CHANGE BLINDNESS, ATTENTION, AND DRIVING PERFORMANCE

Similar documents
THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING

Assessing cognition in ELDERLY drivers

Iran. T. Allahyari, J. Environ. et Health. al., USEFUL Sci. Eng., FIELD 2007, OF Vol. VIEW 4, No. AND 2, RISK pp OF... processing system, i.e

Eye Movement Patterns and Driving Performance

Useful Use of View Test UFOV - Reference List

CAN WE PREDICT STEERING CONTROL PERFORMANCE FROM A 2D SHAPE DETECTION TASK?

A multimodal model for the prediction of driving behavior

The Danger of Incorrect Expectations In Driving: The Failure to Respond

Neuropsychological Predictors of Safety in Urban Left-Turn Scenarios

USEFUL FIELD OF VIEW IMPAIRMENTS IN DRIVERS WITH OBSTRUCTIVE SLEEP APNEA

The Road Map. Collisions and aging Function, skill and driving Licensing and assessment The future

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

Safe Mobility at Any Age Identifiers of High-Risk Drivers: An Occupational Therapy Perspective

The useful field of view test: Normative data for older adults

Changing Driver Behavior Through Unconscious Stereotype Activation

PREDICTING OLDER ADULTS ON-ROAD DRIVING PERFORMANCE

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Manufacturing 3 (2015 )

Positioning Eye Fixation and Vehicle Movement: Visual-motor Coordination Assessment in Naturalistic Driving

LONGITUDINAL ASSESSMENT OF OLDER DRIVERS IN A DMV SETTING

Driving a motor vehicle, although frequently an

Augmented reality cues and elderly driver hazard perception

A FIELD STUDY ASSESSING DRIVING PERFORMANCE, VISUAL ATTENTION, HEART RATE AND SUBJECTIVE RATINGS IN RESPONSE TO TWO TYPES OF COGNITIVE WORKLOAD

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

Cell-Phone Induced Driver Distraction David L. Strayer and Frank A. Drews

The Study of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder on the Driving Related Cognitive Abilities

OPTIC FLOW IN DRIVING SIMULATORS

THE EFFECT OF VOICE INTERACTIONS ON DRIVERS GUIDANCE OF ATTENTION

Hazard Perception and Distraction in Novice Drivers: Effects of 12 Months Driving Experience

THE EFFECTS OF MOMENTARY VISUAL DISRUPTION ON HAZARD ANTICIPATION IN DRIVING. Massachusetts, USA

Driving Simulation: How Low Can You Go?

Traffic Sign Detection and Identification

Predicting Older Drivers' Difficulties Using the Roadwise Review

A Simulator-Based Street-Crossing Training for Older Pedestrians: Short and Long Term Effects

Table 5.11 Stroke Rehabilitation Assessment Tools For Pre-Driving Screening Canadian Best Practice Recommendations for Stroke Care Update

Verbal Collision Avoidance Messages of Varying Perceived Urgency Reduce Crashes in High Risk Scenarios

Which are the critical parameters assessing the driving performance of drivers with cerebral diseases? A literature review

Estimation of Driver Inattention to Forward Objects Using Facial Direction with Application to Forward Collision Avoidance Systems

Koji Sakai. Kyoto Koka Women s University, Ukyo-ku Kyoto, Japan

The role of memory on patients with mild cognitive impairment. Sophia Vardaki, PhD National Technical University of Athens

Aging and the Detection of Collision Events in Fog

Older Driver at Risk: What do I do?

7/2/ Cell Phone Induced Failures of Visual Awareness. During Simulated Driving. David L. Strayer, Frank A. Drews, and William A.

The Pathway from Driver to Passenger: Strategies &Interventions for Enhancing Community Mobility

TIME-TO-CONTACT AND COLLISION DETECTION ESTIMATIONS AS MEASURES OF DRIVING SAFETY IN OLD AND DEMENTIA DRIVERS

Evaluating the Safety of Verbal Interface Use while Driving

Situation Awareness in Driving

ASSESSING DRIVING PERFORMANCE WITH MODERATE VISUAL FIELD LOSS

On the Fast Lane to Road Rage

IAT 814 Knowledge Visualization. Visual Attention. Lyn Bartram

8/26/2013 DMV UCSD CHP. Presenters. Law Enforcement s Identification and Referral of Medically Impaired Older Drivers. NHTSA Priority.

