such as pre natal testosterone levels.

Sex and discipline differences 4 Extreme Male Brain theory extends this idea by arguing that individuals with autism show an extreme example of these sex differences. In the Extreme Male Brain theory, therefore, weak central coherence is regarded as an element of strong systemising, or working out of logical processes, while poor theory of mind would be associated with poor empathising, or a lack of understanding of and appropriate responses to the mental states of others. Individuals with autism could be considered to be at the extremes of the normal distribution in two respects, showing unusually good systemising and unusually weak empathising. If Extreme Male Brain theory was found to be correct, it would be a parsimonious explanation for the range of deficits found in autism: alone, neither theory of mind nor weak central coherence can provide an explanation for all of the behaviours typically associated with autism. For instance, it is not immediately clear why a child who lacks a theory of mind would show repetitive and stereotyped play patterns, or why a child with weak central coherence would show such highly impaired communication. There are three major claims to Baron Cohen s Extreme Male Brain theory. Evidence for each of these three claims is assessed below. First, that there are reliable sex differences both in tasks measuring empathising and systemising and in tasks that separate autistic individuals from the general population, namely theory of mind and central coherence tasks. Second, that strengths and weaknesses that characterise autistic individuals map onto the two major areas of sex differences that he describes: empathising and systemising. Third, as Jarrold, Butler, Cottingham and Jimenez, (2000) suggest, these two areas of skill should be negatively correlated, so

Sex and discipline differences 5 that individuals with high systemising abilities would tend to show low levels of empathising, and vice versa. It is not clear, however, that Baron Cohen assumes that empathising and systemising would be correlated in the general population. In an early review he refers to systemising and empathising as being on a continuum (p. 210, (Baron Cohen & Hammer, 1997, italics added). In addition, he suggests that a single biological factor (pre natal testosterone secretion) could cause both increased systemising and decreased empathising. If this were the case, one would expect the two factors to be highly associated. On the other hand, more recent work by Baron Cohen and colleagues suggests the opposite; in Lawson, Baron Cohen and Wheelwright (2004) the empathising and systemising measures used show no significant correlation and are presented as two independent areas of functioning. However, if the two areas are independent, it is difficult to see how linking the empathising systemising theory to the deficits found in autism increases explanatory power in comparison to previous theories. There is a growing body of evidence showing that sex differences in empathising and systemising are real. For instance, Voyer, Voyer and Bryden (1995) conducted a meta analysis examining sex differences on spatial tasks. While the magnitude of the difference varied according to the task presented, there was a consistent male advantage across tests. With regard to empathising, Hoffman (1977) found that females showed higher levels of empathy across a range of studies. There is also evidence suggesting that there are sex differences in tasks characteristically used to identify autistic individuals. Baron Cohen, Wheelwright,

Sex and discipline differences 6 Hill, Raste and Plumb (2001) asked 15 men and 15 women to complete the Mind in the Eyes test (assumed to be a measure of theory of mind ability) and the Embedded Figures Test (EFT; Witkin, Oltman, Raskin, & Karp, 1971). The men in the group showed worse scores on the Mind in the Eyes test and better scores on the EFT than the women, suggesting that men do indeed show a mild version of the strengths and weaknesses shown in autism, though sex differences on a revised version of the Mind in the Eyes test were only marginally significant (Baron Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill et al., 2001). Jarrold et al. (2000) conducted a detailed investigation of the claims of the Extreme Male Brain theory of autism. They investigated performance on standard theory of mind and embedded figures tasks in typically developing children and adults, as well as a group of children with autism. They did not find clear evidence for sex differences in these two tasks, showing no difference between groups of male and female students on the unrevised Mind in the Eyes test and a marginally significant difference on the EFT. They did, however, find a moderately significant correlation between performances on the two tasks, such that individuals with lower scores on the eye reading task showed shorter completion times on the embedded figures task. These findings were replicated in the two other groups: a group of typically developing 5 year old children and a group of children with autism, though the associations only became significant once verbal mental age was controlled. Other researchers have not shown such clear correlations between these two diagnostic features of autism. Lawson et al (2004) investigated performance on two

