OPTIMISTIC BIAS AND INMATES

Similar documents
Medical Conditions, Mental Health Problems, Disabilities, and Mortality Among Jail Inmates American Jail Association

Bucks County Drug Court Program Application

Study of Recidivism, Race, Gender, and Length of Stay

Pathways to Crime. Female Offender Experiences of Victimization. JRSA/BJS National Conference, Portland Maine, 10/28/10

Domestic Violence Inventory: Annual Summary Report

On an average day in , up to 4.4% of state

Arkansas Department of Correction. Prison Inmate Inventory

Evaluation of the First Judicial District Court Adult Drug Court: Quasi-Experimental Outcome Study Using Historical Information

Sexual Adjustment Inventory

Third-Person Perception and Racism

Allen County Community Corrections. Home Detention-Day Reporting Program. Report for Calendar Years

CHAPTER 1 An Evidence-Based Approach to Corrections

Are Drug Treatment Programs in Prison Effective in Reducing Recidivism Rates?

SAQ-Adult Probation III & SAQ-Short Form

Washington Association of Sheriffs & Police Chiefs

The past three decades have witnessed

LUCAS COUNTY TASC, INC. OUTCOME ANALYSIS

Assessment of the Safe Streets Treatment Options Program (SSTOP)

National Findings on Mental Illness and Drug Use by Prisoners and Jail Inmates. Thursday, August 17

Santa Fe Municipal Court P.O. Box 909 Santa Fe, New Mexico 87504

issue. Some Americans and criminal justice officials want to protect inmates access to

Aging and mortality in the state prison population

SAQ-Short Form Reliability and Validity Study in a Large Sample of Offenders

DESCRIPTION OF FOLLOW-UP SAMPLE AT INTAKE SECTION TWO

Background/Analyses TABLE #1

Part 115 PRISON RAPE ELIMINATION ACT NATIONAL STANDARDS Published June 20, 2012

3726 E. Hampton St., Tucson, AZ Phone (520) Fax (520)

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY. New Mexico Statistical Analysis Center April Prepared by: Kristine Denman, Director, NMSAC

Policy and interventions for adults with serious mental illness and criminal justice involvement

Women Prisoners and Recidivism Factors Associated with Re-Arrest One Year Post-Release

in Indiana Detailed Analysis

BRAZOS VALLEY COUNCIL ON ALCOHOL AND SUBSTANCE ABUSE BOARD POLICY SECTION 600: CRIMINAL JUSTICE. Policy Statement

Allen County Community Corrections. Modified Therapeutic Community. Report for Calendar Years

Unit 2: Inmates Rights to be Free from Sexual Abuse and Sexual Harassment and Staff and Inmates Rights to be Free from Retaliation for Reporting

Transition from Jail to Community. Reentry in Washtenaw County

Shoplifting Inventory: Standardization Study

Nebraska LB605: This bill is designed to reduce prison overcrowding and allows for alternatives to incarceration like CAM.

SAQ-Adult Probation III: Normative Study

PROMISING SHORT TERM INTERVENTIONS:

probation, number of parole revocations, DVI Alcohol Scale scores, DVI Control Scale scores, and DVI Stress Coping Abilities Scale scores.

Limits of Understanding. by Paul Klee ( )

Moving Beyond Incarceration For Justice-involved Women : An Action Platform To Address Women s Needs In Massachusetts

Department of Public Safety Division of Adult Correction and Juvenile Justice

Victim Index Reliability and Validity Study

THE 21ST CENTURY CURES ACT: TACKLING MENTAL HEALTH FROM THE INSIDE OUT

NO. OF PAGES: 1-CORE-4D-14, 1-CORE-4D-15, 1-CORE-4D-16 5

Evaluation of Santa Fe s LEAD Program: Criminal Justice Outcomes

FOCUS. Views from the National Council on Crime and Delinquency. Attitudes of US Voters toward Nonserious Offenders and Alternatives to Incarceration

NORTHAMPTON COUNTY DRUG COURT. An Overview

Working to Reform Marijuana Laws

Fact Sheet: Drug Data Summary

Greg's Place - Application

HIV, STIGMA AND PRISON

Addressing a National Crisis Too Many People with Mental Illnesses in our Jails

Community-based sanctions

The Public Safety Coordinating Council s. Criminal Justice System Data Book January 2014

Success in Drug Offenders in Rehabilitation Programs. Austin Nichols CJUS 4901 FALL 2012

Assessing Risk. October 22, Tammy Meredith, Ph.D. Applied Research Services, Inc.

