Insular Cortex and Consummatory Successive Negative Contrast in the Rat

Similar documents
Insular Cortex Lesions and Morphine-Induced Suppression of Conditioned Stimulus Intake in the Rat

Conditioned Taste Aversion and Latent Inhibition Following Extensive Taste Preexposure in Rats with Insular Cortex Lesions

Morphine-induced Suppression of Conditioned Stimulus Intake: Effects of Stimulus Type and Insular Cortex Lesions

The psychology department at Memorial University provided a supportive environment in which I was surrounded and encouraged by eminent scholars

Supplementary Methods

Basolateral Amygdala and Morphine-Induced Taste Avoidance in the Rat

Taste Neophobia and c-fos Expression in the Rat Brain

Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) Toronto, Ont., Canada M6A 2E1 b Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque,

Decremental Carryover Effects of Sucrose Ingestion in the Negative Anticipatory Contrast Procedure in Rats

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES ABSTRACT

Insular Cortex-Amygdala Dialogue During Taste Recognition Memory Formation

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

Supporting Online Material for

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning

Fatty acids enhance the perceived taste intensity of non-nutritive sweeteners, saccharin & sucralose, in male and female rats

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives?

Supporting Online Material for

Independence of neophobia and taste aversion learning

Do Rats Consume Ethanol to Regulate a Negative Emotion Induced by a Successive Negative Contrast Procedure?

GABAergic Influences Increase Ingestion across All Taste Categories. Liz Miller. Molly McGinnis. Lindsey Richardson

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

Perceptual Learning in Flavor Aversion: Evidence for Learned Changes in Stimulus Effectiveness

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats

BRIEF REPORT. Impairments in Sodium Appetite after Lesions of Gustatory Thalamus: Replication and Extension I

Figure1. Trials. (ml/5min) (licks/3min) (ml/5min) Fischer. Lewis. Mean Intake Mean Saccharin Intake. Saline Cocaine.

Pattern Memory Involves Both Elemental and Configural Processes: Evidence From the Effects of Hippocampal Lesions

Sensitization of Salt Appetite Is Associated With Increased Wanting but Not Liking of a Salt Reward in the Sodium-Deplete Rat

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning

Temporal and Qualitative Dynamics of Conditioned Taste Aversion Processing: Combined Generalization Testing and Licking Microstructure Analysis

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

A learned flavor preference persists despite the extinction of conditioned hedonic reactions to the cue flavors

Subjects. Thirty-five adult male Lister Hooded rats (Charles River, Kent, UK),

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors

Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Diverse anorexigenic signals induce c-fos expression in CEl PKC-δ + neurons

Second-order conditioning during a compound extinction treatment

The Role of Temporal Relationships in the Transfer of Conditioned Inhibition

Disruption of latent inhibition and perceptual learning

Integrated Memory for Object, Place, and Context in Rats: A Possible Model of Episodic-Like Memory?

Conditions under which amobarbital sodium influences contrast in consummatory behavior

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is

The Journal of Physiology Neuroscience

Declarative memory includes semantic, episodic, and spatial memory, and

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats

Limbic system outline

Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning

The highlight for January, 2010 is by Shadna Rana who is in the Department of Physiology and Pharmacology at University of Calgary in Alberta,

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

The Neural Substrates of Incidental Sensory Experience. Senior Thesis. Presented to

Overshadowing not potentiation of illness-based contextual conditioning by a novel taste

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Associative learning

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Adolescent Prozac Exposure Enhances Sensitivity to Cocaine in Adulthood INTRODUCTION

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

Sensorimotor Functioning. Sensory and Motor Systems. Functional Anatomy of Brain- Behavioral Relationships

Effect of Benzodiazepines within the PBN on Taste Guided Licking Behavior. Baker Bragg, Alexandra Brantly, Sarah Evans & Reed Mulbry.