Sex and discipline differences 7 tasks aiming to measure empathising and systemising abilities, the Social Stories Questionnaire and the Physical Prediction Questionnaire. Their sample contained adult males and females together with a group of adult males with Asperger s Syndrome (AS). They found that there were differences between all groups on the Social Stories Questionnaire, with females scoring most highly, non AS males in the middle and AS males having the most difficulty. On the Physical Prediction Questionnaire, results were more equivocal, with AS and non AS males scoring at the same level, while the females did less well. As described above, there were no significant correlations between scores on the two tasks in any group. Briskman, Happe, & Frith (2001) gave questionnaires examining the presence of social and non social autistic characteristics in autistic children and their parents. Significant correlations between scores on the social and non social elements of the questionnaire were shown for parents of autistic children but not for parents of dyslexic or control group children, suggesting that any link between the two areas is not generally present in the population. Recent research indicates that social and non social autistic characteristics may be independent in preschool children on the autistic spectrum (Morgan, Maybery, & Durkin, 2003). While the children showed deficits in joint attention and language and weak central coherence compared to controls, deficits in each of these areas contributed independent variance when predicting the autistic diagnosis. An alternative way of examining the theory of links between empathising and systemising and autistic symptomatology is to consider whether students who have

Sex and discipline differences 8 chosen to study a subject that requires high levels of systemising show correspondingly high levels of autistic symptomatology. Baron Cohen, Wheelwright, Stott, Bolton and Goodyer, (1997) found that engineers were twice as common in the families of autistic children as in the families of control children. Conversely, Baron Cohen et al., (1998) found that the incidence of autism was significantly increased in families of science students in comparison to humanities students. The present study extends this research by assessing the presence of autistic like symptomatology within students themselves. A further theoretical issue concerns the validity of the Systemising Quotient questionnaire normally used by Baron Cohen that assesses the interests and preferences by asking participants to rate how closely statements such as I am fascinated by how machines work apply to them. It could be argued that assessing preferences is very different from assessing abilities: a person may be interested in how machines work, but show no particular aptitude for building or even understanding how they work. This question is addressed in Study 2, in which a subgroup of participants completed both a practical measure of Mechanical Reasoning and the Systemising Quotient questionnaire, as well as the Empathising Quotient, to assess the relationships between the different tasks. Study 1 The present study aims to build on the work carried out by Jarrold et al. (2000) and Lawson et al. (2004) by assessing the relationships among measures of

Sex and discipline differences 9 empathising, systematising, theory of mind, central coherence and autistic like symptomatology in a single study. Male and female science and humanities students were asked to complete this battery of tests. It was predicted that there would be good correlations between the tasks purported to measure similar underlying skills, so that empathising would be correlated with theory of mind and systemising would be correlated with weak central coherence. It was also anticipated that there would be sex and discipline differences in all of the measures, with females and humanities students showing better theory of mind and empathising and lower scores on the Block Design and EFT. Females should also show fewer autistic like symptoms. Finally, we anticipated correlations between the two sets of measures, such that high empathising and theory of mind would be associated with slow and inaccurate performance on the systemising and central coherence tasks. Method Participants The 48 participants were students of the University of York (41 undergraduates and 7 postgraduates). There were 4 equal groups of 12 participants. Group M S comprised male students (N = 12; mean age 21.1 years) studying science. Group M H comprised male students (N = 12; mean age 20.6 years) studying humanities. Group F S comprised female students (N = 12; mean age 21.6 years) studying science. Group F H comprised female students (N = 12; mean age 20.9 years) studying humanities. All participants were British natives with English as

Sex and discipline differences 10 their first language. There were no significant differences between the groups in age (sex: F(1, 44) = 1.20, p = ns, η 2 =.03; discipline: F (1, 44) = 2.38, p = ns, η 2 =.05) or verbal IQ (sex: F(1, 44) = 2.02, p = ns, η 2 =.04; discipline: F(1, 44) = 3.16, p =.08, η 2 =.07). There were no significant interactions. Materials Verbal IQ: Participants verbal IQ was assessed with Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence Vocabulary subtest (WASI; Wechsler, 1999). The test requires participants to provide definitions for words. The task is discontinued after five consecutive scores of zero. Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ; Baron Cohen, Wheelwright, Skinner, Martin, & Clubley, 2001): AQ measures the degree to which any adult of normal IQ possesses traits related to the autistic spectrum by means of a 50 item self report questionnaire. The questionnaire comprises 5 subscales of 10 items each, measuring social skill, attention switching, attention to detail, communication, and imagination. Advanced Theory of Mind test: The revised Mind in the Eyes test was used (Baron Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill et al., 2001). This paper and pencil test included 36 photographs of the eye region of the face of different actors and actresses. In addition to determining the sex of the face as a control task, participants were required to choose which of the four words best described the mental state of the person in the photograph. The test aims to measure individual s ability to put themselves into the mind of another. There are 36 possible marks for each scale. All participants scored at least 32 out of 36 on the sex recognition control task.

