A MILESTONE IN GENETICS: Mendel s 1866 Paper

Similar documents
8.1 Genes Are Particulate and Are Inherited According to Mendel s Laws 8.2 Alleles and Genes Interact to Produce Phenotypes 8.3 Genes Are Carried on

Genetics: field of biology that studies heredity, or the passing of traits from parents to offspring Trait: an inherited characteristic, such as eye

Genetics and heredity. For a long time, general ideas of inheritance were known + =

Mendel explained how a dominant allele can mask the presence of a recessive allele.

Chapter 10 Notes Patterns of Inheritance, Part 1

The Experiments of Gregor Mendel

biology Slide 1 of 32

Writing the Rules of Heredity. 23. Genetics I

Mendelian Genetics: Patterns of Inheritance

Mendel and Heredity. Chapter 12

Genetics. F 1 results. Shape of the seed round/wrinkled all round 5474 round, 1850 wrinkled 2.96 : 1

Objectives. ! Describe the contributions of Gregor Mendel to the science of genetics. ! Explain the Law of Segregation.

Mendel and Heredity. Chapter 12

biology Slide 1 of 32 End Show Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall

OCTOBER 21 Unit 5 Heredity 1. What is Heredity

HEREDITY. Heredity is the transmission of particular characteristics from parent to offspring.

MENDELIAN GENETICS. MENDEL RULE AND LAWS Please read and make sure you understand the following instructions and knowledge before you go on.

Fundamentals of Genetics

Name Hour. Section 11-1 The Work of Gregor Mendel (pages )

Genetics & The Work of Mendel. AP Biology

Mendelian Genetics. Ch. 2

Agro/ANSC/Biol/Gene/Hort 305 Fall, 2017 MENDELIAN INHERITANCE Chapter 2, Genetics by Brooker (Lecture outline) #2

Section 11 1 The Work of Gregor Mendel (pages )

Part 2: Heredity and Mendelian Genetics

VOCABULARY. TRAITS a genetic (inherited) characteristic. HEREDITY The passing of traits from parent to offspring

Mendelism: the Basic principles of Inheritance

Chapter 11. Introduction to Genetics

Lesson Overview. The Work of Gregor Mendel. Lesson Overview The Work of Gregor Mendel

What we mean more precisely is that this gene controls the difference in seed form between the round and wrinkled strains that Mendel worked with

Model of an F 1 and F 2 generation

Gregor Mendel and Genetics Worksheets

Chapter 13: Patterns of Inheritance

INTRODUCTION TO MENDELIAN GENETICS

Gregor Mendel Father of Genetics

Introduction to Genetics

Chapter 11 introduction to genetics 11.1 The work of Gregor mendel

Notes: Mendelian Genetics

Patterns of Inheritance

Section 1 MENDEL S LEGACY

Lesson Overview 11.2 Applying Mendel s Principles

The Work of Gregor Mendel. Lesson Overview. Lesson Overview The Work of Gregor Mendel

Traits and Probability

Genetics & The Work of Mendel

Ch 10 Genetics Mendelian and Post-Medelian Teacher Version.notebook. October 20, * Trait- a character/gene. self-pollination or crosspollination

Biology. Chapter 13. Observing Patterns in Inherited Traits. Concepts and Applications 9e Starr Evers Starr. Cengage Learning 2015

Chapter 7: Genetics Lesson 7.2: Gregor Mendel and Genetics

B.A. / B.Sc. (Honours) 5 th Semester (THEORY) (2/13)

The Law of Segregation Introduction Today, we know that many of people's characteristics, from hair color to height to risk of diabetes, are

MENDELIAN GENETICS. Punnet Squares and Pea Plants

Genetics PPT Part 1 Biology-Mrs. Flannery

Lab 5: Testing Hypotheses about Patterns of Inheritance

Chapter 02 Mendelian Inheritance

VOCABULARY somatic cell autosome fertilization gamete sex chromosome diploid homologous chromosome sexual reproduction meiosis

Mendelian Genetics. KEY CONCEPT Mendel s research showed that traits are inherited as discrete units.