Campus Crime Brochure

Adult Perpetrators. Chapter 10

West Virginia Department of Military Affairs and Public Safety

Legalized Cannabis: The Summit. Nancy E. O Malley District Attorney Alameda County, California

Medication Assisted Treatment in the Justice System. NCSL Law, Criminal Justice and Public Safety Committee Breakfast

Allegheny County Justice Related Services for Individuals with Mental Illness:

UTAH SEXUAL OFFENSE STATUTES STATUTE OF LIMITATIONS DNA EXCEPTION

West Virginia Department of Military Affairs and Public Safety

Substance Use and Disorder, Involvement in the Drug Trade, and Mortality: A longitudinal study of delinquent youth

YMCA of Reading & Berks County Housing Application

Correctional Medicine. By: Robert B. Pritt D.O.

HIV CRIMINALIZATION IN OHIO. Elizabeth Bonham, JD Staff Attorney, ACLU of Ohio

HEALTHIER LIVES, STRONGER FAMILIES, SAFER COMMUNITIES:

Running head: EVALUATION OF PROGRAMS DESIGNED TO REDUCE RECIDIVISM 1. An Evaluation of Programs Designed to Reduce Recidivism.

Who is a Correctional Psychologist? Some authors make a distinction between correctional psychologist and a psychologist who works in a correctional f

DVI Pre-Post: Standardization Study

Perceptions of Disorder: Results from Two Las Vegas Tourist Locations

Trauma and Justice Strategic Initiative: Trauma Informed Care & Trauma Specific Services

Criminal Justice Reform: Treatment and Substance Use Disorder

The Cost of Imprisonment

Improving Prevention and Response to Sexual Misconduct on Campus: How the Data Help Us

Douglas County s Mental Health Diversion Program

elements of change Juveniles

Several studies have researched the effects of framing on opinion formation and decision

Advisory Commission on the Administration of Justice. Justice Reinvestment Presentation #1 September 12, 2018

Dispute Resolution and Psychology

Corrections, Public Safety and Policing

Campus Crime Brochure for academic year

Violence Prevention A Strategy for Reducing Health Inequalities

VISTA COLLEGE ONLINE CAMPUS

Mid-1970s to mid- 80s, U.S. s incarceration rate doubled. Mid- 80s to mid- 90s, it doubled again. In absolute terms, prison/jail population from 1970

Infectious Disease The Public Health Response

PREA. Fact Bulletin. Sexual Harassment

SACRAMENTO DEA: METHAMPHETAMINE. Intelligence Analyst Matthew S. Kregor


Arizona Arrestee Reporting Information Network. Methamphetamine Use Among Recently Booked Arrestees and Detainees in Maricopa County, Arizona

Traumatic Brain Injury Screening in Correctional Populations. Abby Bernett. Marquette University

epic.org EPIC WI-FOIA Production epic.org/algorithmic-transparency/crim-justice/

Responding to Homelessness. 11 Ideas for the Justice System

Smart on Crime, Smart on Drugs

Poisoning Death Review Report. Montgomery County, 2017

Transcription:

OPTIMISTIC BIAS AND INMATES John Chapin and Mari Pierce Pennsylvania State University The study sought to extend the optimistic bias literature by documenting the phenomenon among perpetrators of crime. The theory originated from health psychology and is primarily used in health contexts. While a number of recent studies have documented optimistic bias among crime victims, the current study is the first to document the phenomenon among perpetrators. A small-scale survey of county jail inmates incarcerated for drug-related offenses, property-related offenses, and violent offenses (N = 60) found optimistic bias emerged for all three types of offenders. Participants were optimistic about not getting caught for the offense they were currently serving time and even more optimistic about not being incarcerated again in the future. Predictors of optimistic bias included self-esteem, self-efficacy, and marital status. These results suggest the need for different approaches to crime prevention and rehabilitation of inmates. Keywords: optimistic bias; crime; incarceration According to the Bureau of Justice Statistics (2013), approximately one-third of all adult inmates (nearly three-quarters of a million individuals) incarcerated in correctional facilities throughout the United States are housed in local jails. More individuals pass through jails each year than prisons. There are an estimated 11.7 million individuals annually admitted into local jails, versus 609,800 individuals annually admitted into state and federal prisons (Bureau of Justice Statistics, 2014). This transiency in jail populations is likely due to jails housing individuals who are at various stages within the system (i.e., pre-trial detainees, probation and parole violators, those awaiting transfer to different institutions or jurisdictions, individuals who are convicted but not yet sentenced, those sentenced for misdemeanor and felony offenses, and state or federal offenders contracted to serve their sentences in a jail) versus prisons, which house only sentenced felony offenders. Some inmates may stay in jail for a few hours, days or weeks, while others serve sentences typically up to one year. Inmate populations provide a unique opportunity to study risk perception. Inmates have already taken risks by engaging in criminal activity and received negative consequences (arrest and detainment). The purpose of the study is to test a robust risk-perception theory, optimistic bias, among inmates to see if the phenomenon still occurs under unusual circumstances. Author Note: John Chapin, Department of Communications, Pennsylvania State University; Mari Pierce, Department of Administration of Justice, Pennsylvania State University. * Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to John Chapin, Department of Communications, Pennsylvania State University, Monaca, PA 15061.E-mail: jrc11@psu.edu.

186 OPTIMISTIC BIAS AND INMATES OPTIMISTIC BIAS In lay terms, optimistic bias (Weinstein, 1980) is the belief that bad things happen to other people. More than 30 years and over 100 published articles later, optimistic bias has been documented in a broad array of contexts: For instance, terminal cancer patients believe they are more likely to survive than other cancer patients (Smith & Longo, 2013); people who live in disaster areas believe they are less likely than others to be harmed (Greenberg, Dyen, & Elliott, 2013); adolescents believe they are less likely than peers to contract Sexually Transmitted Diseases (STDs) (Zeeb, Pottgen, & Zeeb, 2013). On its own, optimistic bias is an interesting phenomenon, but it often is paired with a behavioral component: People who exhibit optimistic bias are less likely to take precautions (Becker, Paton, Johnston, & Ronan, 2013; Ludwig & Zimper, 2013; Sesen, 2014); thus people who believe they are less likely to contract STDs are also less likely to practice safe sex. Likewise, people who believe they are less likely to be harmed in a hurricane are also less likely to evacuate. The current study explores whether the same principles apply to criminals. Optimistic Bias and Crime The concept of optimistic bias emerged from the health psychology literature in the 1980s. For the first two decades, it was exclusive to health contexts. More recently, a handful of studies have applied the concept to crime. For instance, a study of 1,600 adolescents (Chapin, Strimel, & Coleman, 2014) found optimistic bias about dating violence. Despite awareness of abusive relationships and community resources, adolescents believe they are less likely than peers to be abused by their boyfriends or girlfriends. Related contexts include burglary (Joshi & Carter, 2013), information security (Rhee, Ryu, & Kim, 2012), and sexual assault (Chapin & Pierce, 2012). These studies have two things in common: (1) In each case, optimistic bias was observed; (2) Each of the studies focus on perceptions of potential victims. The current study extends the literature to potential perpetrators. Self-esteem A number of predictors of optimistic bias have been explored over the years. One of the most promising is self-esteem. In the study of adolescents and dating violence, the strongest predictor of optimistic bias was self-esteem. Students who scored high on the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale exhibited higher degrees of optimistic bias (Chapin et al., 2014). The finding is consistent with earlier work in health contexts, such as pregnancy risk (Smith, Gerrard, & Gibbons, 1997) and suicide (Weinstein & Lachendro, 1982). Self-efficacy Self-efficacy has a similar relationship with optimistic bias: The more confident people are about their abilities, the more optimistic bias increases. This has been documented regarding people who live near earthquake fault lines (Becker et al., 2013), senior citizens with health issues (Warner, Schwarzer, Schuz, Wurm, & Tesch-Romer, 2012), even business students perceived financial risks of starting their own businesses (Sesen, 2014).