Sex and discipline differences 11 Central Coherence: Central Coherence Bias was assessed with both the WASI Block Design subtest (Wechsler, 1999) and the EFT (Witkin et al., 1971). The Block Design subtest involves arranging a specific number of blocks to replicate two coloured designs, either modelled or printed two dimensional geometric patterns, within a specified time limit. The EFT consisted of 12 trials. In each trial, the participant was first shown a card presenting a complex figure for 15 seconds. Next the participant was shown a card showing the target shape for 10 seconds. The time taken to identify the target shape within the previous complex figure was measured. In contrast to the procedure detailed in the scoring manual, trials on which the participant did not find the figure were excluded from the total. No participant made more than three errors and 38 of the 48 participants made one or no errors. Systemizing: Systemizing was assessed with the last 20 items of the Mechanical Reasoning subtest of the Differential Aptitude Test (Bennett, Seashore, & Wesman, 1974). The multiple choice test consisted of items assessing mechanical/technical systems. This involved problems such as reasoning about the relative movements of different wheels and pulleys in a diagram. It is similar in form to the Physical Prediction Questionnaire used by Lawson et al (2004). This type of measure differs from the Systemising Quotient used by Baron Cohen (2003) in that there is a concentration on understanding and abilities rather than preferences. There was a total time allocation of 10 minutes. Each correct item earned a score of 1 point, which added up for a total of 20 possible points for the task. Empathizing: Social Skills Inventory (Riggio, 1989) is a 90 item self report questionnaire assessing basic social communication skills. The questionnaire consisted of

Sex and discipline differences 12 six subscales: Emotional Expressivity, Emotional Sensitivity, Emotional Control, Social Expressivity, Social Sensitivity, and Social Control. Every subscale consisted of 15 items measured on 5 point Likert scales, with 5 representing Exactly like me while 1 representing Not at all like me. There were 75 possible points for every subscale, and 450 points in total. Procedure The experiment involved a 55 minute one to one session that required the participant to complete five cognitive tasks; the WASI Vocabulary subtest, WASI Block Design subtest, the Mind in the Eyes task, the modified Mechanical Reasoning test, and the EFT in that order. Participants completed the Social Skills Inventory and AQ in their own time, and returned the questionnaires to the experimenter. Results and Discussion Sex and Discipline Differences Table 1 provides the summary descriptive statistics for all of the measures. A MANOVA was carried out on the six measures of interest. Two independent factors of two levels each were investigated: sex (male or female), and discipline (science or humanities). Overall, there was a significant effect of sex (F(6, 39) = 6.47, p <.001, η 2 =.50), but not of discipline (F(6, 39) = 1.75, p = ns, η 2 =.21), and these effects were modified by a significant interaction (F(6, 39) = 2.43, p <.05, η 2 =.27).

Sex and discipline differences 13 Results on individual measures were then examined. For AQ scores, statistical analysis revealed a main effect of sex (F (1, 44) = 5.93, p <.05, η 2 =.12) but not of discipline (F(1, 44) <1). The Sex x Discipline interaction was not significant (F (1, 44) = 3.31, p =.08, η 2 =.07). Men had significantly higher AQ scores than did the women participants (mean 19.08 (SD 5.96) vs. 15.13 (SD 5.5)), whereas AQ scores did not differ between science students and humanities students (mean 17.08 (SD 4.7) vs. 17.13 (SD 7.16)). On examination of the individual scores, it was found that the three students with the highest scores on the AQ were all male humanities students, which may account for the marginally significant interaction between sex and discipline. Mind in the Eyes performance yielded a main effect of sex (F (1, 44) = 6.74, p <.05, η 2 =.13) and a marginally significant effect of discipline (F (1, 44) = 3.51, p <.07, η 2 =.07). Men scored significantly lower than females on the Mind in the Eyes test (mean 24.8 (SD 4.6) vs. 27.8 (SD 3.6)), whereas science students were slightly poorer than humanities students (mean 25.2 (SD 4.23) vs. 27.4 (SD 4.2)). There was no significant Sex x Discipline interaction (F(1, 44) <1). Main effects of sex and discipline on the Block Design subtest were both significant (sex, F (1, 44) = 9.17, p <.05, η 2 =.17; discipline, F (1, 44) = 5.68, p <.05, η 2 =.11). On average, men scored higher than women (mean 64.0 (SD 6.7) vs. mean 58.9 (SD 5.4)), while science students scored higher than humanities students (mean 63.5 (SD 6.3) vs. mean 59.5 (SD 6.2)). The Sex x Discipline interaction was not significant (F (1, 44) <1).