Doctor of Philosophy

EFFECTS OF NITRIC OXIDE SYNTHASE INHIBITOR N G -NITRO-L-ARGININE METHYL ESTER ON SPATIAL AND CUED LEANING

Context specificity of sensory preconditioning: Implications for processes of within-event learning

Representation of negative motivational value in the primate

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

Food Intake After Diazepam, Morphine or Muscimol: Microinjections in the Nucleus Accumbens Shell

Chemical Senses: Taste and Smell. Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

Taste. Alexis, Emma, Maureen

Perirhinal Cortex Lesions in Rats: Novelty Detection and Sensitivity to Interference

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

Effects of housing on consummatory successive negative contrast in rats: wirebottom cages versus polycarbonate tubs

Mouse Taste Preference Tests: Influence of Drinking Spout Position

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning

Orbital prefrontal cortex and guidance of instrumental behaviour in rats under reversal conditions

Nucleus accumbens lesions impair context, but not cue, conditioning in rats

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

The differential effects of additional cortical lesions in rats with single- or. multiple-stage lesions of the visual cortex

The individual animals, the basic design of the experiments and the electrophysiological

Introduction to Systems Neuroscience. Nov. 28, The limbic system. Daniel C. Kiper

Taste quality and extinction of a conditioned taste aversion in rats

CROSSMODAL PLASTICITY IN SPECIFIC AUDITORY CORTICES UNDERLIES VISUAL COMPENSATIONS IN THE DEAF "

Reward prediction based on stimulus categorization in. primate lateral prefrontal cortex

The Effects of Benzodiazepines on Ingestive Behavior. Hannah Dinnen & Ivy Far

Veterinary Science Communications, 3 (1979) Elsevier Scientific Publishing Company, Amsterdam - Printed in The Netherlands

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

9.14 Class 32 Review. Limbic system

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Brain Mechanisms of Emotion 1 of 6

The Role of Mitral Cells in State Dependent Olfactory Responses. Trygve Bakken & Gunnar Poplawski

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Transcription:

Butler University Digital Commons @ Butler University Scholarship and Professional Work COPHS College of Pharmacy & Health Sciences 2009 Insular Cortex and Consummatory Successive Negative Contrast in the Rat Jian-You Lin Christopher T. Roman Butler University, croman@butler.edu Steve Reilly Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.butler.edu/cophs_papers Recommended Citation Lin, Jian-You; Roman, Christopher T.; and Reilly, Steve, "Insular Cortex and Consummatory Successive Negative Contrast in the Rat" (2009). Scholarship and Professional Work COPHS. 164. http://digitalcommons.butler.edu/cophs_papers/164 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the College of Pharmacy & Health Sciences at Digital Commons @ Butler University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Scholarship and Professional Work COPHS by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ Butler University. For more information, please contact omacisaa@butler.edu.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Behav Neurosci. 2009 August ; 123(4): 810 814. doi:10.1037/a0016460. Insular Cortex and Consummatory Successive Negative Contrast in the Rat Jian-You Lin, Christopher Roman, and Steve Reilly Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Chicago. Abstract Rats that are expecting a high value reward (e.g., 1.0 M sucrose) show an exaggerated underresponding when they are instead given a low value reward (e.g., 0.15% saccharin), an effect termed successive negative contrast (SNC). In the present experiment, insular cortex-lesioned (ICX) rats showed normal responsivity to sucrose and saccharin prior to the reward downshift. However, when switched from sucrose to saccharin during the postshift trials these rats displayed no evidence of SNC. Indeed, over the downshift trials these ICX rats consistently drank more saccharin than the ICX rats maintained on saccharin throughout the experiment. Potential interpretations are discussed including a lesion-induced impairment in the ability to accurately recognize the novelty of the postshift saccharin stimulus. Keywords insular cortex; incentive relativity; neophobia; rats Introduction The ability to accurately predict the nutritive quality of a gustatory stimulus is important to ensure that humans and non-human animals make sound decisions about the foods they ingest. Comparisons between foods are therefore of great consequence, allowing for the optimizing of nutritional gains from a given feeding episode. Rats that are expecting a high-value reward (H; e.g., 1.0 M sucrose) make significantly fewer licks when given a low-value reward (L; e.g., 0.15% saccharin) relative to rats that receive an expected L. This phenomenon is known as consummatory successive negative contrast (SNC; Flaherty & Hamilton, 1971; Vogel, Mikulka & Spear, 1968), and demonstrates that rats respond according to the relative value of the current L as determined by a comparison with the absolute value of an expected but absent H. Because of the relevance to foraging strategies and food selection, a great deal of information has accrued about the behavioral characteristics of SNC (for a review see Flaherty, 1996). Indeed, based on his exhaustive review of the literature, Flaherty described a multistage model for SNC as follows. Consummatory contact with the unexpected L on the first postshift trial Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Steve Reilly Department of Psychology University of Illinois at Chicago 1007 West Harrison Street Chicago, IL 60607 Fax: (312) 413-4122 sreilly@uic.edu. Publisher's Disclaimer: The following manuscript is the final accepted manuscript. It has not been subjected to the final copyediting, fact-checking, and proofreading required for formal publication. It is not the definitive, publisher-authenticated version. The American Psychological Association and its Council of Editors disclaim any responsibility or liabilities for errors or omissions of this manuscript version, any version derived from this manuscript by NIH, or other third parties. The published version is available at www.apa.org/journals/bne.

