Effects of partial masking for vehicle sounds

Similar documents
An algorithm modelling the Irrelevant Sound Effect (ISE)

Comparison of subjective impression of copy machine noise between Japanese, American and German participants

CROSSCULTURAL EFFECTS IN AUDIO-VISUAL INTERACTIONS

Issues faced by people with a Sensorineural Hearing Loss

Psychoacoustical Models WS 2016/17

Effect on car interior sound quality according to the variation of noisy components of tire-pattern noise

Effects of aural combination tones on the loudness of a pure tone masked by an inharmonic pure-tone

Frequency refers to how often something happens. Period refers to the time it takes something to happen.

Detectability and hearing impression of additional warning sounds for electric or hybrid vehicles

Keywords: time perception; illusion; empty interval; filled intervals; cluster analysis

Design of Sporty SQI using Semantic Differential and Verification of its Effectiveness

Study of perceptual balance for binaural dichotic presentation

Masked Perception Thresholds of Low Frequency Tones Under Background Noises and Their Estimation by Loudness Model

Effects of speaker's and listener's environments on speech intelligibili annoyance. Author(s)Kubo, Rieko; Morikawa, Daisuke; Akag

A NOVEL HEAD-RELATED TRANSFER FUNCTION MODEL BASED ON SPECTRAL AND INTERAURAL DIFFERENCE CUES

functions grow at a higher rate than in normal{hearing subjects. In this chapter, the correlation

Automatic Live Monitoring of Communication Quality for Normal-Hearing and Hearing-Impaired Listeners

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization

ClaroTM Digital Perception ProcessingTM

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Japan Suggestions for AVAS sound requirements JASIC

1706 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113 (3), March /2003/113(3)/1706/12/$ Acoustical Society of America

Subjective impression of copy machine noises: an improvement of their sound quality based on physical metrics

Linguistic Phonetics. Basic Audition. Diagram of the inner ear removed due to copyright restrictions.

Development of a new loudness model in consideration of audio-visual interaction

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Masking effects of one s own voice

BINAURAL DICHOTIC PRESENTATION FOR MODERATE BILATERAL SENSORINEURAL HEARING-IMPAIRED

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

Hearing threshold measurements of infrasound combined with audio frequency sound

An Examination on Required Sound Levels for Acoustic Warning Devices for Quiet Vehicles

Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Week 1. Laboratory session: Measuring thresholds

Hearing. Figure 1. The human ear (from Kessel and Kardon, 1979)

Basic Environmental Noise and Noise Perception. 4-Feb-16

Hearing in Noise Test in Subjects With Conductive Hearing Loss

Comparison of some listening test methods : a case study

Modeling individual loudness perception in cochlear implant recipients with normal contralateral hearing

INVESTIGATION OF PERCEPTION AT INFRASOUND FRE- QUENCIES BY FUNCTIONAL MAGNETIC RESONANCE IM- AGING (FMRI) AND MAGNETOENCEPHALOGRAPHY (MEG)

Perceptual Effects of Nasal Cue Modification

How high-frequency do children hear?

DOES FILLED DURATION ILLUSION TAKE PLACE FOR VERY SHORT TIME INTERVALS?

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 THE DUPLEX-THEORY OF LOCALIZATION INVESTIGATED UNDER NATURAL CONDITIONS

Ambiguity in the recognition of phonetic vowels when using a bone conduction microphone

Temporal offset judgments for concurrent vowels by young, middle-aged, and older adults

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

EEL 6586, Project - Hearing Aids algorithms

Thresholds for different mammals

MedRx HLS Plus. An Instructional Guide to operating the Hearing Loss Simulator and Master Hearing Aid. Hearing Loss Simulator

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology. Psychoacoustics of hearing impairment

Sound Quality Evaluation of Hermetic Compressors Using Artificial Neural Networks

Audiogram+: The ReSound Proprietary Fitting Algorithm

Categorical loudness scaling and equal-loudness contours in listeners with normal hearing and hearing loss

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Statistical distribution of normal hearing thresholds under free-field listening conditions

Sound design of electric vehicles - Challenges and risks

Perception of tonal components contained in wind turbine noise

Auditory fmri correlates of loudness perception for monaural and diotic stimulation

A SYSTEM FOR CONTROL AND EVALUATION OF ACOUSTIC STARTLE RESPONSES OF ANIMALS

Review of Methods for Quantifying Tonalness in Noise. Quantifying Tonalness

A PROTOTYPE OF AN INFORMATION SYSTEM FOR URBAN SOUNDSCAPE A PROTOTYPE OF AN INFORMATION SYSTEM FOR URBAN SOUNDSCAPE.