Sex and discipline differences 14 Mean completion time on the EFT showed a significant main effect of sex (F(1, 44) = 4.62, p <.05, η 2 =.10) but not of discipline, F (1, 44) = 3.13, p =.08, η 2 =.07). There was also a significant interaction, F(1, 44) = 8.86, p <.01, η 2 =.17). Female humanities students showed higher mean completion times than the other three groups (24.0s vs. 15.8s, 13.3s and 14.1s respectively). As predicted in Baron Cohen s (2002) Empathising Systemising theory, independent analysis of Social Skills and Mechanical Reasoning scores revealed strong main effects of sex (Social Skills Inventory: F (1, 44) = 9.75, p <.01, η 2 =.18; Mechanical Reasoning: F (1, 44) = 17.02, p <.01, η 2 =.28). Men were significantly lower on the Social Skills Inventory than women (mean 275.6 (22.6) vs. 294.8 (19.6)), while superior on the modified Mechanical Reasoning test (mean 13.04 (SD 2.9) vs. 9.8 (2.5)). There were no significant main effects of Discipline or Sex x Discipline interaction for either Social Skills Inventory or Mechanical Reasoning (F(1,44)<1 in all cases). Relationships among measures of theory of mind, central coherence, empathising, systemising and autistic symptomatology. Correlations between the different measures are shown in Table 2. Correlational analysis largely confirmed the findings of the factor analysis that measures of empathising and theory of mind tasks correlate with one another, as do measures of systemising and central coherence. Scores on the AQ were significantly associated with empathising and theory of mind, but not with Mechanical Reasoning or either of the two measures of central coherence.

Sex and discipline differences 15 A link between theory of mind and central coherence was not supported. Mind in the Eyes performance did not correlate with Block Design test scores (r (48) =.15, p = ns) or mean completion time on the EFT (r (48) =.05, p = ns). The correlation between Social Skills and Mechanical Reasoning scores did not reach significance (r (48) =.28, p =.06). Partial correlations controlling for chronological age and verbal IQ did not affect the relationship (r (44) =.28, p =.06). Male Brain Profile and the Broader Autism Phenotype. A further prediction of the experiment was that Baron Cohenʹs (2002) male brain profile would be associated with the autism spectrum manifestation, as measured on the AQ. To assess the tendency for participants to show the male brain profile, Empathising and Systemising scores were first converted into z scores. The participant s Brain Type was then identified by calculating for the discrepancy between the two sets of z scores: Systemising z score Empathising z score. A solution greater than 1.0 from the equation represented the male brain profile, while a solution less than 1.0 represented the female brain profile. A score between 1.0 and 1.0 represented a balanced brain profile. Table 3 shows the distribution of participants in each group along the three brain types. First inspection suggests that men were distributed towards the male brain profile, while women were more distributed towards the female brain profile. Chi square tests were used to assess the association between brain type and sex and discipline. There was a significant association between Sex and Brain Type (χ 2 (2) = 19.80, p <.001), but not between Discipline and Brain Type (χ 2 (2) =.38, p = ns). Thus, consistent with the Empathising Systemising

