This is a publisher-deposited version published in: Eprints ID: 16195

Similar documents
Electrophysiological correlates of inattentional deafness: no hearing without listening

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure

Evaluation of Human Machine Interface Design in Air Traffic Control using behavioral. and EEG/ERP measures

SPATIAL AUDIO VS. VERBAL DIRECTIONAL CUES: AN EXAMINATION OF SALIENCE AND DISRUPTIVENESS WITHIN A SIMULATED DRIVING CONTEXT

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

Verbal-Spatial Cue Conflict: Implications for the Design of Collision-Avoidance Warning Systems

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude

Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change

Visual Selection and Attention

Profiling Attention s Pure Effect on the Sensory-Evoked P1 and N1 Event-Related Potentials of Human Electroencephalography

Running Head: VISUAL WORKING MEMORY LOAD AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL

Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention.

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

NeuroImage 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

Modeling Neural Correlates of Selective Attention

Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized. Readiness Potential. Dianna Monteith. Saint Thomas University

INVESTIGATION COGNITIVE AVEC LES EFRPS (EYE-FIXATION-RELATED POTENTIALS) Thierry Baccino CHART-LUTIN (EA 4004) Université de Paris VIII

Address for correspondence: School of Psychological Sciences, Zochonis Building, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, M139PL, Manchester, UK

Online Publication Date: 15 th July 2012 Publisher: Asian Economic and Social Society

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus)

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search

International Journal of Neurology Research

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

Jan Kaiser, Andrzej Beauvale and Jarostaw Bener. Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, 13 Golcbia St., ?

The Gaze Cueing Paradigm with Eye Tracking Background Set-up Lab

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Manufacturing 3 (2015 )

Fatigue and Readability when Reading with LED White Light

The EEG Analysis of Auditory Emotional Stimuli Perception in TBI Patients with Different SCG Score

Two-object attentional interference depends on attentional set

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set

Supporting Information

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention

Studying the time course of sensory substitution mechanisms (CSAIL, 2014)

ATTENTION! Learning Objective Topics. (Specifically Divided and Selective Attention) Chapter 4. Selective Attention

Research Review: Multiple Resource Theory. out in multi-task environments. Specifically, multiple display layout and control design

Visual Short-term Memory Capacity for Simple and Complex Objects

ERP Correlates of Identity Negative Priming

The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture

No Behavioral or ERP Evidence for a Developmental Lag in Visual Working Memory Capacity or Filtering in Adolescents and Adults with ADHD

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation

Support Vector Machine Classification and Psychophysiological Evaluation of Mental Workload and Engagement of Intuition- and Analysis-Inducing Tasks

Enhancement of Application Software for Examination of Differential Magnification Methods and Magnification Interface Factors

An Electrophysiological Study on Sex-Related Differences in Emotion Perception

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

EEG-Rhythm Dynamics during a 2-back Working Memory Task and Performance

The role of modality congruence in the presentation and recognition of taskirrelevant stimuli in dual task paradigms.

Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): ISSN X

An Electrophysiological Study of Attention Capture by Salience: Does Rarity Enable Capture?

Immediate and delayed stimulus repetitions evoke different ERPs in a serial-probe recognition task.

Asymmetry between the upper and lower visual fields: An event-related potential study

The influence of predictive value of cues in the endogenous orienting paradigm examined with event-related lateralizations

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies

DCNV RESEARCH TOOL: INVESTIGATION OF ANTICIPATORY BRAIN POTENTIALS

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory?

In what way does the parietal ERP old new effect index recollection?

Behavioural and electrophysiological measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention

Classifying P300 Responses to Vowel Stimuli for Auditory Brain-Computer Interface

Electroencephalography

Material-speci c neural correlates of memory retrieval

Prestimulus Alpha as a Precursor to Errors in a UAV Target Orientation Detection Task

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

International Journal of Psychophysiology

Enhanced Performance for Recognition of Irrelevant Target-Aligned Auditory Stimuli: Unimodal and Cross-modal Considerations

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory?

Title of Thesis. Studies on Mechanisms of Visual and Auditory Attention in Temporal or Spatial Cueing Paradigm

SENSATION AND PERCEPTION KEY TERMS

Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

Effects of dynamic rotation on event-related brain potentials

Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening

Title change detection system in the visu

Practicalities of EEG Measurement and Experiment Design YATING LIU

Rapid Context-based Identification of Target Sounds in an Auditory Scene

REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS

Dual Mechanisms for the Cross-Sensory Spread of Attention: How Much Do Learned Associations Matter?