READING ASSIGNMENT GENETIC ANALYSIS OF DROSOPHILA POPULATIONS I. HOW DO MITOSIS AND MEIOSIS COMPARE?

He called these new plants hybrids because they received different genetic information, or different alleles, for a trait from each parent.

Introduction to Genetics and Heredity

Lesson Overview 11.1 The Work of Gregor Mendel

Mendel s Law of Heredity. Page 254

Writing the Rules of Heredity

Mendelian Genetics. You are who you are due to the interaction of HEREDITY and ENVIRONMENT. ENVIRONMENT: all outside forces that act on an organism.

Genetics & The Work of Mendel

The Work of Gregor Mendel. Guided Reading

When Mendel crossed 2 plants that were different in a single trait, he called that a monohybrid cross. The resulting offspring were called the F1

Laws of Inheritance. Bởi: OpenStaxCollege

Chapter 6 Heredity The Big Idea Heredity is the passing of the instructions for traits from one generation to the next.

Fundamentals of Genetics

Gregor Mendel. Father of Genetics

draw and interpret pedigree charts from data on human single allele and multiple allele inheritance patterns; e.g., hemophilia, blood types

6.1 Mendel s Investigations

Mendelian Genetics Chapter 11

Sexual Reproduction and Genetics. Section 1. Meiosis

Extra Review Practice Biology Test Genetics

Genes and Inheritance

Labrador Coat Color Similar to coat color in mice: Black lab is BxEx Yellow lab is xxee Chocolate lab is bbex Probable pathway:

Guided Reading and Study. Definition a. The scientific study of heredity. b. Physical characteristics

2. Was there a scientific way to predict the outcome of a cross between two parents?

Genetics. Why do offspring resemble their parents? What role can technology play in genetics? Let s explore the answers to these questions.

You are who you are because of a combination of HEREDITY and ENVIRONMENT. ENVIRONMENT: all outside forces that act on an organism.

He was a Chezch priest and math teacher.

For a long time, people have observed that offspring look like their parents.

5.5 Genes and patterns of inheritance

Unit 6.2: Mendelian Inheritance

Genetics and Heredity Notes

Mendel rigorously followed various traits in the pea plants he bred. He analyzed

Topics. Introduction to Genetics and heredity Genetic terminology (glossary) Gregor Mendel a brief bio Monohybrid crosses

BIO 202 : GENETICS AND EVOLUTION

IB BIO I Genetics Test Madden

Genetics Test- Mendel, Probablility and Heredity

Biology 12. Mendelian Genetics

Mendelian Genetics. Gregor Mendel. Father of modern genetics

PROBABILITY and MENDELIAN GENETICS

Mendelian Genetics and Beyond Chapter 4 Study Prompts

Patterns of Inheritance. { Unit 3

Name Period. Keystone Vocabulary: genetics fertilization trait hybrid gene allele Principle of dominance segregation gamete probability

DNA Review??? gene???

2 Traits and Inheritance

UNIT III (Notes) : Genetics : Mendelian. (MHR Biology p ) Traits are distinguishing characteristics that make a unique individual.

11-1: Introduction to Genetics

Transcription:

A MILESTONE IN GENETICS: Mendel s 1866 Paper The paper that launched the science of genetics had the title Versuche über Pflanzenhybriden which translates from the German as Experiments with Plant-Hybrids. This paper detailed Gregor Mendel s studies on inheritance in peas. It was published in 1866 in the proceedings of the Natural History Society of Brünn. 1 Early in the previous year, Mendel had presented the results of his studies in lectures at two of the Society s meetings. Not many scientific papers have had the impact that Mendel s has had. Initially, the paper was ignored. However, when its significance was recognized, it became required reading for anyone interested in the study of heredity. Scrutiny by generations of readers has raised many questions about Mendel s paper. Did Mendel report his results literally? Is the fit between the data and the predictions of his hypotheses too good? Did he alter or fabricate the data to present the most compelling case for his hypotheses? In 1936 Ronald A. Fisher, a British statistician and geneticist, presented an analysis of Mendel s paper in the Annals of Science, a journal devoted to the history of science. 2 Fisher carefully attempted to reconstruct what Mendel actually did and when he did it. Mendel s research seems to have begun with the cultivation of stocks of peas in 1857. The first hybridizations between different varieties were apparently performed in 1858; other hybridizations were performed in 1859. Mendel followed the progeny of these crosses for as many as six generations. Fisher conjectures that Mendel began his dihybrid and trihybrid crosses in 1861. In that year, he also apparently began testcrosses to determine gametic ratios from heterozygous plants. Altogether, Mendel s experiments with peas spanned eight years from 1857 to 1864. The magnitude of these experiments is impressive. In some years Mendel grew more than 5000 pea plants in the monastery garden.

Fisher considers the question of whether or not Mendel s account of the experiments with peas can be taken literally: Mendel s paper is, as has been frequently noted, a model in respect of the order and lucidity with which the successive relevant facts are presented, and such orderly presentation would be much facilitated had the author felt himself at liberty to ignore the particular crosses and years to which the plants contributing to any special result might belong. Mendel was an experienced and successful teacher, and might well have adopted a style of presentation suitable for the lecture-room without feeling under any obligation to complicate his story by unessential details. The style of didactic presentation, with its conventional simplifications, represents, as is well known, a tradition far more ancient among scientific writers than the more literal narratives in which experiments are now habitually presented. 3 After examining the evidence in the paper, Fisher concludes that Mendel s account of his experiments should be taken literally: His experiments were carried out in just the way and much in the order that they are recounted. 4 Fisher also considers the question of whether or not Mendel s data agree too well with the predictions of his hypotheses. For example, using the data Mendel obtained in 1863, Fisher calculates a 2 statistic to test for the goodness of fit between the observations and the expectations. The result, 15.54, is less than half the expected value of the 2 distribution with 41 degrees of freedom, and Fisher says that so low a value could scarcely occur by chance once in 2000 trials. 5 Thus, he concludes, There can be no doubt that the data from the later years of the experiment have been biased strongly in the direction of agreement with expectation. 6 The bias in favor of the expected results is most apparent in the experiments Mendel performed to determine if F 2 plants with a dominant phenotype were homozygous or

heterozygous for the dominant allele. His procedure was to allow the plants to self-fertilize; then he examined 10 of the offspring. If any of the 10 showed the recessive phenotype, the parent was classified as a heterozygote. If none of the 10 showed this phenotype, it was classified as a homozygote. Fisher notes that with this procedure, some heterozygotes will incorrectly be classified as homozygotes simply by chance. The probability of this error is (0.75) 10 = 0.0563. Thus, the expected ratio of segregating to nonsegregating parents is not 2:1 but 2 2 0.0563: 1 + 2 0.0563, or 1.88:1.11. Among the 600 plants that Mendel tested, 399 were classified as heterozygotes and 201 were classified as homozygotes. These numbers are very close to the expectations based on a 2:1 ratio, but not so close to the expectations based on the ratio corrected for the probability of misclassifying heterozygotes as homozygotes. With this ratio, the expected numbers are 377.5 heterozygotes and 222.5 homozygotes. Fisher notes that the deviation of Mendel s data from these predicted numbers is to be expected once in twenty-nine trials. 7 For this discrepancy, Fisher suggests that Mendel was deceived by some assistant who knew too well what was expected. This possibility is supported by independent evidence that the data of most, if not all, of the experiments have been falsified so as to agree closely with Mendel s expectations. 8 In 2004 the respected geneticist Edward Novitski published an article about Fisher s criticism of Mendel s results. 9 Novitski concedes that overall, these results conform more closely with the ratios... theoretically expected than one might reasonably expect to obtain on a chance basis, and that Particularly troubling are those two groups of experiments in which Mendel s results are in close agreement with ratios that Mendel may have considered appropriate, but which were, according to Fisher (1936), incorrect. 10 However, Novitski argues that Fisher s criticism of Mendel especially his suggestion that some of the experimental data