Sex and discipline differences 16 theory, men were more likely to have the male brain profile, whereas women were likely to have the female brain profile. Science students were not any more likely to be classified as having either the male or female brain profile than humanities students. Group differences between with reference to Baron Cohenʹs (2002) brain types were investigated using one way between subjects ANOVA with Mind in the Eyes performance and AQ scores as the dependent variables. The groups differed significantly on Mind in the Eyes performance (F(2,45) = 3.84, p <.05, η 2 =.15). Post hoc Tukey s analyses showed a significant difference between those with male and female brains (p<.05), but no other significant differences. There was no significant group difference in AQ scores (F (2, 45) = 2.24, p = ns, η 2 =.09). However, once a single outlier (a male humanities student) with an unusually high AQ score was excluded, the difference between the groups became significant (F(2,44) = 4.95, p<.05, η 2 =.18). Post hoc Tukey s HSD tests confirmed that those with female type brains had significantly lower AQ scores than those with male type brains (p <.05) and marginally lower than those with balanced brains (p =.06). Individuals with male brains and balanced brains did not differ significantly. Study 2 At the time of testing, Baron Cohen s Empathising and Systemising Quotient questionnaires had recently been published and have not, to our knowledge, previously been directly compared to standard tests within the literature. This study describes a pilot project in which different measures of Empathising and Systemising were compared. The Systemising Quotient is concerned with the likes and interests

Sex and discipline differences 17 of the participants, rather than their knowledge and abilities, containing items such as I do not enjoy games that involve a high degree of strategy. Scores on this test were analysed in relation to the Mechanical Reasoning test included in Study 1, in which participants had to determine the correct answer to a mechanical problem. It was anticipated that scores in these two areas would be correlated, but may be separable. A closer relationship might be predicted between the two measures of Empathising. The Empathising Quotient includes a few questions assessing basic social skills ( I can sense if I am intruding, even if the other person doesn t tell me ), but centres on assessment of a respondent s empathy in different situations ( I usually stay emotionally detached when watching a film ). Scores on this questionnaire were compared to scores on the Social Skills Inventory (Riggio, 1989), which measures basic social skills that underlie social competence. It contains six subscales which measure, respectively, emotional and social expressivity, sensitivity and control. Twenty students from Study 1 were asked to complete the Systemising and Empathising Quotient questionnaires and their scores on these tasks were compared to those on the tasks included in Study 1. A delay of approximately two months existed between completion of the first set of tasks and the completion of the Empathising and Systemising Quotient questionnaires.

Sex and discipline differences 18 Method Participants A subgroup of twenty students was created which included 5 male humanities students (group M H), 5 female humanities students (group F H), 5 male science students (group M S) and 5 female science students (group F S). Each of these students had taken part in Study 1. The mean ages and verbal IQs of the participants were as follows; group M H mean age: 21.1 years, verbal IQ: 140.40; group F H mean age: 21.41 years, verbal IQ: 140.40, group M S mean age: 21.40 years, verbal IQ: 132.40; group F S mean age: 22.27 years, verbal IQ: 136.00. Additional Materials In addition to the tasks described in Study 1 the students were asked to complete the following two tasks. Empathising and Systemising Quotients: Participants were asked to complete two questionnaires: the Systemising Quotient and the Empathising Quotient, as described by Baron Cohen (2003). These consist of a set of 60 items which participants rate on a four point scale as strongly agree, slightly agree, slightly disagree, strongly disagree. Forty items go forward to make the final score, while the other items are fillers. Scores on each item are added together to make a total maximum score of forty.

Sex and discipline differences 19 Results and Discussion Group differences on the Empathising Quotient and Systemising Quotient were examined. On the Empathising Quotient, there was a significant main effect of sex (F(1,16) = 6.39, p <.05, η 2 =.29), no significant effect of discipline (F(1,16) = 1.54, p = ns, η 2 =.09), and a marginally significant interaction (F(1,16) = 3.88, p =.07, η 2 =.20). This was due to the fact that male humanities students showed lower scores than the other three groups (28.00 vs. 43.00, 45.60 and 49.00 respectively). On the Systemising Quotient there was a significant main effect of sex (F(1,16) = 22.53, p <.01, η 2 =.59), with males outperforming females and a significant main effect of discipline (F(1,16) = 7.86, p <.05, η 2 =.33), with science students outperforming humanities students, but no significant interaction. This effect of discipline was not mirrored in performance on the Mechanical Reasoning task in Study 1, perhaps suggesting that preferences in systemising are more closely related to choice of university degree course than skills are, at least in this sample. Correlations between scores on the Empathising and Systemising Quotient and the other measures carried out in Study 1 are presented in Table 4. These must be interpreted with caution given the small sample size. Significant correlations were found between Systemising, Block Design and EFT. The association between Mechanical Reasoning and Systemising was moderate, but not significant. The correlations were not substantially altered by controlling for age and verbal ability.

