THE IMPACT OF SELF-REGULATION ON BRAND PREFERENCE

Similar documents
Validating Measures of Self Control via Rasch Measurement. Jonathan Hasford Department of Marketing, University of Kentucky

ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract

Repeated choosing increases susceptibility to affective product features

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Consumers must often decide whether they want to reengage

HEDONIC HUNGER AND SELF-CONTROL: THE IMPACT OF PALATABILITY, POWER OF FOOD AND DIETARY RESTRAINT ON SELF-CONTROL DEPLETION. Kathleen M.

Domain-specificity and domain-generality in self-control

TIME DISCOUNTING FOR PRIMARY AND MONETARY REWARDS *

Sweet instigator. Choosing increases susceptibility to affective product features. Sabrina Bruyneel 1 Katholieke Universiteit Leuven

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Cognitive Load and Neuro-Economics: Implications for Food Consumption and Health. George Davis and Elena Serrano Virginia Tech 8/15/2016

Value From Regulatory Fit E. Tory Higgins

The Effect of Regulatory Focus and Delay Type on Consumers Reactions to Delay

Chapter 8: Consumer Attitude Formation and Change

Snacking in Canada. Background. Updates from Competitiveness and Market Analysis Branch. By Jeewani Fernando and Lukas Matejovsky

CONCEPT OF PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR

Discounting of Monetary and Directly Consumable Rewards Sara J. Estle, Leonard Green, Joel Myerson, and Daniel D. Holt

International Journal of Educational Advancement (2011) 10, doi: /ijea

Self-Regulating Enhances Self-Regulation in Subsequent Consumer Decisions Involving. Similar Response Conflicts SIEGFRIED DEWITTE SABRINA BRUYNEEL

Effects of Written Disclosure on Post- Disaster Psychological Adjustment and Symptomatology

MANAGEMENT. MGMT 0021 THE MANAGEMENT PROCESS 3 cr. MGMT 0022 FINANCIAL ACCOUNTING 3 cr. MGMT 0023 MANAGERIAL ACCOUNTING 3 cr.

Eat Well Live Well. Making Healthy Choices

Neuroplasticity What Graduate Students Need to Know about Learning, Memory and Development

Ego Depletion After Social Interference

USING SELF-AFFIRMATION TO COUNTER SELF-CONTROL DEPLETION

Problem Solving

The Color of Similarity

Amplifying the Role of Supermarkets in Wellness Marketing. September 13, 2009 Group Vice President Health & Wellness Marketing

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect

By Reuven Bar-On. Development Report

Self-Regulation, Heuristics and Compliance: A Deeper Insight in the Underlying Process of Sequential Request Techniques

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect. David K. Sherman

Grade 6: Healthy Body Lesson 5: Have a Heart Healthy Body

Advantage EAP Employee Assistance Program

The Relationship Between Inhibitory Control and Free Will Beliefs in 4-to 6-Year- Old-Children

Purolite Life Sciences Brand Positioning Catalyst

110 Westwood Plaza Los Angeles, CA Visiting Researcher Jan August 2018

Appendix: Instructions for Treatment Index B (Human Opponents, With Recommendations)

Number of grams of fat x 9 calories per gram = Total number of calories from fat

Coach on Call. Please give me a call if you have more questions about this or other topics.

Veronika Grimm, Friederike Mengel. Let me sleep on it: Delay reduces rejection rates in Ultimatum Games RM/10/017

For general queries, contact

motivation workbook why do you want to change?

Lesson Yes No Comments and/or Changes

Defining emotion: a clinical perspective

weight watchers instant pot 2019 freestyle cookbook easy and delicious ww smart points recipes for quick and healthy meals

MANAGING DISTRESS TOLERANCE - HOW CAN I IMPROVE MY PATIENT'S DISTRESS TOLERANCE?

Affective Forecasting and Self-Control: Why Anticipating Pride Wins Over Anticipating Shame in a Self-Regulation Context

ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH

EVERYDAY TEMPTATIONS AN EXPERIENCE SAMPLING STUDY ON HOW PEOPLE CONTROL THEIR DESIRES

When Getting Angry Is Smart: Emotional Preferences and Emotional Intelligence

Chapter 11: Behaviorism: After the Founding

Smart self-control strategies: Beyond effortful inhibition

Does regulatory focus play a role in dietary restraint?