Lin et al. Page 2 Method Subjects triggers a comparison with the neural representation of the memory of the expected H. This comparison generates a mismatch that results in a search for the missing H. Continuing for a few minutes, the unsuccessful search precipitates a negative emotional response, due to an approach-avoidance conflict between accepting (because of its absolute reward value) and avoiding (because of its relative reward value) the unexpected L. Indicative of conflict resolution, the rat eventually consumes the postshift reward because of the absolute value of the now expected L. Relative to the number of behavioral and pharmacological investigations, substantially fewer studies have examined the neural substrates of SNC. One brain area of particular relevance to present purposes is the parvicellular region of the ventral posteromedial nucleus of the thalamus, also known as the gustatory thalamus (GT). Reilly and Trifunovic (1999) and Sastre and Reilly (2006) found that GT-lesioned (GTX) rats that were switched from H to L rapidly (i.e., on the first postshift trial) adjusted their responding to the same level as their unshifted counterparts maintained on L throughout the experiment. Thus, irrespective of whether they had prior experience with H or not, the GTX rats responded according to the absolute value of the current reward. That is, GT lesions prevent the occurrence of SNC. Providing evidence that the absence of SNC in GTX rats was not due to an impairment of gustatory memory, Reilly and Trifunovic (2003) favored an explanation in terms of a lesion-induced disruption of the reward comparison mechanism that underlies the phenomenon. This disruption, it was argued, preempts both the search for the missing H and the ensuing approach-avoidance conflict and consequently the GTX rats respond according to the absolute value of unexpected L. Another form of consummatory contrast, anticipatory negative contrast (ANC), is also dependent upon a determination of the relative values of two rewards. In the ANC task, rats suppress responding to L when it is sequentially paired with H in a one-trial per day procedure (Flaherty & Checke, 1982). Supportive of the view that SNC and ANC each involve a similar reward comparison mechanism, GT lesions also prevent the occurrence of ANC (Reilly, Bornovalova & Trifunovic, 2004; Reilly & Pritchard, 1996; Schroy et al., 2005). The GT is reciprocally connected with the gustatory area of the insular cortex (IC; Cechetto & Saper, 1987; Kosar, Grill, & Norgren, 1986), which encourages the view that the IC also may be involved in SNC. Indeed, it is possible that the SNC deficit displayed by GTX rats results from a lesion-induced disruption in the transmission of gustatory information to an IC reward comparison mechanism. Using the same behavioral procedure as Reilly and Trifunovic (1999) and Sastre and Reilly (2006), the present experiment examined the influence of bilateral excitotoxic lesions of the IC on consummatory SNC. Forty-one male Sprague-Dawley rats, weighing ~300 g, were obtained from Charles River Laboratories (Wilmington, MA) and individually housed in stainless steel hanging cages in a vivarium maintained on a 12 hr alternating light-dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 AM). All treatments and behavioral testing occurred during the light phase of the cycle. The rats had free access to food and water at all times except during behavioral testing when they were maintained at 80% body weight by a once per day feeding (~15 g lab chow) given a minimum of 30 min after completion of the behavioral testing. The Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Illinois at Chicago approved the experimental protocols and the care and use of the rats conformed to the guidelines of the American Psychological Association (1996).