Sex and discipline differences 20 Empathising was significantly associated with the AQ and with the Social Skills Inventory, as predicted. It also showed a moderate, but nonsignificant, correlation with the Mind in the Eyes measure. There was no significant correlation between empathising and systemising either before (r(20) =.214, p = ns) or after controlling for age and verbal ability (r(16) =.187, p = ns). A brain type score was determined for the Empathising and Systemising Quotients in the same manner as it was for the Social Skills and Mechanical Reasoning measures in Study 1. Both scores were standardised and the standard score for Empathising was subtracted from the standard score for Systemising. This gave a second brain type score, referred to as BCBT (Baron Cohen Brain Type). This measure was used to form groups of male and female brain profiles in the same way as for the first brain type measure. As with the first brain type measure, BCBT was significant associated with sex (Fishers Exact Test = 9.77, p <.001), but not with discipline (Fishers Exact Test = 3.43, p = ns). There was no significant difference between the three brain profiles on the Mind in the Eyes measure (F(2,17) = 1.54, p = ns, η 2 =.15) and a marginally significant difference between the groups on the AQ measure (F(2,17) = 3.16, p =.07, η 2 =.27). The effect sizes suggest that these differences might become significant with a larger group. The brain type group classification system used in Study 1 was not significantly associated with BCBT (Fisher s Exact test = 7.09, p =.11). Table 5 shows the association between the two classification systems. 11 of the 20 students were in the same group over the two measures. 3 moved from the balanced group on the first

Sex and discipline differences 21 measure to one of other two groups on the BCBT, and 5 moved from one of the unbalanced groups on our measure to the balanced group on the BCBT measure. One student was considered to have a female brain on our criteria and a male brain on the BCBT. This participant, A, is potentially interesting as he has the highest score in the sample on the AQ, a score that would normally indicate someone with Asperger s type difficulties. His score on the Mechanical Reasoning task was well below average, while his score on the Systemising Quotient was above average. This indicates that perhaps A prefers to work with systems rather than people or animals, but that he shows no particular skills in reasoning through mechanical systems. This is in line with the findings of Lawson et al (2004) that individuals with Asperger s Syndrome do not show a better understanding of physical systems than typical adult males. General Discussion The work presented above replicates previous results suggesting sex differences in tasks measuring social skills, central coherence, empathising and systemising (Baron Cohen & Hammer, 1997; Hoffman, 1977; Voyer et al., 1995). Further, on both classification systems male participants were more likely to have male brains and females were more likely to have female brains. Discipline differences were less consistent, occurring in only the Block Design, EFT and Systemising tasks. Overall, the tasks purported to measure empathising and social skills were associated with one another, and the tasks purporting to measure

Sex and discipline differences 22 systemising and spatial awareness clustered together. In contrast to the findings of Jarrold et al. (2000), there was not a clear association between the two sets of skills. The brain type measure showed some utility: females were more likely to have female type brains than males, and the groups formed on the basis of the brain type measure also differed with respect to their AQ scores, suggesting that students with female brains tended to show lower levels of autistic like symptomatology than those with male or balanced brains. However, there was no significant difference in AQ score between those with male brains and those with balanced brains, and a similar result was found with respect to the Mind in the Eyes measure. These findings weaken the argument that high levels of autistic symptoms are associated with an extreme male brain profile. Brain Types: Empathising Systemising theory The findings were consistent across a range of tasks in each area. Overall, Baron Cohen s (2002) central claim concerning empathising and systemising was supported. Men were more likely to have more developed systemising than empathising, and vice versa for women. As predicted, empathising was associated with theory of mind ability, as measured by the Mind in the Eyes test, while systemising was associated with weak central coherence. Results were similar using the two different sets of empathising and systemising measures. However, it is important to note that the two sets of measures were not highly correlated.