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

Supporting Online Material for

Perspective. Biological perspective entering psychology. Understanding Eating from a psychological perspective

The Emotional Survival Training Manual

Repressive Coping and Pain: Shifts in Attention from Distress toward Physical Pain May Partly Explain Conversion

Affective forecasting and self-control: Why anticipating pride wins over anticipating shame in a self-regulation context

Chapter 3 Tools for Practical Theorizing: Theoretical Maps and Ecosystem Maps

Ainslie 1975 Hoch & Lowenstein 1991 Khan Dhar & Wertenbroch 2005 Mischel Shoda & Rodriguez 1989

Positive Psychological Interventions: A Food Pantry Field Study Pre-Analysis Plan

Conscious and unconscious thought preceding complex decisions: The influence of taking notes and intelligence.

MBA SEMESTER III. MB0050 Research Methodology- 4 Credits. (Book ID: B1206 ) Assignment Set- 1 (60 Marks)

ABSTRACT. Professor Dr. Rebecca Hamilton and Professor Dr. Joydeep Srivastava, Department of Marketing

Presented by: Jennifer Mitchell, MS, LPCC

BEHAVIOR CHANGE THEORY

Food and Consumers: Current Issues and Future Directions

Celebrity Endorsement: The Effects of Social Comparisons on Women s Self-Esteem and Purchase Intensions

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

A nudge in the right direction?

Consumer Insights Gum Category. Empowering Manufacturers and Retailers for Category 1 Growth

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2)

BASIC VOLUME. Elements of Drug Dependence Treatment

Ogden, J., Whyman, C. (1997) The effects of repeated weighing on psychological state. European Eating Disorders Review 5,

Research on happiness and economics

STRAINING THE MENTAL MUSCLE: A RESOURCE DEPLETION ACCOUNT OF THE EFFECTIVENESS OF SOCIAL INFLUENCE TECHNIQUES

BODY RESPONSIBLE FOR IMPLEMENTING THE SCHEME

At the Israel Electric Company: Israel Railways

Coaching, a scientific method

Personality means how a person affects others and how he understands and views himself as well as the pattern of inner and outer measurable traits,

The Drinking Age and TrafficSafety

Number of grams of fat x 9 calories per gram = Total number of calories from fat

PERCEIVED AUTONOMOUS HELP AND RECIPIENTS WELL-BEING: IS AUTONOMOUS HELP GOOD FOR EVERYONE?

MANAGEMENT (MGMT) Management (MGMT) 1

Impact and Evidence briefing

CHAPTER NINE INTERPERSONAL DETERMINANTS OF CONSUMER BEHAVIOR

TTI SUCCESS INSIGHTS Personal Interests, Attitudes and Values TM

COACH WORKPLACE REPORT. Jane Doe. Sample Report July 18, Copyright 2011 Multi-Health Systems Inc. All rights reserved.

Emotional Quotient Inventory EQ-i

International Symposium on Quality of Life and Well-being of People with Disabilities: Theory, Research & Implementation - Sep.

Negative affect words prime beer consumption in young drinkers

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

Focus of Today s Presentation. Partners in Healing Model. Partners in Healing: Background. Data Collection Tools. Research Design

Analysis on the Dilemma of Restricting the Development of Leisure Sports in China

FIGHTING FAT A ROLE FOR FOOD RETAILERS

Here are the biggest offenders of 'healthy foods' that aren't so healthy.

Beth J. Massie. The Effect of Training on User Buy-In. A Master s Paper for the M.S. in I.S degree. April, pages. Advisor: Dr. Robert Losee.

Transcription:

Session 1-6 THE IMPACT OF SELF-REGULATION ON BRAND PREFERENCE Rod Duclos University of North Carolina Rod_Duclos@unc.edu S appuyant sur la recherche fondamentale menée en psychologie sociale, cet article met en lumière les effets inattendus du processus de contrôle de soi sur les préférences pour marques. Une expérimentation démontre qu un état affaibli de la ressource régulatrice conduit à un renforcement des préférences. Les conséquences marketing sont discutées. Drawing on research from social psychology, this paper highlights the inconspicuous impact of self-regulation on brand preference. In one experiment, we establish that a state of depletion of one s regulatory resource leads to the strengthening of one s preferences. We further rule out the possibility that such strengthening be caused by affect manipulations. Managerial implications are discussed.