Technical Discussion HUSHCORE Acoustical Products & Systems

Abstract.

Enrique A. Lopez-Poveda Alan R. Palmer Ray Meddis Editors. The Neurophysiological Bases of Auditory Perception

Investigation on high-frequency noise in public space.

Phonak Target. SoundRecover2 adult fitting guide. Content. The Connecting the hearing instruments. February 2018

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Impact of the ambient sound level on the system's measurements CAPA

Speech intelligibility in simulated acoustic conditions for normal hearing and hearing-impaired listeners

Spraak verstaan in fluctuerend achtergrond geluid bij normaal en slechthorenden als functie van ruis niveau

Linguistic Phonetics Fall 2005

Optimal Design: Bekesy Test for Mobile

Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Perceptual evaluation of noise reduction in hearing aids Brons, I. Link to publication

Neural correlates of the perception of sound source separation

Hearing. Juan P Bello

Healthy Organ of Corti. Loss of OHCs. How to use and interpret the TEN(HL) test for diagnosis of Dead Regions in the cochlea

Audiogram+: GN Resound proprietary fitting rule

CLASSROOM AMPLIFICATION: WHO CARES? AND WHY SHOULD WE? James Blair and Jeffery Larsen Utah State University ASHA, San Diego, 2011

Effect of spectral content and learning on auditory distance perception

Binaural hearing and future hearing-aids technology

Threshold of hearing for pure tone under free-field

ID# Exam 2 PS 325, Fall 2003

Minimum Audible Angles Measured with Simulated Normally-Sized and Oversized Pinnas for Normal-Hearing and Hearing- Impaired Test Subjects

SPEECH PERCEPTION IN A 3-D WORLD

Power Instruments, Power sources: Trends and Drivers. Steve Armstrong September 2015

The Hearwig as hearing protector for musicians a practical investigation

An active unpleasantness control system for indoor noise based on auditory masking

Intelligibility of narrow-band speech and its relation to auditory functions in hearing-impaired listeners

Time Varying Comb Filters to Reduce Spectral and Temporal Masking in Sensorineural Hearing Impairment

What s New in Primus

Influence of Visual Information on Subjective Evaluation of Road Traffic Noise

INTRODUCTION TO PURE (AUDIOMETER & TESTING ENVIRONMENT) TONE AUDIOMETERY. By Mrs. Wedad Alhudaib with many thanks to Mrs.

APPENDIX G NOISE TERMINOLOGY

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

Community Noise Fundamentals

Speech perception in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer s type (DAT) Mitchell S. Sommers Department of Psychology Washington University

A linear relation between loudness and decibels

Sound Waves. Sensation and Perception. Sound Waves. Sound Waves. Sound Waves

Transcription:

Effects of partial masking for vehicle sounds Hugo FASTL 1 ; Josef KONRADL 2 ; Stefan KERBER 3 1 AG Technische Akustik, MMK, TU München, Germany 2 now at: ithera Medical GmbH, München, Germany 3 now at: Müller-BBM Active Sound Technology GmbH, Planegg, Germany ABSTRACT In psychoacoustic assessments of vehicle sounds, the target sounds are usually presented in a quiet background. However, in practical applications, vehicle sounds are commonly superimposed by background noises. Therefore, in a pilot study, idle noises of two passenger cars with gasoline vs. Diesel engine were partially masked by a synthetic vs. a natural background noise. As synthetic background noise, uniform exciting noise (UEN) was chosen, which is similar to pink noise, but has the additional advantage that it shows the same level in each critical band. As natural background noise, a recording of a pedestrian area was used. The results reveal different partial masking for different engines as well as background noises. Effects of sound signature seem to play an important role in partial masking of real world sounds. Keywords: Partial masking, Uniform exciting noise, Pedestrian area, Diesel engine, Gasoline engine, Sound signature I-INCE Classification of Subjects Numbers: 61.5, 63.1 INTRODUCTION In psychoacoustic assessments of vehicle sounds, the target sounds are usually presented in a quiet background. However, in practical applications, vehicle sounds are commonly superimposed by background noises. Therefore, in a pilot study, idle noises of two passenger cars with gasoline vs. Diesel engine were partially masked by a synthetic vs. a natural background noise. As synthetic background noise, uniform exciting noise (UEN) was chosen which contains no audible fluctuations. As natural background noise, a recording of a pedestrian area was used, including voice sounds and clearly audible fluctuations. Classic work on partial masking (1, 2, 3) addresses the superposition of a test sound by a masking sound which, however, does not completely mask the test sound to inaudibility, but reduces its loudness. Effects of such partial masking are illustrated in figure 1. 1 2 3 fastl@mmk.ei.tum.de josefkonradl@web.de stefan.kerber@mbbm.com 4583