Sex and discipline differences 23 Extreme Male Brain Theory of Autism There was some evidence that individuals with male brain profiles showed higher levels of autistic symptomatology than individuals with female brain profiles once a single outlier was excluded. However, the effect was carried by the female group showing lower scores on the AQ than the other two groups. There was no evidence that those with male brain profiles had higher levels of autistic symptomatology than those with balanced brain profiles. These findings therefore provide only limited support for the Extreme Male Brain theory of autism. There was also no clear evidence of a link between the two sets of abilities. The results are in line with the findings of Lawson et al. (2004), Briskman et al. (2001) and Morgan et al. (2003) that these two areas are not closely related, and cast doubt upon the explanatory value of the Extreme Male Brain theory as an explanation for the symptoms found in autistic individuals. As described in the introduction, Baron Cohen does not assume a correlation between empathising and systemising, or between theory of mind and central coherence. However, if one is to assume that autism can be explained in terms of extreme scores in two unrelated areas in which variation is normally distributed, then statistically one would expect to find many more individuals who show weak theory of mind in the absence of weak central coherence, and vice versa. More research is needed to establish whether this is in fact the case, but at present it seems

Sex and discipline differences 24 more likely that autism forms the basis of a clustering of symptoms, rather than being the co occurrence of two independent extremes of processing. Findings in the present study are somewhat limited by the sample used, which consisted of university students with verbal abilities in the above average range. Using this sample may have restricted the range of empathising and systemising abilities present in the sample, and therefore reduced the potential for correlation between the two skills. In general, individuals with autistic difficulties tend to have weak verbal abilities, and it is possible that the results would be altered if this study was carried out in this kind of sample, though Lawson et al. (2004) find similar results in a group of individuals with Asperger s syndrome. Further research is needed to assess the relationships between the skills in individuals with more low functioning forms of autism. However, results at present imply that individuals with autism spectrum difficulties will typically show deficits in theory of mind and empathising compared to typically developing individuals, but that high levels of good systemising abilities are not specifically associated with autistic symptomatology. This somewhat surprising result can be illustrated with an example from our sample. Student A, who achieved the highest score in the sample on the AQ, showed difficulties with Mechanical Reasoning but achieved a high score on the Systemising Quotient. This illustrates the conceptual difference between preferences and interests, on the one hand, and abilities on the other hand. While an individual may enjoy working with machines and systems, this does not entail that they have an

Sex and discipline differences 25 aptitude for this way of reasoning. Hence, it may be that individuals with low social skills would rather interact with logical systems than with people as they are more predictable and less stressful to these individuals. This does not imply that these individuals will have a strength in working with machines, merely that they have a preference for this type of pastime. Overall, evidence from the experiment supports Baron Cohen s (2002) Empathising Systemising theory. Theory of mind, social skills and empathising are associated and spatial awareness, mechanical reasoning and systemising are also associated with one another. However, the evidence in favour of the Extreme Male Brain theory is more equivocal. The two areas of processing were not closely related to one another, and those with male brain profiles did not show increased levels of autistic symptomatology compared to those with balanced brains. Finally, it was shown that an aptitude for and interest in systems are separable, though associated, fields. References: Baron Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248 255. Baron Cohen, S. (2003). The essential difference: Men, women and the extreme male brain. London: Penguin Books Ltd.

Sex and discipline differences 26 Baron Cohen, S., Bolton, P., Wheelwright, S., Scahill, V., Short, L., Mead, G., et al. (1998). Does autism occur more often in the families of physicists, engineers, and mathematicians? Autism, 2, 296 301. Baron Cohen, S., & Hammer, J. (1997). Is autism an extreme form of the ʺmale brainʺ? Advances in Infancy Research, 11, 196 217. Baron Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a theory of mind? Cognition, 21, 37 46. Baron Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The ʺreading the mind in the eyesʺ test revised version: A study with normal adults and adults with asperger syndrome or high functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241 251. Baron Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J. G., & Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism Spectrum Quotient: Evidence from Asperger syndrome/ high functioning autism, males, females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5 17. Baron Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Stott, C., Bolton, P., & Goodyer, I. (1997). Is there a link between engineering and autism? Autism, 1(1), 101 109. Bennett, G. K., Seashore, H. G., & Wesman, A. G. (1974). Differential Aptitude Tests (5th ed.). New York: Psychological Corporation. Briskman, J., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2001). Exploring the cognitive phenotype of autism: Weak ʺcentral coherenceʺ in parents and siblings of children with

Sex and discipline differences 27 autism II: Real life skills and preferences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(3), 309 316. Frith, U. (2003). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Frith, U., & Happe, F. (1994). Autism: ʺbeyond theory of mindʺ. Cognition, 50, 115 132. Hoffman, M. L. (1977). Sex differences in empathy and related behaviours. Psychological Bulletin, 84(4), 712 722. Jarrold, C., Butler, D. W., Cottingham, E. M., & Jimenez, F. (2000). Linking theory of mind and central coherence in autism and in the general population. Developmental Psychology, 36(1), 126 138. Lawson, J., Baron Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). Empathising and systemising in adults with and without Asperber syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 301 310. Morgan, B., Maybery, M., & Durkin, K. (2003). Weak central coherence, poor joint attention and low verbal ability: Independent deficits in early autism. Developmental Psychology, 39(4), 646 656. Riggio, R. E. (1989). Social Skills Inventory. In. USA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250 270. Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, USA: Psychological Corporation.