might have been falsified is unfounded. For one thing, Mendel s procedure of ascertaining whether an F 2 plant with a dominant phenotype was homozygous or heterozygous for the dominant allele may have been affected by an error in the opposite direction of the one that Fisher described. Mendel based his decision about the genotype of a plant on the phenotypes of 10 of its progeny. Novitski proposes that for some of the plants, Mendel could not obtain the required 10 progeny. The failure rate for seed germination may have been as high as 2 percent. If fewer than 10 progeny were examined, and at least one of them had the recessive phenotype, then the parent plant could have been correctly classified as a heterozygote. However, if fewer than 10 progeny were examined say only 8 or 9 and none of them showed the recessive phenotype, what was Mendel to do? Novitski conjectures that Mendel would not have classified the parent plant as a homozygote for the dominant allele; rather, he would have excluded that plant from consideration and replaced it with another plant that had been held in reserve. The excluded plant was most likely a dominant homozygote, and the reserve plant was most likely a heterozygote. Thus, in an effort to adhere to his rule of classifying a plant as a dominant homozygote only after counting 10 dominant progeny, Mendel may have skewed his results in favor of heterozygous plants. This error would counterbalance the one that Fisher described and move the observed ratio of heterozygotes to dominant homozygotes toward 2:1, which is what Mendel reported. Novitski also suggests that Mendel may have repeated some of the experimental runs in which the results he obtained did not appear to bear out his expectations, not with any intent to deceive, but to make certain for his own benefit that those runs were in fact not bona fide cases of exceptions to his rules. Having obtained additional data, he could have either replaced the earlier deviating data with the better numbers or combined the two sets of data, which would

usually obscure the extent of the deviations in the first set.... It might be observed that such procedures probably exist in preparing data for publication even today. 11 As for the possibility that Mendel altered or manipulated his data, Novitski says that if he did, his alterations would stem not from any desire to mislead, but as a concession to his illprepared audience. We can imagine that at the time of writing for oral presentation, Mendel changed, for didactic purposes, some specific results that might have distracted his audience from the main theme of the article because of their seemingly aberrant nature. Surely words like fraud or dishonesty should be used with caution. Perhaps his situation can be compared to that of the competent high school science teacher who, in explaining the structure of the atom to his students, falls back on the simple Bohr model, well aware that while it is not the correct picture, it is appropriate for the audience for which it is intended. 12 QUESTIONS FOR DISCUSSION 1. Today many scientific papers contain four main sections of text Introduction, Materials and Methods, Results, and Discussion and they are usually preceded by an abstract (or summary) and followed by a bibliography of references cited. How does this format vary among journals that publish papers on genetics (e.g., Cell, Genetics, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Nature, and Science)? How does it differ from the format of papers in other disciplines such as sociology, law, history, and literature? 2. Gregor Mendel seems to have done his research to satisfy his own curiosity. He did not benefit from the research financially or professionally, and, except for space in the monastery garden and perhaps the help of a few assistants, his work did not require any other type of

support, say, for example, a government grant. What do these circumstances say about the kind of person Mendel was? How does he compare with professional scientists working on research projects today? 1 Mendel, G. 1866. Versuche über Pflanzenhybriden. Verhandlungen Naturforshender Vereines in Brünn 10. 2 Fisher, R. A. 1936. Has Mendel s work been rediscovered? Annals of Science 1:115 137. 3 Ibid. 4 Ibid. 5 Ibid. 6 Ibid. 7 Ibid. 8 Ibid. 9 Novitski, E. 2004. On Fisher s criticism of Mendel s results with the garden pea. Genetics 166:1133 1136. 10 Ibid. 11 Ibid. 12 Ibid.