Figure 1 Partial masking of pure tones at 1 khz by pink noise (4) In figure 1, the perceived loudness of a 1 khz tone is plotted as a function of its level. Without masking background noise, the dashed curve is obtained. This is the classical curve showing how perceived loudness increases with increasing sound pressure level. For tone levels above 40 db it illustrates the well known relation that a level increase by 10 db increases perceived loudness by a factor of two. If in addition to the 1 khz tones pink noise with 1/3-oct band levels of 40 db (circles) or 60 db (triangles) is added, results illustrated by the respective solid lines are obtained. They show that the test tones remain inaudible until they reach a certain level (filled symbols). Increasing the level beyond this point leads to a steep increase of loudness above threshold and an approach to normal loudness values for high test tone levels. This behavior is typical for partial masking and it shows also up in persons with hearing deficits, e.g. noise induced hearing loss, where it is called recruitment. EXPERIMENTS Subjects Ten subjects with normal hearing ability (HL < 20 db) aged between 20 and 28 years (median 24 years) took part and concluded all experiments. One subject was female, nine subjects were male. From the originally 12 subjects, two were excluded, since they had problems to understand the task and gave always the same answers irrespective of the vehicle sounds presented. Obviously, they rated the background sounds instead of the vehicle sounds. Sounds and procedure Vehicle sounds were recorded in a semi-anechoic chamber 1.2 m in front of the idling vehicle. Two rather different vehicle sounds were used: one from a vehicle with a six cylinder gasoline engine, and one from a vehicle with a four cylinder Diesel engine. The original levels of the vehicle sounds were attenuated or amplified to yield levels L T of 45, 50, 55, 60, 70, and 80 db, respectively. As background noises, also two rather different sounds were chosen: The synthetic background noise was uniform exciting noise (UEN) which is rather similar to pink noise, but has the additional advantage that it shows the same level L cb in each critical band (4). For a level of L cb = 40 db in each critical band and 24 critical bands a total level of L UEN = 40 + 10 lg 24 = 53.8 db is obtained. As natural background noise, the sound recorded in a pedestrian area with a level of L PedAr = 68 db was chosen. 4584

Sounds were presented in a sound attenuating booth diotically via electrodynamic headphones (Beyer DT 48) with freefield equalizer (4). Each sound was presented to each subject four times in random order; hence the median values reported are based on 40 datapoints each. The sequence of sounds is illustrated in figure 2. Figure 2 Illustration of the sequence of sounds used for the experiments According to the illustration in figure 2, first the background noise with 1 s duration was presented. After 0.3 s from the start of the background noise, the vehicle sound of 0.7 s duration was added. Then after a pause of 0.3 s duration the vehicle sound of 0.7 s duration (without background noise) was presented. The initial time gap of 0.3 s helps to distinguish between background noise and vehicle sound. The procedure used was a Method of Adjustment. The task of the subject was to vary the level of the second vehicle sound in such a way that it produced the same perceived loudness as the first vehicle sound partially masked by the background noise. The user interface to implement this Method of Adjustment is illustrated in figure 3. Figure 3 Illustration of the user interface for the method of adjustment The sequence of sounds illustrated in figure 2 was started by hitting the button PLAY indicated in figure 3. By operating the slider with the mouse, the level of the second vehicle sound was varied. This procedure was repeated until the first and the second vehicle sound elicited the same loudness. By hitting NEXT the level of the previous adjustment was stored and the next combination of vehicle sound and background noise was started. 4585