Test-Retest Reliability of Simulated Driving Performance: A Pilot Study

Screening Drivers for Cognitive Impairment

Analysis of Glance Movements in Critical Intersection Scenarios

Driver Fitness in Patients with Cognitive Impairment and Glaucoma

Cognitive Impairment and Driving:

Characterizing Visual Attention during Driving and Non-driving Hazard Perception Tasks in a Simulated Environment

Development of a Driving Attitude Scale

Objectives. The Problem. The Problem. The Problem. Assessing the Older Driver

MEDICAL FITNESS TO DRIVE EVALUATIONS

Cooperative Intersection Collision Avoidance System Stop Sign Assist: Experiments to Validate Use of an In-Vehicle Interface Design

Cognitive and Psychomotor Correlates of Self- Reported Driving Skills and Behavior

Exploring the Needs of Aging Drivers in the Transportation System

Visual Selection and Attention

Driving at Night. It's More Dangerous

Driving to Distraction

The Effects of Age and Distraction on Reaction Time in a Driving Simulator

Application of ecological interface design to driver support systems

APPENDIX FOR "DRIVING SIMULATOR FOR PERSONS WITH IMPAIRED COGNITION", PI WILLIAM K DURFEE

Protecting Workers with Smart E-Vests

COGNITIVE AND PSYCHOMOTOR CORRELATES OF HAZARD PERCEPTION ABILITY AND RISKY DRIVING

Training Working Memory of Older Drivers: The Effect on Working Memory and Simulated Driving Performance

Driver Distraction. Bryan Reimer. Auto UI Tutorials October 17, 2012

Analysis of License Renewal Course Data

Self-Rated Distress Related to Medical Conditions is Associated with Future Crashes or Traffic Offences in Older Drivers

Does Workload Modulate the Effects of In-Vehicle Display Location on Concurrent Driving and Side Task Performance?

briefing notes - road safety issues Auckland Motorways

Chapter 5 Car driving

(In)Attention and Visual Awareness IAT814

CANTAB Test descriptions by function

Attention. What is attention? Attention metaphors. Definitions of attention. Chapter 6. Attention as a mental process

Partial Representations of Scenes in Change Blindness: In the Eyes and in the Hands

Naturalistic Driving Performance During Secondary Tasks

Cell Phones and Driving 1. Why Do Cell Phone Conversations Interfere With Driving?

TRAINING TO IMRPOVE COLLISION DETECTION IN OLDER ADULTS

Evaluating the Driving Ability in Patients with Parkinson s Disease Using a Driving Simulator

Visual Attention Problems as a Predictor of Vehicle Crashes in Older Drivers

PREVENTING DISTRACTED DRIVING. Maintaining Focus Behind the Wheel of a School Bus

Crash Risk Analysis of Distracted Driving Behavior: Influence of Secondary Task Engagement and Driver Characteristics

Useful Field of View Impairment in Partial Epilepsy

Modeling the Real World using STISIM Drive Simulation Software: A Study Contrasting High and Low Locality Simulations.

Cognitive-Motor Interference in Persons with Parkinson Disease

I just didn t see it It s all about hazard perception. Dr. Robert B. Isler Associate Professor School of Psychology University of Waikato Hamilton

Case description - a man lying on a road who has been run over

Do Drowsy Driver Drugs Differ?

Overestimation of Skills Affects Drivers Adaptation to Task Demands

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Multimodal Driver Displays: Potential and Limitations. Ioannis Politis

Lori Grismore, OTR/L, Occupational Therapist USF Health Byrd Alzheimer s Center and Research Institute

Potential Benefits of a Concurrent Verbal Task when Feeling Fatigued Due to Monotonous Driving Conditions

Transcription:

PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and icle Design CHANGE BLINDNESS, ATTENTION, AND DRIVING PERFORMANCE Monica N. Lees, JonDavid Sparks, John D. Lee, Matthew Rizzo University of Iowa University of Iowa Carver College of Medicine Iowa City, Iowa, USA E-mail: mnlees@engineering.uiowa.edu Summary: Concern over older driver high traffic fatality rates has resulted in an effort to identify risk factors and to develop methods of assessment. This study examines two attention-related tasks, Useful Field of View (UFOV) and Change Blindness (CB), in relation to vision and cognitive test batteries and driving performance measures collected using a simulator and an instrumented vehicle. Eight older adults with Alzheimer s disease and nine comparison subjects between 64 and 81 participated. Factor analysis results indicate that UFOV and CB relate to different factors. While UFOV relates to memory, decision-making, attention, and visual spatial ability, CB relates to vision and attention. The type of images used on a CB task influence how the task relates to driving performance measures. Researchers should be thoughtful when selecting images to include in CB tasks to maximize insight into real-world driving. INTRODUCTION Older drivers are overrepresented in fatal traffic accidents on a per-mile basis (Bedard, Guyatt, Stones, & Hirdes, 2002). Yet, the majority of older drivers remain crash-free. Because it would be unfair to restrict driving privileges based solely on age, there is a growing effort to understand the factors that increase crash risk and to identify metrics for identifying unsafe drivers. Failure to detect relevant changes may impede driver reactions in dynamic visual environments. Change blindness (CB) is an inability to detect large changes in a visual scene, often in association with a visual disruption, such as an eye-movement, blink, or film cut (McCarley et al., 2004; O'Regan, Rensink, & Clark, 1999; Pringle, Irwin, Kramer, & Atchley, 2001; Rizzo et al., submitted). Recent findings suggest that noticing changes requires both attention and visual working memory, the ability to store and compare visual information before and after a change occurs (Pringle et al., 2001; Simons & Ambinder, 2005). Older adults show reduced speed and accuracy on CB tasks compared to younger adults (Caird, Edwards, Creaser, & Horrey, 2005; Pringle et al., 2001; Rensink, 2002). The degree of task difficulty depends on the eccentricity, meaningfulness, and salience of the changing object (Pringle et al., 2001). While CB studies have included traffic scenes, this study directly tests the hypothetical relationship between CB using traffic scenes and actual driving performance. The useful field of view (UFOV) is another attention-related construct, operationalized as the area from which visual information can be extracted during a single glance without moving the eyes or head (Sanders, 1970). UFOV scores are influenced by visual sensory function, visual processing speed, divided attention, and selective attention. Several studies have shown that the 32

PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and icle Design UFOV test correlates with visual/cognitive tests and can successfully predict driving performance, measured by state crash records, on-road driving tests, and driving simulator performance (Ball & Owsley, 1993; Goode et al., 1998; Myers, Ball, Kalina, Roth, & Goode, 2000; Roenker, Cissell, Ball, Wadley, & Edwards, 2003). This study examines the underlying features of these two tasks in relation to commonly used vision and cognitive test batteries, and driving performance measures assessed using a simulator and an instrumented vehicle. METHOD Participants Seventeen elderly individuals participated in the study. Eight participants with early AD (74-81 years, M = 77.5) were recruited from a registry in the Department of Neurology. Nine comparison subjects (64-73 years, M = 68.3) without AD were recruited from the Iowa City and Coralville area. Corrective lenses were allowed during testing. Vision and Cognitive Assessment All participants completed a test battery examining: (1) basic vision: contrast sensitivity, acuity (2) visual-construction: judgment of line orientation (JLO), complex figure test copy version (CFT-Copy), WAIS- III block design (BLOCKS) (3) memory: complex figure test recall version (CFT-Recall), Benton visual retention test (BVRT), Rey auditory verbal learning test (AVLT) (4) decision making (executive functioning, EF): trail-making test (TMT-B), controlled oral word association (COWA) and (5) Attention: UFOV, CB. UFOV. UFOV scores represent a composite of four subtests administered with the Visual Attention Analyzer (Model 3000, Vision Resources, Chicago, IL, Ball & Owsley, 1993; Edwards et al., 2005). Performance on each subtest is expressed as the display duration (ms) required for a subject to achieve 75% correct. Task 1, of visual processing speed (ms), measures how fast subjects can perform a two-alternative forced choice task presented in central fixation. Task 2 of divided attention, measures how fast the subject can concurrently identify a central and a peripheral target. Task 3, of selective attention, is the same as Task 2 but the peripheral target is surrounded by a set of distracters shapes. Subtest 4 resembles subtest 3 but presents a more difficult central task (same/different discrimination). Change Blindness Task. The CB task was administered on a personal computer with a 21-inch touch-screen color monitor positioned approximately two feet from the participant. Table 1 lists the 21 pairs of images used. Each pair consisted of an original and a modified image (i.e., a single object in the photograph was changed). For change trials the original image transitioned into the modified image over a two-second duration (Simons, Franconeri, & Reimer, 2000). Up to five complete changes could occur during the 10 s presentation time. For catch trials, a static image was presented for the entire 10 s. Participants were instructed that an object could appear, disappear, change location, change color or not change. Participants were asked to touch the screen where the perceived change occurred or to press the space bar if no change occurred. 33

PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and icle Design Table 1. Images for change and catch trials by image type Description Image Type Short Name Change Trial Catch Trial Intersection Lights 1 Green to Red Traffic lights change from green to red Traffic lights are red 2 Red to Green Traffic lights change from red to green Traffic lights are green Safety Sign 3 Const Construction sign disappears Construction sign 4 sign appears sign on left Information Sign 5 Town Green town sign changes Green town sign 6 Left Turn Left turn only sign appears Left turn only sign 7 Billboard Billboard sign disappears Billboard sign Sign on icle 8 Truck Sign on back of delivery truck disappears Sign on back of delivery truck 9 Bus Sign on bus disappears Sign on bus 10 License Plate License plate holder changes License plate holder icle 11 Intersection icle moves from stop light through the intersection icle in intersection 12 Blue vehicle Blue vehicle appears in far left lane Blue vehicle in far left lane 13 Silver icle vehicle changes from black to silver icle on left is silver 14 White icle White vehicle disappears White vehicle on right 15 Red icle Red vehicle in front disappears Red vehicle in front 16 Left-Turn icle icle making left turn disappears icle making left turn 17 icle Grass icle in grass disappears vehicle in grass Pedestrian 18 Ped Pedestrians move from street corner to middle of street Pedestrians on street corner 19 Ped / Sign Sign and pedestrian disappear Sign and pedestrian on right 20 Yellow Helmet Person with yellow helmet disappears Person with yellow helmet Other 21 Train Train changes position Train on left Driving Simulator Assessment. Data were collected at 30 Hz using a medium-fidelity, fixed-base DriveSafety Vection Research Simulator (VRS), which consists of a fully instrumented 1994 four-door GM Saturn equipped with a 150-degree forward and 50-degree rear field of view. Fully-textured graphics were generated at a 60-Hz frame rate and a resolution of 1024 x 768. Participants drove in a rural setting with a speed limit of 55 mph. During the drive, participants encountered a number of situations. In this paper, responses during two events are examined (1) a police car on the side of the road, and (2) an incursion vehicle that ran a stop sign. These events require drivers to detect and recognize unsafe situations, and respond in an appropriate manner. The dependent measures for the police car event included reaction time and whether the driver slowed down. The dependent measure for the incursion event was whether a crash resulted. Instrumented icle Assessment. Drivers also completed a range of tasks in an instrumented mid-size 1995 Ford Taurus station wagon. Driving data (e.g., speed, accelerator and brake pedal position) were digitized at 10 Hz and reduced to mean, SD, or count. On-road tasks in the IV included a route-following task (RFT), and a landmark and traffic sign identification task (LIT) (See Uc, Rizzo, & Anderson, 2005; Uc, Rizzo, Anderson, Shi, & Dawson, 2004). The RFT required drivers to navigate to a location based on a set of explicit verbal directions. The dependent measures of the task include (1) number of incorrect turns, (2) times lost, and (3) atfault safety errors (e.g., lane deviation, stopping/slowing in unsafe situations, unsafe intersection behavior). The LIT required drivers to look for and verbally report traffic signs and restaurants located along a one-mile four-lane roadway segment. There were 16 road signs and 13 restaurants. The dependent measures included (1) percentage of landmarks and traffic signs identified, and (2) number of at-fault safety errors. RESULTS A factor analysis examined associations between driving performance measures, UFOV, and CB. This analysis extracted sets of co-varying measures from performance on the vision and 34

PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and icle Design cognitive tests and driving tasks and identified three factors that accounted for 57% of variation across measures (Table 2). Attention and decision-making, 33% of the variation, represents variables related to attention, decision-making (EF), memory, and visual spatial ability. Considering that many of these functions are required to successfully navigate, it is not surprising that this factor also includes errors on the route-following task. Detection and evaluation, 13% of the variation, relates to vision and attentional abilities. This factor included at-fault errors during tasks in the IV and the ability to respond appropriately to the police car event in the simulator, all of which require the ability to detect and evaluate changes in the environment. Identification and intervention, 12% of the variation, represents variables related to identifying information and determining how to respond. These results suggest that UFOV may reflect multiple constructs, whereas CB may only have one underlying construct. Table 2. Factor analysis of cognitive/visual tests, and driving performance measures Attention UFOV 2 (.657) Decision Making (EF) Memory Visuoconstruction ability Vision Response Selection Identification Factor 1: Attention and Factor 2: Detection and Evaluation Factor 3: Identification and Intervention Decision-Making CB 2 - Response time (.756) CB 2 z-score (-.649) COWA 3 (-.449) TMT-B 3 (.804) RFT - wrong turns (.704) RFT - times lost (.716) BVRT 3 (.624) AVLT 3 (-.543) JLO (-.713) CFT-Recall 3 (-.566) CFT-Copy 3 (-.372) BLOCKS 3 (-.870) Visual Acuity (.520) Contrast Sensitivity (-.529) RFT 1 - at-fault errors (.826) Crash at intersection 1 (.604) LIT 1 - at-fault errors (.667) Police Car reaction time 1 (-.626) Police Car 1 - response made (.522) LIT task performance 2 - critical signs (.818) LIT task performance 2 - restaurants (.710) 1. Driving Task; 2.CB/UFOV; 3.Visual/cognitive task Although UFOV is correlated with overall CB detection time (R 2 =.578, p=.015) and hit-rate (R 2 = -.668, p<.01), these two tests exhibit a different pattern of association with driving performance measures. As shown in Table 3, UFOV correlated with on-road performance measures related to RFT and LIT. In contrast, overall CB detection time (performance across all 21 image pairs) only correlated with one driving measure, at-fault errors during the LIT. Neither CB nor UFOV correlated with simulator performance on the police car and incursion tasks. To further examine the association between driving and CB we examined the correlation between each of the 21 CB image pairs and simulator and on-road driving performance measures. Due to the large set of correlations, Table 3 and Table 4 only include image pairs where either CB accuracy or detection time correlated with one or more driving performance measures. The results indicate that the association between CB and driving performance depends on the specific CB image pair. For instance, accuracy on detection tasks with a changing vehicle rarely correlated with driving performance measures, yet the ability to correctly detect the 35