CHAPIN AND PIERCE 187 Demographics Given theories about adolescent invulnerability, it makes sense that age would predict optimistic bias. This is not always the case. There are similar mixed results with other demographic variables. The study of adolescents and dating violence (Chapin et al., 2014) reported optimistic bias increased with age, but was unrelated to gender and race. Another study found adolescents believed they were less likely than peers to get cancer from smoking, but optimistic bias in this case was unrelated to age (Pepper, Cameron, Reiter, McRee, & Brewer, 2013). A study conducted in India and the United Kingdom found optimistic bias different by race/country for a number of future life events, including divorce, suicide, and burglary (Joshi & Carter, 2013). A study about optimistic bias and sexual assault found men exhibited more optimistic bias than women; optimistic bias increased with age, but was unrelated to race (Chapin & Pierce, 2012). Based on the preceding review of the literature, the following hypotheses and research question are posited: H1: Criminals believe they are less likely than other criminals to get caught. H2: Inmates believe they are less likely than other inmates to get caught again in the future. H3: Optimistic bias will increase as self-esteem increases. H4: Optimistic bias will increase as self-efficacy increases. RQ1: What is the relationship between optimistic bias among inmates and demographics (age, gender, race, marital status, and criminal record)? METHOD Participants and Procedures Access to the files of each inmate (18 years of age and older) incarcerated on a predetermined day was granted from the warden of a county jail, located within a mid-atlantic state. According to the U.S. Census (2012), the population estimate for the county is just over 170,000 residents. Over 90% of the county s residents are white. The total capacity for the county jail is 403 inmates. The official count on the pre-determined date was 358 inmates. The total collected sample size was 351. Of the collected sample, all but two individuals were classified as either white or black. The remaining two cases were excluded from the analysis, leaving a total sample size of 349. Of the 349 eligible, 60 (17%) agreed to complete a survey with one of the researchers. Inmates received no compensation or special privileges for their participation and completed informed consent prior to the interviews. Both researchers completed special training on working with inmates prior to receiving approval from the university s Institutional Review Board (IRB) for conducting research with inmate populations. Descriptive statistics for the sample are in Table 1.

188 OPTIMISTIC BIAS AND INMATES Table 1 Descriptive Statistics for Variables, N = 60 Dichotomous Variables N % Gender Male 52 86.7 Female 8 13.3 Marital Status Not Married 55 90.0 Married 6 10.0 Race White 36 60.0 Black 24 40.0 High School Diploma/GED HS Diploma 36 60.0 No HS Diploma 24 40.0 Sentenced No 50 83.3 Yes 10 16.7 Offense Type Drug Related 26 43.3 Property Related 20 33.3 Violent 14 23.3 Most Serious Prior Conviction Drug Related 23 46.0 Property Related 15 30.0 Violent 12 24.0 Continuous Variables Mean SD Range N Age at Intake 32.2 10.7 18-57 60 Incarceration Time (months) 7.7 6.3 2-28 60 Sentence Length (in months) 17.4 10.8 6-48 60 Materials Optimistic bias was measured with a standard instrument (Weinstein, 1989): Compared to others convicted of similar offenses, how likely are you to become incarcerated again in the future? Responses were on a Likert-type scale (-3 = much less than others; 3 = much greater than others). Optimistic bias is indicated by a negative mean.

CHAPIN AND PIERCE 189 Participants also were asked Compared to others committing similar offenses, how confident were you at the time you committed the offense which resulted in your incarceration that you would get away with it? Responses ranged from -3 (not confident) to 3 (very confident). Self-esteem was measured with the Rosenberg scale (1979), a 10-item scale that measures global self-worth by measuring both positive and negative feelings about the self. All items are answered using a 4-point scale ranging from strongly disagree (0) to strongly agree (3). Negative items were reverse coded, so higher numbers indicate higher esteem. The resulting scale was reliable (α =.83). Self-efficacy was measured with a single item: How confident are you in your ability to re-enter society (get a job, find housing, etc.) following release? Responses were measured on a Likert-type scale (0 = not confident; 6 = very confident). Demographic information was obtained from inmate files. Gender of the offender was coded as male (1) or female (2). Marital status at the time of incarceration was coded as (0) not married, separated, widowed or divorced or (1) married. Inmates with a high school diploma or GED were coded as 1 and those without a diploma or GED were coded as 0. Race was coded as white (1) or black (2). The nature of incarceration was coded as either not-sentenced (0) or sentenced (1). Length of incarceration and age also were included as continuous variables. RESULTS H1 predicted optimistic bias, that inmates believed they are less likely than others to get caught for the offense (for which they are currently incarcerated). Optimistic bias is indicated by a group mean significantly less than zero. As predicted, inmates believed they were less likely than others to get caught (M = -.2, SD = 1.9), t (56) = 13.0, p<.000. H1 was supported. H2 predicted optimistic bias about future crimes, that inmates believed they are less likely than other inmates to become incarcerated again. As predicted, inmates believed they were less likely than others to be incarcerated in the future (M = -1.0, SD = 2.0), t (56) = -3.6, p<.01. H2 was supported. Table 2 displays zero-order correlations among the variables predicting optimistic bias. Standard multiple regression was used to identify the predictors of optimistic bias. Analysis of residual plots indicates that assumptions regarding normality, linearity and homoscadasticity were met. Because optimistic bias is indicated by a negative mean, signs are reversed in the tables for ease of interpretation. H3 predicted that optimistic bias would increase as self-esteem increased. Selfesteem ranged from 1 (very low) to 30 (very high), with an average of 20.1. Most of the inmates (76%) rated in the top half (positive self-esteem). Only 5% exhibited very low