Session 1-7 THE IMPACT OF SELF-REGULATION ON BRAND PREFERENCE Crucial to practitioners as firms attempt to reduce demand variability and ensure stable streams of revenues, consumer choice models have occupied much of the scholarly research in the past three decades (Bass 1974, McFadden 1986, Allenby and Rossi 1999, Seetharaman 2003). Inherent to choice is the idea of preference. Whether it be cereals, flowers, clothing designers, or a dinner s main course, consumers tend to choose the product they prefer, that is, the one they think will maximize their utility. In this paper, we investigate the stability of preferences over time. Specifically, we draw on recent research from social psychology to articulate the relationship among temptation, self-regulation, and preference strength. We start this paper by swiftly clarifying the self-control process and then move on to the experimental design of our study. Three factors intervene in the self-control process. The first, goals, refers to the standards, ideals, norms and other guidelines that one holds for himself. Examples of goals include an ideal weight, a shopping list, a desired emotional state (e.g., happiness), or achieving a certain performance. Along the absence of goals, conflict among goals can also undermine self-control. In several experiments, Tice, Bratslavski, and Baumeister (2001) have established that when participants are induced in a state of negative emotions, the short term goal of feeling better takes precedence over long term goals such as saving money. The second factor, monitoring, refers to the process of keeping track of one s own behavior. In a series of experiments where participants were asked to taste-test candies, Polivy et al. (1986) demonstrated how self-monitoring (or lack of thereof) led to lesser (greater) candy consumption. Other examples of monitoring failure can be found in the addiction literature where alcohol has been shown to reduce attention to the self (Hull 1981), thereby increasing the likelihood of self-control failure (Carver and Scheier 1981, 1982). The last factor in the triad, willpower, refers to the capacity to alter one s own behavior. Indeed, knowing precisely what we want (i.e., factor #1) and monitoring our behavior (i.e., factor #2) may not be sufficient to self-control. When confronted to a choice situation featuring one response option promising in the short term but conflicting with long term goals and another response option less tempting in the short term but in line with long term goals, for the individual to choose the righteous option, the self must produce an amount of energy powerful enough to surpass the strength of the impulse generated by the tempting option. Empirical approach The research question we tackle thereafter is that of the impact of self-regulation on preferences. Willpower theory (Baumeister, Heatherton, and Tice 1994) posits that, as one uses his regulatory resource (e.g., to make decisions or resist temptation), less energy remains available for subsequent activities. Since this source of energy is limited, yet so essential to our daily activities, we conjecture that humans attempt to spare it as much as possible. Therefore, we predict that depletion of regulatory resource should contribute to the strengthening of consumers preferences. Indeed, we believe that preferences can be used by consumers strategically, though not necessarily consciously, as a way to economize their self-regulatory resource. Doing so would enable them to cope with day-today tasks (e.g., grocery shopping) without further depleting their finite inner strength.

Session 1-8 Participants and procedure. A total of 96 undergraduate students from a large American university participated voluntarily in a one-hour research session as part of a course requirement. The procedure follows a two-group design and was conducted in two phases. Phase 1 took place at the beginning of the semester when we asked participants to complete a preference survey featuring ten product categories (e.g., automakers, television networks, cola brands, etc). Each category was composed of four brands. Students were to select which brand in the set of four they prefer. In addition, participants indicated on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 9 (very) how strong their preference for their favorite option was over the next best one. Phase 2 took place at the end of the semester. Participants were invited back to the laboratory to participate in yet another hour long research session in exchange for course credit. Phase 2 manipulation. Upon arrival to the laboratory, participants were randomly assigned to either one of two conditions (i.e., control vs. depletion) and invited to a room where snacks (e.g., a granny smith apple and cream pie cookie) had been displayed on the table. Students were instructed not to touch the snacks as they were part of a study coming later in the session. To manipulate the state of depletion of our subjects, we used duration of exposure to the snacks. Indeed, because resisting temptation (i.e., eating the snacks) depletes one s regulatory resource, we gave control (depletion) participants five (fifteen) minutes of filler tasks before allowing them to complete a version of our preference survey similar to the one they had completed months earlier (i.e., in phase 1). Upon completion of the survey, participants filled out Watson, Clark, and Tellegen s (1988) PANAS scales. Dependent variables. We hypothesized that a state of depletion of the regulatory resource would lead to preference strengthening while a normal (i.e., control) state would leave preferences unchanged. To test our prediction, we subtracted our measure of preference strength in phase 2 from that of phase 1. As an additional measure, we computed the number of preference matches between the two phases. This number were to range from 0 (i.e., no match at all) to 10 (i.e., perfect match on all ten product categories). Results. Results for our secondary measure were not significant: Participants in both conditions exhibited the same number of matches (M control = 8.3 vs. M depletion = 8.3; F(1, 94) < 1, NS). An ANOVA on our primary measure, however, validated our prediction: Unlike control participants, depleted participants experienced a strengthening of their preferences (M control =.0 vs. M depletion =.8; F(1, 94) > 7, p <.01). An alternative explanation for these results might be that depleted participants became upset and irritable because of their longer waiting before being allowed to get to the snacks. Their state of irritability, rather than their state of depletion, may thus have caused their preference strengthening. Yet, results from the PANAS scales rule out such alternative explanation. Indeed, participants of both conditions exhibited the same levels of positive affect (M control = 24.2 vs. M depletion = 25.8; F(1, 94) < 1, NS) and negative affect (M control = 15.2 vs. M depletion = 13.6; F(1, 94) < 1, NS). Discussion Drawing from recent findings in social psychology, we derived new insights for research on consumer behavior and decision making. Using an experimental paradigm manipulating one s state of regulatory resource, we established that, while depletion of regulatory resource does not lead to greater brand choice consistency (i.e., more matches), it does lead consumers to strengthen their preferences for their favorite brands. Managerial implications. Brands and retailers are the ones with most to gain from our findings. Indeed, since self-regulatory resource is limited, yet central to most daily