Sex and discipline differences 28 Witkin, H. A., Oltman, P. K., Raskin, E., & Karp, S. A. (1971). A manual for the embedded figures test. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Sex and discipline differences 29 Tables and Figures Table 1. Mean scores on the AQ, Mind in the Eyes, Central Coherence, Social Skills and Mechanical Reasoning Tasks for the Four Subgroups. Male Female Science Humanities Science Humanities n = 12 n = 12 n = 12 n = 12 AQ 1 17.58 (4.36) 20.58 (7.09) 16.58 (5.18) 13.67 (5.55) Mind in the Eyes test 2 24.00 (4.92) 25.58 (4.21) 26.42 (3.18) 29.17 (3.49) WASI Block Design 3 129.17 124.83 123.83 116.67 (8.76) (8.76) (7.06) (6.68) EFT 4 15.78 (9.89) 13.26 (5.87) 14.05 (3.85) 23.96 (7.87) Mechanical Reasoning 5 12.75 (2.93) 13.33 (2.90) 10.17 (2.86) 9.42 (2.15) Social Skills Inventory 6 277.00 274.25 289.17 300.33 (18.25) (27.04) (22.93) (14.54) Note. Standard deviations in parentheses. AQ = Autism Spectrum Quotient; EFT = Embedded Figures Test; 1 Maximum score = 50; 2 Maximum score = 36; 3 Standard scores; 4 Mean reaction time; 5 Maximum score = 20; 6 Maximum score = 450.

Sex and discipline differences 30 Table 2. Correlations between AQ, Mind in the Eyes, Central Coherence, Social Skills and Mechanical Reasoning tasks Measure (N = 48) Vocab AQ ME BD EFT Mech. Reasoning SSI Age.084.232.018.057.115.068.250 Vocab.150.092.111.046.127.101 AQ.382**.128.095.141.433** ME.413**.153.054.018.375** BD.169.145.563**.429**.195 EFT.019.071.547**.348**.027 Mech. Reasoning.108.007.457**.437**.277 SSI.421**.378.209.135.284 Note. Partial correlations controlling for age and verbal ability are shown below the diagonal. Vocab = WASI Vocabulary subtest; AQ = Autism Spectrum Quotient; ME = Mind in the Eyes test; BD = Block Design; EFT = Embedded Figures Test; Mech. Reasoning = Mechanical Reasoning Test; SSI = Social Skills Inventory. ** Significant at p <.01

Sex and discipline differences 31 Table 3: Distribution of participants in the three Brain profiles Group Male Brain Balanced Female Brain (N = 48) Profile (E < S) Brain (E = S) Profile (E > S) Male sciences (n = 12) Male humanities (n = 12) Female sciences (n = 12) Female humanities (n = 12) 5 7 6 5 1 2 3 7 4 8 TOTAL 13 19 16

Sex and discipline differences 32 Table 4: Correlations between Empathising and Systemising and other measures. Bivariate Correlations Partial Correlations N = 20 (age and verbal IQ controlled) Empathising Systemising Empathising Systemising AQ.609**.373.686**.341 ME.364.199.406.207 EFT.089.499*.146.507* Block Design.113.579**.140.615** Mech. Reasoning.031.354.021.337 Social Skills.459*.029.462.010 Note. AQ = Autism Spectrum Quotient; ME = Mind in the Eyes test; EFT = Embedded Figures Test; Mech. Reasoning = Mechanical Reasoning Test. ** Significant at p<.01; * Significant at p<.05.

Sex and discipline differences 33 Table 5: The association between the two brain type measures. Baron Cohen Brain Type N = 20 Male profile Balanced Female profile Carroll & Chiew Male profile 2 4 Brain type Balanced 2 5 1 Female profile 1 1 4