RESULTS The results of the present pilot study are summarized in figure 4. In each panel, the level of the respective second vehicle sound which produces the same loudness as the partially masked first vehicle sound is given as a function of the level L T of the respective first vehicle sound. Figure 4 Level of the respective second vehicle sound as a function of the corresponding first vehicle sound L T. Upper panels: Background noise of pedestrian area. Lower panels: Background noise uniform exciting noise (UEN). Left panels: Vehicle with Diesel engine. Right panels: Vehicle with gasoline engine. The solid curves connect medians of adjusted values of, the dashed curves illustrate L T = The results displayed in figure 4 suggest that at high levels L T of the vehicle sounds no partial masking occurred. This effect is indicated in figure 4 by the dotted line which represents equality of L T and. For low levels L T, the solid curves clearly deviate from the dotted lines, i.e. substantial partial masking showed up. The magnitude of the partial masking depended both on background noise and vehicle sound. In order to assess these influences in detail, the median levels are again compiled in Tables 1 and 2. For the levels L T of the vehicle sounds, the corresponding levels of the respective second sound as well as the level difference = L T are given. can be regarded as a measure of the reduction of the loudness of the vehicle sound by the background noise and hence as a measure of the magnitude of partial masking. 4586

Table 1 Partial masking of vehicle sounds by uniform exciting noise Diesel Gasoline L T 45 21 24 17.5 27.5 50 33 17 38 12 55 49.5 5.5 48 7 60 58 2 58 2 70 69.8 0.2 69 1 80 80 0 80 0 The data displayed in Table 1 would seem to suggest that at low level L T of the vehicle sound, the partial masking is larger for the vehicle sound with gasoline engine than Diesel engine. This holds in particular for levels L T of 45 and 55 db. Because of less partial masking, the vehicle with the Diesel engine might be easier detected in the continuous UEN background due to its time-variant sound signature. If this argument of sound signature holds, the vehicle with gasoline engine should incur less partial masking in the pedestrian area, because the steady engine sound contrasts with the time varying background noise. This hypothesis can be checked by means of the data displayed in Table 2. Table 2 Partial masking of vehicle sounds by noise from a pedestrian area Diesel Gasoline L T 45 20 25 25 20 50 32 18 35 15 55 44 11 51 4 60 57 3 58 2 70 69 1 70 0 80 80 0 80 0 A comparison of values in Table 2 for sounds from vehicles with Diesel vs. gasoline engines reveals that the approach to take into account the signature of the vehicle sounds would be supported by the data displayed. At all values of L T from 45 to 70 db the values for the vehicle with the Diesel engine are larger than the values for the vehicle with the gasoline engine. DISCUSSION Results of this pilot study showed that partial masking of vehicle sounds by background noise may be influenced by effects of sound signature. If in contrast to partial masking, (complete) masking of vehicle sounds by background noise is studied, sound signature seems to be of minor importance. For example, Kerber (5) in his concept of critical distance could faithfully predict the audibility of approaching vehicles in various background noises from the sound pressure of the vehicle, the vehicle speed, and the sound pressure of the background noise (6). Before giving effects of sound signature too much weight it should be mentioned that the present pilot study shows some drawbacks: Most severe may be the fact that different levels of the vehicle sounds were produced by attenuation or amplification of original levels. This inevitably leads to substantial distortions in timbre. Also, durations of vehicle sounds of only 0.7 s may be somewhat short compared to durations in real life. Although the partial masking of vehicle sounds by background noises during idling definitely is of interest, experiments with sounds from approaching vehicles as used by Kerber (7) may be even more important for practical applications. 4587

Despite these drawbacks the present pilot study clearly showed that experiments on partial masking are not confined to synthetic signals but can be performed by subjects also with real world sounds with comparable accuracy of measurement. For future experiments, both the sequence of sounds and the implemented method of adjustment can be recommended. REFERENCES 1. Zwicker E. Über die Lautheit von ungedrosselten und gedrosselten Schallen. Acustica 1963; 13:194-211. 2. Scharf B. Partial masking. Acustica 1964;14:16-23. 3. Gleiss N, Zwicker E. Loudness function in the presence of masking noise. J Acoust Soc Am. 1964;36:393-394. 4. Fastl H, Zwicker E. Psychoacoustics, Facts and Models (3 rd edition). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer-Verlag; 2007. 5. Kerber S. The Importance of Vehicle Exterior Noise Levels in Urban Traffic for Pedestrian - Vehicle Interaction. ATZ Worldwide 2006; 108(7-8):19-21. 6. Kerber S, Fastl H. Perceptibility of approaching vehicles in urban background noise. Proc. of 36th Intern. Congress on Noise Control Engineering INTER-NOISE 2007; 28-31 August 2007; Istanbul, Turkey 2007. 7. Kerber S. Wahrnehmbarkeit von Fahrzeugaußengeräuschen in Hintergrundgeräuschen: Psychoakustische Beurteilungen und modellbasierte Prognosen. München: Verlag Dr. Hut; 2008. 4588