Lin et al. Page 3 Surgery Apparatus Procedure Twenty-one rats were given bilateral lesions of the IC (Group ICX). Each rat was anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (injected i.p. at 50 mg/kg), fixed in a stereotaxic apparatus with blunt ear bars, and the head was shaved and sterilized with Povidone (Qualitest Pharmaceuticals, Huntsville, AL). A midline incision was made to expose the skull surface, and the head was leveled between bregma and lamda by adjusting the level of the bite bar. Body temperature was monitored with a rectal thermometer and maintained at 37 C with a heating pad. Trephine holes were drilled above the IC in each hemisphere. A glass micropipette (~70 μm tip diameter) filled with 0.15 M N-methyl-D-aspartic acid (NMDA; Sigma, St. Louis, MO), was lowered, in turn, to two lesions sites in each hemisphere. At site 1 (AP +1.2 mm; ML ±5.2 mm; DV 5.0 mm) a 10-min iontophoretic current was applied using a Midgard precision current source (Stoelting, Wood Dale, IL); site 2 (AP +1.2 mm; ML ±5.2 mm; DV 4.3 mm) involved a 6-min infusion of NMDA. After the final infusion, the incision was closed with wound clip and the rat was allowed to recover under a heat lamp until it regained consciousness. Ten rats received surgical treatments that were identical in all respects to the ICX surgeries with the exception that no NMDA infusions took place. An additional 10 rats were anesthetized but did not undergo any further treatment. These two control groups were collapsed together into a single group, Group SHAM. All subjects were given a minimum of 1 week to recover from surgery before undergoing weight reduction in preparation for the behavioral procedure. Six identical operant chambers (Med Associates Inc., St. Albans, VT), housed inside soundattenuating cubicles, were used. Each chamber had a stainless steel grid floor, aluminum sidewalls, and the front and back walls were, like the ceiling, made of clear Plexiglas. An oval access hole (1.3 2.6 cm; width height) was centered on the right side wall of each chamber, 6.0 cm above the floor. The stainless steel spout of a retractable stimulus bottle was accessible through this hole. Tongue contact with the spout was monitored using a lickometer circuit. A shaded bulb, which reflected light off the ceiling, was located directly above the cage speaker on the left sidewall. Inside each operant chamber, white noise provided an ambient sound level of 70 db background. Control of events in the chambers and collection of the data were carried out on-line by a computer running Med-PC software (Med Associates Inc.). Normal rats sometimes show large individual differences in saccharin consumption during the first few days in the test apparatus when they are learning to drink from the retractable spout, an effect we attribute, in part, to taste neophobia. To minimize the occurrence of this disruptive influence on performance in the test apparatus, we followed the procedure of Sastre and Reilly (2006) and gave the randomly assigned rats access to 5 ml of either 0.15% saccharin (Unshifted condition; 10 SHAM, 10 ICX) or 1.0 M sucrose (Shifted condition; 10 SHAM, 11 ICX) in the home cage on the day before the first context habituation session. All subjects were given 5 min access to the illuminated test chambers on each of the next two days. Beginning the next day, SHAM and ICX rats in the Unshifted control condition were given 5-min per day access to 0.15% saccharin for a total of 20 trials on a one trial per day protocol; the SHAM and ICX rats in the Shifted experimental condition were given 5-min daily access to 1.0 M sucrose on preshift Trials 1-14 and then switched to 0.15% saccharin on postshift Trials 15-20. The number of licks made during each trial was measured as the dependent variable.