Table 3. Correlations of UFOV and time to detect change with driving performance for specific CB images Intersection Safety Sign Info Sign Sign on Pedestrian Other Overall Red to Green Const Sign Town Truck Bus Intersection Blue Silver White Red Left-Turn Grass Ped Ped / Yellow Helmet Train UFOV CB Change Catch Change Change Change Change Change Catch Change Catch Change Catch Change Catch Change Change Change Catch Catch Change Catch Catch Catch Change Catch r.566.340.113 -.170.250 -.142.340.504.454 -.226.101 -.085.311 -.085.399.113 0 -.142 -.198 -.295 -.231.122 -.125 -.070.017.304.085 # Times Lost p.018.182.665.515.349.588.182.047.089.382.731.745.224.746.125.665 1.588.446.267.407.654.644.805.949.291.746 r.351.313.394 -.148.338.011.035.386.392.016.245 -.023.509 -.102.086.176.073.056.071 -.089 -.051.206 -.100.123.167.042.187 # Wrong Turns p.167.221.117.570.200.967.893.140.149.950.398.930.037.697.752.500.789.832.787.744.856.444.712.663.537.887.474 r -.615 -.147 -.448 -.117 -.057 -.169.009 -.111 -.140 -.025.073 0 -.264 -.010 -.292 -.142.003.209 -.043.085.260 -.214.056.138 -.136.033 -.152 # Restaurants Identified p.009.573.071.656.833.516.974.684.620.926.805 1.306.970.273.589.991.421.870.753.350.427.837.625.616.911.560 r -.548 -.228 -.389 -.427 -.212 -.327 -.223 -.004 -.139 -.236.007 -.264 -.284 -.291 -.374 -.298.046.038 -.146 -.141 -.054 -.334 -.185 -.106 -.399.074 -.347 # Critical Signs Identified p.023.378.123.087.431.200.389.987.621.362.982.307.269.257.154.246.866.884.577.603.848.206.492.707.126.803.173 r -.150.025.126.101.112 -.151 -.101 -.102.140.151.314.076 -.101.174.015 -.075 -.196.578.402.424.229.307.168.283.307.392 0 Incursion Event Crash p.564.924.631.701.680.564.701.706.620.564.275.773.701.500.957.774.467.015.110.102.411.247.534.306.247.165 1 r.024.241.205 -.072.096.048 -.337 -.068.441.337.430.193.481.048 -.150 -.096.380.385.553 -.014.062.287 -.068.371 -.164.072.048 Police Car Slowed Down p.927.352.431.783.725.855.186.802.100.186.125.458.051.855.578.713.147.127.021.960.827.281.802.173.544.808.855 r.083.017 -.395.152.179 -.093.208.162 -.100 -.049 -.534.342.147.189 -.368.091.562 -.179 -.164 -.074.093 -.141 -.038 -.096 -.018 -.301.167 Police Car Reaction Time p i.948.117.560.506.722.422.550.723.852.049.180.573.468.165.729.024.492.529.787.742.602.888.733.948.296.523 r.511.601.393.571.489.424.507.141.229.510.225.534.546.571.499.633.400.372.447.566.551.653.312.524.719.265.694 # At Fault Errors during LIT p.036.011.118.017.055.090.038.603.412.037.440.027.024.017.049.006.124.142.072.022.033.006.239.045.002.361.002 r.233.414.490.399.285.411.343.032.195.588.411.385.591.315.285.583.376.329.512.464.439.415.246.428.457.061.496 # At Fault Errors during RFT p.369.098.046.112.284.102.178.908.487.013.145.127.013.217.286.014.152.197.036.071.102.110.358.111.075.837.043 AD - Mean 1119.0 7.6 7.9 8.7 8.7 8.2 7.2 6.7 7.4 8.5 4.8 8.8 8.5 9.0 8.4 8.9 6.4 6.7 7.2 8.6 8.2 6.2 7.9 8.6 9.1 8.1 9.2 AD - SD 284.3 1.1 2.1 2.9 2.0 2.7 2.3 3.3 2.8 2.1 1.9 2.0 1.9 1.6 2.1 1.9 2.9 2.4 2.9 2.1 2.9 2.2 2.6 1.4 1.9 2.1 2.2 AD - Range 899.0 3.3 4.6 8.3 4.9 6.7 6.2 7.9 7.4 5.5 5.0 5.6 4.9 3.9 4.7 5.2 7.7 6.5 7.4 5.0 7.0 6.5 5.9 3.8 5.1 5.7 6.3 Control - Mean 655.7 5.4 4.9 7.6 5.3 5.9 5.7 4.4 4.9 7.2 3.5 7.8 5.6 7.1 5.8 7.4 5.4 5.0 5.6 7.6 7.0 3.3 7.3 7.1 7.0 5.7 7.0 Control - SD 138.8 1.3 2.7 2.7 2.4 2.0 2.4 2.6 2.1 2.2 1.7 2.3 2.5 2.2 1.6 2.1 2.7 3.0 2.3 2.3 2.1 1.2 2.4 2.2 1.9 2.5 2.9 Control - Range 431.0 2.5 9.7 6.8 6.8 6.1 5.9 7.7 6.1 6.5 4.1 7.4 7.1 6.0 5.0 5.4 7.3 8.2 7.0 7.5 6.7 3.2 6.4 6.8 6.1 7.2 6.6 Table 4. Correlations of accuracy with driving performance for specific CB images Overall CB Intersection Safety Sign Info Sign Sign on icle Pedestrian Other Hitrate - Correct responses on Red to Green Const Sign Town Truck Bus Intersection Blue Silver White Red Left-Turn Grass Ped Ped / Yellow Helmet Train change trials Change Catch Change Change Change Change Change Catch Change Catch Change Catch Change Catch Change Change Change Catch Catch Change Catch Catch Catch Change Catch r -.368.171.182 -.389 -.410 -.308 -.545 -.419 -.033 -.194 -.099 -.182 -.033 -.222 -.381 -.218 -.311.245 -.424 -.272.289 -.289 -.123 -.222 -.304 -.245 # Times Lost p.146.512.484.136.103.230.029.120.901.507.704.484.901.409.131.417.225.343.102.326.277.278.662.409.290.343 r -.302 -.042.149 -.193 -.461 -.031 -.537 -.607 -.067.112 -.176 -.352 -.067.015 -.271 -.107 -.067.053 -.295 -.419.048 -.328 -.344 -.230 -.125 -.454 # Wrong Turns p.238.873.568.473.063.905.032.016.799.704.499.165.799.955.293.692.799.839.267.120.860.214.209.391.671.067 r.229 -.378.000.293.579.156.247.481 -.084.098.147.134.205.164.134 -.274.169.024.110.032.112.231.000.151.074.048 # Restaurants Identified p.376.135 ####.271.015.550.357.070.748.738.574.607.430.543.607.305.518.927.686.911.679.389 1.000.578.801.854 r.250 -.316.283.428.494.043.069.266.109.020.344.061.376.165.111 -.276.097.109.303.191.127.369.079.221.300.133 # Critical Signs Identified p.333.216.272.099.044.871.800.338.677.946.176.815.137.540.673.301.711.677.253.495.639.159.779.412.298.610 r -.088 -.227 -.118 -.035.288.119.221.040 -.203 -.400 -.118 -.132 -.203 -.051.132.163 -.203 -.203.051.000 -.313 0 -.111 -.051 -.189 -.044 Incursion Event Crash p.737.380.653.898.263.648.411.887.434.156.653.612.434.851.612.547.434.434.851 1.000.237 1.693.851.519.868 r -.048 -.290.169.323 -.290.245 -.270 -.764 -.417.091 -.070 -.169 -.181 -.016 -.070 -.126.056 -.181 -.238 -.218.163 -.126 -.218 -.270.344.181 Police Car Slowed Down p.854.259.517.223.259.343.312 <.001.096.756.788.517.488.954.788.642.832.488.375.435.547.642.435.312.228.488 r.249 -.101 -.268 -.102.050 -.113 -.014 0.000.431 -.122 -.024 -.048 -.150 -.073 -.451.337.289 -.232.063.644 -.027.220.068.721.192 Police Car Reaction Time p.335.701.298.706.848.665.960 1 1.000.124.641.926.855.578.780.080.186.261.387.824.007.921.430.802.004.459 r -.446.110 -.240 -.407 -.507 -.062 -.365 -.456 -.210.000 -.240 -.160 -.354 -.700 -.386.136 -.184 -.289 -.499 -.326.078 -.555 -.326 -.274 -.227 -.249 # At Fault Errors during LIT p.073.675.354.118.038.814.165.087.418 1.000.354.540.163.003.126.615.480.261.049.235.775.026 0.235 0.305.436.334 r -.219.013 -.050 -.164 -.603.101 -.294 -.429 -.184.321 -.150 -.262 -.184 -.378 -.386.365.025 -.381 -.392 -.305.172 -.415-0.305-0.112 -.189 -.209 # At Fault Errors during RFT p.398.961.849.543.010.699.270.110.479.264.567.310.479.149.126.165.925.132.134.269.524.110 0.269 0.680.518.421 AD - Mean.26.38.50.00.00.25.29.17.38.50.38.25.25.29.38.43.38.13.50.29.29.13.29.29.17.25 AD - SD.15.52.53.00.00.46.49.41.52.55.52.46.46.49.52.53.52.35.53.49.49.35.49.49.41.46 Control - Mean.53.33.67.56.67.22.78.78.67.88.78.56.78.78.78.44.67.56.67.88.44.88.88.78.50.67 Control - SD.13.50.50.53.50.44.44.44.50.35.44.53.44.44.44.53.50.53.50.35.53.35.35.44.53.50

PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and icle Design appearance of a stop sign correlated with multiple driving performance measures (e.g., LIT and RFT performance and at-fault errors). The range in accuracy and detection time performance across images indicates an absence of floor and/or ceiling effects (bottom of Table 3 and Table 4). Instead, the results suggest that certain situations depicted in CB trials correspond better with the types of errors drivers make. Comparison of the two tables suggests that detection time in the CB task corresponds better with driving performance than does CB accuracy. CONCLUSIONS Growing concern over the high traffic fatality rates of older drivers motivates effort to identify risk factors and to develop methods to assess at-risk drivers. The utility of driver assessment tests depends on how well they capture the demands of driving. In this study, we examined how UFOV and CB assessment tools correspond to driving performance of older drivers. Three factors (attention and decision-making, detection and evaluation, and identification and intervention) were identified using a factor analysis that incorporated visual/cognitive tests, CB, UFOV, and driving performance data. While UFOV and CB correlate with each other, they load on different factors, with UFOV relating to attention and decision-making (EF), and CB relating to detection and evaluation. These different underlying dimensions may account for the differing correlations of UFOV and CB with driving performance in the simulator and on the road. While UFOV appears to show a general relationship to driving, the association between CB and driving largely depends on the particular task images used. Given the wide range of images utilized in this experiment, eccentricity, meaningfulness, and salience are likely to have influenced the varying degree of correlation between CB and driving performance (Pringle et al., 2001). These variables are being assessed in ongoing studies of CB in follow-up research in our lab and others. The results suggest that specific situations/images used in a CB task are particularly relevant when trying to relate this task to driving performance measures. Given these results, CB tasks should be refined to reflect driving situations that map more directly onto driving performance measures. Researchers should consider common crash types of specific populations and individual characteristics when determining what types of images to utilize. For example, older and younger drivers may require different stimulus sets to capture driving relevant impairments. Once refined, we believe CB would differentiate different driver populations and be better equipped to predict poor driving performance. Understanding the relationship between attention and driving can help identify the specific needs of drivers to better design systems that support appropriate attention allocation for demanding situations. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This research was sponsored by NIA AG017177. REFERENCES Ball, K. K., & Owsley, C. (1993). The useful field of view test: A new technique for evaluating age-related declines in visual function. Journal of the American Optometric Association, 64, 71-79. 37

PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and icle Design Bedard, M., Guyatt, G. H., Stones, M. J., & Hirdes, J. P. (2002). The independent contribution of driver, crash, and vehicle characteristics to driver fatalities. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 34(6), 717-727. Caird, J. K., Edwards, C. J., Creaser, J. I., & Horrey, W. J. (2005). Older driver failures of attention at intersections: using change blindness methods to assess turn decision accuracy. Human Factors, 47(2), 235-249. Edwards, J. D., Vance, D. E., Wadley, V. G., Cissell, G. M., Roenker, D. L., & Ball, K. K. (2005). Reliability and validity of useful field of view test scores as administered by a personal computer. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 529-543. Goode, K. T., Ball, K. K., Sloane, M., Roenker, D. L., Roth, D. L., Myers, R. S., et al. (1998). Useful field of view and other neurocognitive indicators of crash risk in older adults. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 5(4), 425-438. McCarley, J. S., Vais, M. J., Pringle, H. L., Kramer, A. F., Irwin, D. E., & Strayer, D. L. (2004). Conversation disrupts change detection in complex traffic scenes. Human Factors, 46, 424-436. Myers, R. S., Ball, K. K., Kalina, T. D., Roth, D. L., & Goode, K. T. (2000). Relation of useful field of view and other screening tests to on-road driving performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 91, 279-290. O'Regan, J. K., Rensink, R. A., & Clark, J. J. (1999). Change-blindness as a result of "mudsplashes." Nature, 398, 34. Pringle, H. L., Irwin, D. E., Kramer, A. F., & Atchley, P. (2001). The role of attentional breadth in perceptual change detection. Psychological Bulletin and Review, 8, 89-95. Rensink, R. A. (2002). Change detection. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 245-277. Rizzo, M., Sparks, J. D., McEvoy, S., Batchelder, S., Kellison, I., & Vecera, S. (submitted). Change blindness in aging and early Alzheimer's disease. Roenker, D. L., Cissell, G. M., Ball, K. K., Wadley, V. G., & Edwards, J. D. (2003). Speed-ofprocessing and driving simulator training result in improved driving performance. Human Factors, 45(2), 218-233. Sanders, A. F. (1970). Some aspects of the selective process in the functional field of view. Ergonomics, 13, 101-117. Simons, D. J., & Ambinder, M. S. (2005). Change Blindness: Theory and Consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(1), 44-48. Simons, D. J., Franconeri, S. L., & Reimer, R. L. (2000). Change Blindness in the absence of a visual disruption. Perception, 29, 1143-1154. Uc, E. Y., Rizzo, M., & Anderson, S. W. (2005). Driver landmark and traffic sign identification in early Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 76, 764. Uc, E. Y., Rizzo, M., Anderson, S. W., Shi, Q., & Dawson, J. D. (2004). Driver route-following and safety errors in early Alzheimer disease. Neurology, 63(5), 832-837. 38