A Brain Computer Interface System For Auto Piloting Wheelchair

Event-related potentials as an index of similarity between words and pictures

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Technology 18 (2014 ) 25 31

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making

The development of visual- and auditory processing in Rett syndrome: An ERP study

An investigation of the auditory streaming effect using event-related brain potentials

The Influence of Induced Anxiety on the P3 Event Related Brain Potentials of Athletes and Novices in a Go/NoGo Task

Working with EEG/ERP data. Sara Bögels Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics

Effects of task difficulty and time-on-task on mental workload

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY

The Influence of the Attention Set on Exogenous Orienting

Transcription:

Open Archive TOULOUSE Archive Ouverte (OATAO) OATAO is an open access repository that collects the work of Toulouse researchers and makes it freely available over the web where possible. This is a publisher-deposited version published in: http://oatao.univ-toulouse.fr/ Eprints ID: 16195 To link to this article: DOI: 10.1016/j.promfg.2015.07.594 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.promfg.2015.07.594 To cite this version: Causse, Mickael and Fabre, Eve F. and Giraudet, Louise and Gonzalez, Marine and Peysakhovich, Vsevolod EEG/ERP as a Measure of Mental Workload in a Simple Piloting Task. (2015) Procedia Manufacturing, vol. 3. pp. 5230-5236. ISSN 2351-9789 Any correspondence concerning this service should be sent to the repository administrator: staff-oatao@listes-diff.inp-toulouse.fr

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia Manufacturing 3 (2015 ) 5230 5236 6th International Conference on Applied Human Factors and Ergonomics (AHFE 2015) and the Affiliated Conferences, AHFE 2015 EEG/ERP as a measure of mental workload in a simple piloting task Mickaël Causse a,b, Eve Fabre a, Louise Giraudet a, Marine Gonzalez a, Vsevolod Peysakhovich a a Département Conception et Conduite des véhicules Aéronautiques et Spatiaux, Institut Supérieur de l aéronautique et de l espace b Ecole de Psychologie,Université Laval, Québec, Canada Abstract Operating an aircraft is cognitively challenging: pilots have to control the plane and must remain responsive to potential verbalauditory stimuli (e.g. Air Traffic Control Communication) and auditory alerts (e.g. Terrain Awareness and Warning System). Fifteen participants had to control an aircraft in order to target one of three differently-colored aircrafts displayed on a computer screen. The name of the color (written in black ink) corresponding to the aircraft to target was displayed in the center of the screen. Simultaneously with the onset of the written name of the color, a spoken color name distractor that participants had to ignore was played. This auditory distractor was either congruent (10%, spoken color name matched the written color) or incongruent (10%, spoken color name did not match the written color). The task difficulty varied in terms of working memory load with an n-back-like sub-task. In the low load condition, participants had to target the aircraft corresponding to the currently presented written instruction (n = 0). In the high load condition, participants had to target the aircraft corresponding to the instruction presented two trials before (n = 2). Behavioral analysis showed that increased mental workload provoked a decrease in piloting performance, i.e. participants tended to forget the correct instruction. On the physiological level, EEG/ERP measurements related to instructions showed that increased mental workload was accompanied by lower P3b amplitude. We assume that the lower P3b amplitude reflects the depletion of the cognitive resources allocated to the processing of the instructions. These results suggest that P3b can be a relevant indicator of the openness of the system to sudden and unexpected critical stimuli such as auditory alerts. 2015 2015 The The Authors. Authors. Published Published by by Elsevier Elsevier B.V. B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/). Peer-review under responsibility of AHFE Conference. Peer-review under responsibility of AHFE Conference Keywords: Aviation safety, Electroencephalography, Cognitive workload, Auditory alerts 2351-9789 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/). Peer-review under responsibility of AHFE Conference doi:10.1016/j.promfg.2015.07.594

Mickaël Causse et al. / Procedia Manufacturing 3 ( 2015 ) 5230 5236 5231 1. Introduction Operating an aircraft is cognitively challenging: pilots have to fly the aircraft and navigate while remaining responsive to potential verbal-auditory stimuli (e.g. Air Traffic Control Communication) and auditory alerts (e.g. Terrain Awareness and Warning System). Previous researches from cognitive psychology emphasized that individuals in attention-demanding settings can remain unaware of unexpected stimuli [1-4]. An important amount of studies demonstrates that attentional capacity may be shared by both visual and auditory modalities [5-7]. To this extent, when engaged in a visual task associated with a high perceptive charge, an individual may be less likely to process an auditory stimulus when presented at the same time. In the same way, several authors showed that an increased cognitive load can provoke a reduced openness of the attentional system to distractors [8-10]. Generally used in static laboratory tasks, the interference paradigm is an interesting way to measure this phenomenon. In this paradigm, a target is accompanied by a visual or an auditory distractor item. The participant is asked to react to the target as quickly as possible while attempting to ignore the distractor. Depending on the relation between the target and the distractor, the target processing may be slowed down or accelerated. Traditionally, the available attentional resources can be evaluated by measuring the level of interference caused by the distractor stimulus at a behavioral level (i.e. accuracy and response times). According to Lavie [11], high load on frontal cognitive control processes increases distractor processing. Available attentional resources can be also examined at an electrophysiological level (i.e., brain reaction to visual and auditory stimuli), according to [8-10], reduced openness in an interference paradigm is associated with a lower P300 amplitude, an event related potential (ERP) associated with both cognitive and attentional processes [12]. The present study aims at testing the impact of cognitive load and incongruence on the processing of relevant instruction during a dynamic piloting situation in combination with EEG/ERP measurements. Participants had to continuously control an aircraft in order to target one of three differently-colored aircrafts displayed on a computer screen. The black inked color name corresponding to the color of the aircraft to target was displayed in the center of the screen while a spoken color name distractor that participants had to ignore was played. The task difficulty varied in terms of working memory load with an n-back-like sub-task. In the low load condition, participants had to target the aircraft corresponding to the currently presented written instruction (n = 0). In the high load condition, participants had to target the aircraft corresponding to the instruction presented two trials before (n = 2). We examined the following questions: a) How do cognitive load and incongruence affect the processing of the target instruction and consequently the piloting performance? b) Is there any interaction effect between cognitive load and incongruence? c) How do these two factors influence brain activity observed through the amplitude of the P300 component. 2. Material and methods 2.1. Ethic statement All participants were informed of their rights and gave written informed consent for participation in the study, according to the Declaration of Helsinki. The research was carried out fulfilling ethical requirements in accordance with the standard procedures of the ISAE Supaero. 2.2. Participants Fifteen French male participants (M Age = 24.6, SD ± 1.86) participated in this study. All were right-handed as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971), had normal auditory acuity and normal or corrected-to-normal vision. None of the participants reported a history of prior neurological disorder. 2.3. Aviation task Participants had to continuously control an aircraft using a joystick in order to target one of three differentlycolored aircrafts displayed on a computer screen. The name of the color (written in black ink) corresponding to the