Lin et al. Page 4 Histology Results Anatomical Behavioral After the conclusion of behavioral testing, the rats were given a lethal injection of sodium pentobarbital (100 mg/kg) and perfused transcardially with 4% buffered formalin. The brains were extracted and stored in 4% buffered formalin for 2 days, followed by two days in 20% sucrose. The brains were then blocked and frozen in a cryostat, and tissue sections were taken through the IC and mounted on slides. These sections were stained with cresyl violet and viewed under a light microscope. Photomicrographs were taken using Q-Capture (Quantitative Imaging Corporation; Burnaby, BC), and serial schematics created with Photoshop (Adobe; Seattle, WA) based upon images from Paxinos and Watson (2005) The target of the excitotoxic lesions in this experiment was the gustatory region of the IC which extends (Kosar et al., 1986) ~1 mm both anterior and posterior from the intersection of the middle cerebral artery and the rhinal fissure (approximately 0.8 mm anterior to bregma). Serial schematics of the largest and smallest IC lesions that were analyzed and representative lesions are shown in Figure 1A; Figure 1B shows a photomicrograph of an intact IC and Figure 1C displays a representative IC lesion. As shown, NMDA-induced damage was primary located within the IC. Some lesions spread into the somatosensory cortex, the claustrum and the piriform cortex but such damage was not consistently found across subjects. Rats with lesions that were either unilateral (n = 2) or subtotal (n = 4) were excluded and the final sample sizes were: 10 SHAM-Unshifted, 10 SHAM-Shifted, 8 ICX-Unshifted, and 7 ICX-Shifted. During the preshift phase (Trials 1-14), all groups showed a gradual increase in the number of licks made for either L or H (see Figure 2). This impression of the data was confirmed with an analysis of variance (ANOVA) that found a main effect of Trial, F(13, 390) = 42.15, p <.001. Additionally, a main effect of Condition (Unshifted, Shifted), F(1, 31) = 454.84, p <.001, indicated that the Shifted rats made more licks for H than the Unshifted rats made for L. There was, however, no main effect of IC lesions on responding (F > 1), and no interaction involving the Lesion factor (ps >.25). These results confirm that axon-sparing lesions of the IC have no influence on the detection or responsivity to the absolute value of L (0.15% saccharin) or H (1.0 M sucrose) during the preshift phase. When the SHAM-Shifted subjects were given L instead of H on Trial 15, they demonstrated SNC by decreasing their consumption below the level of the SHAM-Unshifted subjects. An ANOVA conducted on the six post-shift trials uncovered a Lesion X Condition X Trial interaction, F(5, 155) = 16.59, p <.001. To detect the source of the significant interaction, planned comparisons (F-test using the adjusted error term taken from the overall ANOVA) of the SHAM data revealed that the SHAM-Shifted rats made significantly fewer licks for L than did the SHAM-Unshifted subjects on Trials 15 and 16 (ps >.002), but not on Trials 17-20 (ps >.25). The performance of the SHAM-Unshifted subjects and the ICX-Unshifted rats did not differ during the post-shift phase of the experiment (F < 1). While the SHAM-Shifted subjects showed an exaggerated decrease in licks for L, the ICX- Shifted rats failed to show SNC, making more licks for L than the ICX-Unshifted rats on Trials 15-18 and 20 (ps <.03). On Trial 19, the performance of the two sets ICX rats was not significantly different (p =.09). As is clear from inspection of Figure 2, there was a dramatic difference in licking for L in the SHAM-Shifted and ICX-Shifted groups. That is, the ICX- Shifted rats made significantly more licks for L than the SHAM-Shifted subjects on Trials 15-18 (ps >.001), but not on the final two trials (ps >.05). So, the SHAM-Shifted and ICX-