Session 1-9 activities, it follows that consumers tend to be far more depleted later in the day than in the morning hours. One direct takeaway from this research would be for retailers to promote utilitarian & righteous products before 4pm, when shoppers still have enough resource available to fight temptation and persuasive attempts. Promotion for hedonic & indulgent products, on the other hand, should take place later when shoppers are more likely to succumb temptation and preference strength is heightened. Point of purchase promotion may include live speakers interacting with the shopping crowd, or automated couponrelease systems. Such systems would be programmed to release instant rebates promoting righteous products during the day and indulging products at night, thereby exploiting the natural depletion pattern of consumers. Future research. Self-regulation is a critical factor of daily life. Whether it be coping with stress, dealing with co-workers, or simply driving, the self is constantly required to focus one s thoughts and emotions toward making decisions, solving problems and pursuing goals. Research by Tangney and Baumeister (2001) suggests that self-control can be a stable trait of personality. Given the relationship between self-control and preferences our findings highlight, the interplay of such trait with the preference formation process offers promising avenues of research.

Session 1-10 References Allenby, G. and P. Rossi (1999), Marketing Models of Consumer Heterogeneity, Journal of Econometrics, 89, 57-78. Baumeister, Roy, F., Todd F. Heatherton, and Dianne M. Tice (1994), Losing Control: How and Why People Fail at Self-Regulation, San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Bass, F. (1974), The Stochastic of Preference and Brand Switching, Journal of Marketing Research, 11 (May), 1-20. Carver, Charles s. and Michael F. Scheier (1981), Attention and Self-Regulation: A Control Theory Approach to Human Behavior, New York: Springer-Verlag. Carver, Charles s. and Michael F. Scheier (1982), Control Theory: A Useful Conceptual Framework for Personality-Social, Clinical, and Health Psychology, Psychological Bulletin, 92 (July), 111-135. Hull, Jay G. (1981), A Self-Awareness Model of the Causes and Effects of Alcohol Consumption, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90 (December), 586-600. McFadden, D. (1986), The Choice Theory Approach to Market Research, Marketing Science, 5 (Fall), 275-297. Polivy, Janet, C. Peter Herman, Rick Hackett, and Irka Kileshnyk (1986), The Effects of Self-Attention and Public Attention on Eating in Restrained and Unrestrained Subjects, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50 (June), 1253-1260. Seetharaman, P. (2003), Probabilistic vs. Random-Utility Models of State Dependence: An Empirical Comparison, International Journal of Research in Marketing, 20, 87-96. Tangney, June P. and Roy F. Baumeister (2001), High Self-Control Predicts Good Adjustment, Less Pathology, Better Grades, and Interpersonal Success, unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA, 22030. Tice, D, Ellen Bratislavski, and Roy Baumeister (2001), Emotional Distress Regulation Takes Precedence over Impulse Control: If You Feel Bad, Do it!, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80 (January), 53-67. Watson, David, Lee Anna Clark, and Auke Tellegen (1988), Development and Validation of Brief Measures of Positive Affect and Negative Affect: The PANAS Scales, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 54, No. 6, 1063-1070.