190 OPTIMISTIC BIAS AND INMATES self-esteem. Table 3 shows the predicted relationship emerged for the past offense, but not for future incarceration. H3 was supported, but only for past criminal activities. Table 2 Zero-Order Correlations among Variables Related to Optimistic Bias 2 3 4 5 1. Optimistic Bias (future).09.28*.08.06 2. Optimistic Bias (past) ----.07.32** -.14 3. Self-Efficacy ----.19.05 4. Self-esteem ----.02 5. Age ---- Note. *p<.05, **p<.01. Because optimistic bias is indicated by a negative mean, signs are reversed in rows one and two for ease of interpretation. Table 3 Summary of Linear Regression Analysis for Variables Related to Optimistic Bias Adj. r 2 =.14 N = 56 Predictor B SE B β Self-efficacy.24.13.15* Marital status.11.06.12* *p<.05 Because optimistic bias is indicated by a negative mean, signs have been reversed for ease of interpretation. H4 predicted that optimistic bias would increase as self-efficacy increased. Confidence in one s ability to be re-enter society (home, job, etc.) following incarceration ranged from zero (3.3%) to six (41.7%), with an average of 4.3. Self-efficacy skewed high, with 65% of inmates exhibiting at least moderately high confidence. Tables 2 and 3 show self-efficacy as the strongest predictor of optimistic bias regarding future incarceration. H4 is supported, but only for optimistic bias regarding the future. RQ1 explored the relationship between optimistic bias and demographics (age, gender, race, marital status, and criminal record). The demographic breakdown of the sample is described in Table 1. T-tests were used to test for differences in optimistic bias in binary variables (gender, marital status, and race). One-way ANOVA was used to test for differences between categorical variables (type of offense). Correlation was used to test for a relationship between optimistic bias and age. The only significant relationship to emerge

CHAPIN AND PIERCE 191 was between future optimistic bias and marital status. Married (M = -3.0, SD =.1) inmates were more optimistic about their ability to avoid future incarceration than were unmarried (M = -.8, SD = 2.1) inmates, t (55) = 2.4, p<.01. Table 3 shows marital status to be a significant predictor of optimistic bias regarding future incarceration. DISCUSSION The purpose of the current study was to see if optimistic bias emerges among inmates. The theory originated from health psychology. While a number of recent studies have documented the phenomenon among crime victims, the current study extends the literature by documenting optimistic bias among perpetrators. Participants in the study were incarcerated for drug-related offenses, property-related offenses, and violent offenses. Optimistic bias emerged for all three types of offenders, with no significant differences between offender types. Inmates were optimistic about not getting caught for the offense they were currently serving time, and even more optimistic about not being incarcerated again in the future. The use of qualitative measures (indepth interviews) could further explain if inmates are optimistic about not committing further offenses or optimistic about not getting caught again in the future. The current findings do provide some useful insights into the perceptual bias among current inmates. Despite being convicted and serving time for a criminal offense, inmates exhibited high self-esteem. This positive self-perception was the strongest predictor of optimistic bias for the offense that resulted in the current incarceration. This could be interpreted as being smarter or more highly skilled at evading the police than other offenders. Marital status and family relationships didn t prevent the perpetration of the crime, but the higher the self-esteem, the more likely they were to think they were going to get away with it. While inmates self-esteem wasn t diminished following conviction, it no longer predicted optimistic bias about future crimes. Instead, marital status and confidence in one s ability to re-enter society (get a job, find a place to live) emerge as the strongest predictors of optimistic bias about future incarceration. This relationship held true, regardless of the race or gender of the offender. These results suggest the need for different approaches to crime prevention and rehabilitation of inmates, with an emphasis on stabilizing families and personal relationships during periods of incarceration. A number of limitations should be considered before interpreting the results of this study. Inmates are a protected population for research. This limited the sample size of the current study and may discourage others from conducting research in jails and prisons. For the current study, this limits the generalizability of the results. Despite these limitations, it is important for research in this area to continue. None to report. CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT

192 OPTIMISTIC BIAS AND INMATES REFERENCES Becker, J., Paton, D., Johnston, D., & Ronan, K. (2013). Salient beliefs about earthquake hazards and household preparedness. Risk Analysis, 33(9), 1710-1727. Bureau of Justice Statistics. (2013). Correctional populations in the United States, 2012. Retrieved June 27, 2014, from http://www.bjs.gov/index.cfm?ty=pbdetail&iid=4843. Bureau of Justice Statistics. (2014). Jail inmates at midyear, 2013 Statistical tables, 2013. Retrieved June 27, 2014, from http://www.bjs.gov/index.cfm?ty=pbdetail&iid=4988. Chapin, J., & Pierce, M. (2012). Optimistic bias, sexual assault, and fear. Journal of General Psychology, 139(1), 19-28. Chapin, J., Strimel, L., & Coleman, G. (2014). It won t happen to me: Addressing adolescents risk perception of dating violence. International Journal of Violence and Schools, 14, 44-54. Greenberg, M., Dyen, S., & Elliott, S. (2013). The public s preparedness: Self-reliance, flashbulb memories, and conservative values. American Journal of Public Health, 103(6), 85-91. Joshi, M., & Carter, W. (2013). Unrealistic optimism: East and west? Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1-15. Ludwig, A., & Zimper, A. (2013). A parsimonious model of subjective life expectancy. Theory and Decision, 75, 519-541. Pepper, J., Cameron, L., Reiter, P., McRee, A., & Brewer, N. (2013). Non-smoking male adolescents reactions to cigarette warnings. PLOS One, 8(8), 533-541. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0065533 Rhee, H., Ryu, Y., & Kim, C. (2012). Unrealistic optimism on information security management. Computers and Security, 31, 221-232. Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books. Sesen, H. (2014). Personality or environment? A comprehensive study on the entrepreneurial intentions of university students. Education and Training, 55(7), 624-640. Smith, G., Gerrard, M., & Gibbons, F. (1997). Self-esteem and the relation between risk behavior and perceptions of vulnerability to unplanned pregnancy in college women. Health Psychology, 16(2), 137-146. Smith, T., & Longo, D. (2013). Talking with patients about dying. The New England Journal of Medicine, 36, 17-19. United States Census Bureau. (2013) State and county quick facts. Retrieved June 27, 2014, from http:// quickfacts.census.gov/qfd/index.html# Warner, L., Schwarzer, R., Schuz, B., Wurm, S., & Tesch-Romer, C. (2012). Health-specific optimism mediates between objective and perceived psychical functioning in older adults. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 35, 400-406. Weinstein, N. (1989). Perceptions of personal susceptibility to harm. In V. Mays, G. Albee & F. Schneider (Eds.), Psychological approaches to the primary prevention of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome (142-167). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Weinstein, N. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 806-460. Weinstein, N., & Lachendro, E. (1982). Egocentrism as a source of unrealistic optimism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8(2), 195-200. Zeeb, F., Pottgen, S., & Zeeb, H. (2013). Higher risk perception of HIV than chlamydia and HPV among secondary school students in two German cities. PLOS One, 8(4), 1-8. doi:10.1371/journal. pone.0061636 Received: 3/2015 Accepted: 10/2015 Chapin, J., & Pierce, M. (2015) Optimistic bias and inmates.[electronic Version]. Applied Psychology in Criminal Justice, 11(3), 185-192.