Lin et al. Page 5 Discussion Shifted rats eventually made the same number of licks for L, but the SHAM subjects achieved this level of performance by gradually increasing their postshift responding over trials, F(5, 45) = 17.49, p <.001, whereas the ICX rats showed a gradual decrease of licking to achieve the same baseline, F(5, 30) = 2.77, p <.05. Overall, the postshift performance indicates the presence of SNC in SHAM subjects and the absence of SNC in ICX rats. As indicated by an exaggerated under-responding to the unexpected L on the initial postshift trials, neurologically intact (SHAM) rats displayed SNC. The ICX rats, however, showed no evidence of SNC. In fact, the ICX-Shifted rats over-responded for L on the postshift trials. Given the anatomical connectivity between the GT and IC, it is tempting to suggest that the IC, like the GT (e.g., Reilly & Trifunovic, 1999, 2003), is a neural component of the comparison mechanism that computes the relative value of gustatory rewards over time. If this is the case, then presumably these two structures are performing interdependent not identical functions because if the latter were true then one might expect that the intact GT would compensate for the absent IC and that ICX rats would show normal SNC. That this does not occur encourages the view that the GT and IC are each involved in different aspects of the reward comparison process. Beyond the present report, some recent findings shed additional light on the role of the IC in taste-guided behavior and a brief survey of these results provides reason to question the preceding analysis of the role of the IC in SNC. Lin, Roman, St. Andre and Reilly (2009) found that IC lesions disrupted the initial occurrence of, but not the recovery from, taste neophobia. Of further interest, the same ICX rats displayed normal neophobia to both an aqueous odor and an oral trigeminal stimulus. We interpret this pattern of impaired and spared neophobic reactions as a lesion-induced disruption of the perception of taste novelty. Second, this hypothesis anticipates that ICX rats should only show impaired CTA acquisition when the taste CS is novel; when the taste is familiar ICX rats would be expected to acquire the aversion at a similar slow rate as normal rats that have been preexposed to the taste CS. This prediction was confirmed by Roman, Lin and Reilly, 2009 (see also Kiefer & Braun, 1977, Roman & Reilly 2007). The results obtained from neophobia and CTA experiments encourage the view that ICX rats treat novel taste stimuli as though they were familiar (Reilly, 2009). In the present experiment IC lesions not only disrupted SNC but the pattern of postshift responding in ICX rats was different from that found in rats with GT lesions (Reilly & Trifunovic, 1999; Sastre & Reilly, 2006). Whereas ICX rats showed a gradual reduction of responding for L over the postshift trials relative to the ICX-Unshifted rats, GTX-Shifted rats in our earlier experiments immediately reduced their lick rate to that of the GTX-unshifted rats on the first postshift trial. Although this may represent a quantitative difference between the effects of the two types of lesions, the foregoing analysis encourages the view that an IC lesioninduced disruption of taste novelty is contributing to the pattern of results obtained in the present experiment. That is, the patterns of postshift responding may represent a qualitative difference between ICX and GTX rats. If this analysis is correct, then taste neophobia has a greater role in SNC than previously suspected. Few studies have examined whether the novelty of the postshift solution influences SNC. Indeed, we are aware of only one such report. Meinrath and Flaherty (1988) approached this issue by exposing subjects to a variety of preshift flavors on the assumption that such experience would reduce the influence of taste neophobia on the postshift L trials and thereby reduce the magnitude of SNC. Although Meinrath and Flaherty found that prior taste experience does influence postshift responding, the result was a reduction, not an elimination of SNC. Perhaps not unsurprisingly, the authors rejected the idea that a neophobic reaction to the postshift L

Lin et al. Page 6 Acknowledgments References was responsible for SNC. However, there are reasons to suspect that this rejection may have been premature. First, the preshift and postshift solutions used by Meinrath and Flaherty were different concentrations of the same taste stimulus (32% and 4% sucrose, respectively). Such a within-stimulus change might be expected to support less of a postshift neophobic reaction than occurs in our standard procedure which involves a between-stimulus switch from sucrose to saccharin, which is over-consumed by ICX rats when it is novel (Lin et al. 2009; Roman et al., 2006). That is, the bittersweet taste of saccharin might engage a more pronounced neophobic response than occurs following a reduction in the sweetness of sucrose. Second, the experimental manipulation used by Meinrath and Flaherty involved seven different flavors (i.e., maple, banana, lemon, chocolate, anise, orange and cherry) of the preshift solution (32% sucrose). It is possible that this procedure may have non-maximally influenced the neophobic reaction to the postshift solution (4% sucrose). Thus, there are reasons to suppose that neophobia has a meaningful, if under-appreciated role in SNC, a role that may be more pronounced when the expectancy violation involves a between-stimulus rather than a withinstimulus downshift in reward value. In the present experiment, excitotoxic lesions of the IC were found to have no influence on responsivity for sucrose or saccharin during the preshift trials. As noted in the Procedure section, rats were given pretraining access in the home cages to their preshift solution (either sucrose or saccharin) to obviate the problem that neurologically intact animals tend to show large degrees of individual variability in saccharin intake during the first few preshift trials in the test apparatus. As in previous studies (e.g., Sastre & Reilly, 2006), this treatment had the desired effect on the performance of the SHAM rats. Absent this pretraining access to the preshift solutions it would have been difficult, if not impossible, to interpret with any degree of confidence any differences in the performance of the SHAM and ICX rats over the initial preshift trials. The experimental design thus ensured that any lesion-induced deficit in the neophobic reactions to the test solutions would likely not be detected during the preshift trials. It might also be noted that the brief duration of the SNC trials (5 min) relative to the longer duration of trials (15 min) in our neophobia experiments (Lin et al., 2009) would tend, particularly at the outset of training, to minimize the likelihood of finding group differences in saccharin intake. However, once the animals became familiar with the apparatus and the task demands during the preshift trials, these constraints would not mask the occurrence of neophobia during the initial postshift trials. As argued above, although IC lesions, like GT lesions, might disrupt the reward comparison mechanism that governs expression of SNC in normal rats, we propose that the most parsimonious explanation of the present finding is a lesion-induced impairment in the inability to accurately recognize the novelty of the postshift saccharin stimulus. If this analysis is correct, then neophobia has a more significant role in the expression of SNC than previous suspected. This work was supported by grants DC04341 and DC06456 from the National Institute of Deafness and Other Communication Disorders. Portions of the data in this article were presented at the 80th Annual Meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago, IL, May, 2008. American Psychological Association. Guidelines for ethical conduct in the care and use of animals. Author; Washington, DC: 1996. Cechetto DF, Saper CB. Evidence for a viscerotopic sensory representations in the cortex and thalamus in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 1987;262:27 45. [PubMed: 2442207] Flaherty, CF. Incentive relativity. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 1996. Flaherty CF, Checke S. Anticipation of incentive gain. Animal Learning and Behavior 1982;10:177 182.

Lin et al. Page 7 Flaherty CF, Hamilton LW. Responsivity to decreasing sucrose concentrations following septal lesions in the rat. Physiology and Behavior 1971;6:431 437. [PubMed: 5148754] Kiefer SW, Braun JJ. Absence of differential associative responses to novel and familiar taste stimuli in rats lacking gustatory neocortex. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1977;91:498 507. [PubMed: 874118] Kosar E, Grill HJ, Norgren R. Gustatory cortex in the rat. I. Physiological properties and cytoarchitecture. Brain Research 1986;379:329 341. [PubMed: 3742225] Lin J-Y, Roman C, Andre J, Reilly S. Taste, olfactory and trigeminal neophobia in rats with forebrain lesions. Brain Research 2009;1251:195 203. [PubMed: 19059225] Meinrath AB, Flaherty CF. Effect of varied taste experience on negative contrast in consummatory behavior. American Journal of Psychology 1988;101:87 96. [PubMed: 3364616] Paxinos, G.; Watson, C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. Vol. 5th ed.. Academic Press; San Diego, CA: 2005. Reilly, S. Central gustatory system lesions and conditioned taste aversion. In: Reilly, S.; Schachtman, TR., editors. Conditioned taste aversion: Behavioral and neural processes. Oxford University Press; New York: 2009. p. 309-327. Reilly S, Bornovalova M, Trifunovic R. Excitotoxic lesions of the gustatory thalamus spare simultaneous contrast effects but eliminate anticipatory negative contrast: Evidence against a memory deficit. Behavioral Neuroscience 2004;118:365 376. [PubMed: 15113262] Reilly S, Pritchard TC. Gustatory thalamus lesions in the rat: II. Aversive and appetitive taste conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience 1996;110:746 759. [PubMed: 8864266] Reilly S, Trifunovic R. Gustatory thalamus lesions eliminate successive negative contrast in the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience 1999;113:1242 1248. [PubMed: 10636302] Reilly S, Trifunovic R. Gustatory thalamus lesions disrupt successive negative contrast: Evidence against a memory deficit. Behavioral Neuroscience 2003;117:606 615. [PubMed: 12802888] Roman C, Lin J-Y, Reilly S. Conditioned taste aversion and latent inhibition following extensive taste preexposure in rats with insular cortex lesions. Brain Research 2009;1259:68 73. [PubMed: 19150440] Roman C, Nebieridze N, Sastre A, Reilly S. Effects of lesions of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, lateral hypothalamus, or insular cortex on conditioned taste aversion and conditioned odor aversion. Behavioral Neuroscience 2006;120:1257 1267. [PubMed: 17201470] Roman C, Reilly S. Effects of insular cortex lesions on conditioned taste aversion and latent inhibition. European Journal of Neuroscience 2007;26:2627 2632. [PubMed: 17970726] Sastre A, Reilly S. Excitotoxic lesions of the gustatory thalamus eliminate consummatory but not instrumental successive negative contrast in rats. Behavioral Brain Research 2006;170:34 40. Schroy PL, Wheeler RA, Davidson C, Scalera G, Twinning RC, Grigson PS. The role of the gustatory thalamus in the anticipation and comparison of rewards over time in rats. American Journal of Physiology 2005;288:R966 R980. [PubMed: 15591157] Vogel JR, Mikulka PJ, Spear NE. Effects of shifts in sucrose and saccharine concentrations on licking behavior in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968;66:661 66. [PubMed: 5721493]

Lin et al. Page 8 Figure 1. (A) Schematic reconstructions of the smallest (black) and largest (gray) neurotoxic lesions of the insular cortex (IC; adapted with permission from the Paxinos & Watson, 2005). The numbers (0.00 mm, +0.60 mm, +1.20 mm, +1.80 mm, +2.28 mm) beneath each diagram refer to the anteroposterior coordinates relative to bregma. (B) Digitized photomicrograph of the IC (left hemisphere) taken from a neurological intact subject. (C) Representative neurotoxic lesion of the IC of an experimental rat. The dashed line outlines the extent of the cell loss. CPu, caudate putamen; rf: rhinal fissure; S2, secondary somatosensory cortex.

Lin et al. Page 9 Figure 2. Mean number of licks (± SE) during the 20, 5-min trials for SHAM subjects (Panel A) and the ICX rats (Panel B). In the Unshifted condition, 0.15% saccharin was presented through the experiment. On the other hand, rats in the Shifted condition were given 1.0 M sucrose during the first 14 trials and 0.15% saccharin in the following 6 trials.