5232 Mickaël Causse et al. / Procedia Manufacturing 3 ( 2015 ) 5230 5236 aircraft to target was displayed for 1000ms in the center of the screen every 4500 ms. Simultaneously with the onset of the written name of the color, a spoken color name distractor that participants had to ignore was presented during 280 ms. This auditory distractor was either congruent (10%, spoken color name matched the written color), incongruent (10%, spoken color name did not matched the written color) or neutral (10%, no aircraft on the screen corresponded to the spoken color name). A frequent spoken color name was also presented (70%, always gray ). The task difficulty varied in terms of working memory load with an n-back-like sub-task. In the low load condition, participants had to target the aircraft corresponding to the currently presented written instruction (n = 0). In the high load condition, participants had to target the aircraft corresponding to the instruction presented two trials before (n = 2) (Figure 1). The quality of target aircraft pursuit was indicative of participants' piloting performance. We computed accuracy for each condition as a percentage of correct aircraft targeting. We considered that an aircraft was followed correctly if the distance between the user s aircraft and the target one was inferior to 100 pixels and it remained below this threshold for the 90% of the remaining trial length. 2.4. Procedure Participants were seated comfortably in a reclining armchair in a dimmed and sound-dampened experimental room. The task was displayed on a high-resolution computer located at eye level 70 cm in front of the participant. Participants were first trained for the two difficulty conditions using two sets of 20 trials. They were then equipped with the EEG electrode cap as well as the Electro-OculoGraphic (EOG) electrodes (blinks or saccades detection) and went through the two experimental blocks of 250 trials each. Immediately after each block, participants filled out the NASA Task Load Index (NASA TLX, [13]) questionnaire (Figure 2). This questionnaire provides an evaluation of the subjective mental demand elicited by the task for each level of difficulty. Fig. 1. In the low load condition (n-back 0), participants had to immediately target the aircraft corresponding to the currently presented written instruction blue. In the high load condition (n-back 2), participants had to target the blue aircraft two trials later. The auditory distractor was played for 280 ms while the written instruction was displayed for 1000ms. Each trial lasted for 4500 ms.

Mickaël Causse et al. / Procedia Manufacturing 3 ( 2015 ) 5230 5236 5233 Fig. 2. Timeline of the experiment. The whole procedure lasted 85min, including 5 min training, 30min to install the EEG sensors, 40 min of EEG recording and 10 min to fill the two NASA TLX questionnaires. 2.5. Electroencephalography EEG was amplified and recorded with a ActiveTwo BioSemi system (BioSemi, Amsterdam, The Netherlands) from 30 Ag/AgCl active electrodes (http://www.biosemi.com) mounted on a cap and placed on the scalp according to the International 10 20 System (FP1, FP2, AF3, AF4, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, FC5, FC1, FC2, FC6, CP5, CP1, Cz, CP2, CP6, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8, T7, T8, PO3, PO4, O1, Oz, O2) plus two sites below the eyes for eye movement monitoring. Two additional electrodes placed close to Cz, the Common Mode Sense [CMS] active electrode and the Driven Right Leg [DRL] passive electrode, were used to form the feedback loop that drives the average potential of the participant as close as possible to the AD-box reference potential. Electrode impedance was kept below 5 kω for scalp electrodes, and below 10 kω for the four eye channels. Skin-electrode contact, obtained using electroconductive gel, was monitored, keeping voltage offset from the CMS below 25 mv for each measurement site. All the signals were (DC) amplified and digitalized continuously with a sampling rate of 512 Hz with an anti-aliasing filter with 3 db point at 104 Hz (fifth order sinc filter); no high-pass filtering was applied online. The triggering signals to each word onset were recorded on additional digital channels. EEG data were off-line re-referenced to the average activity of the two mastoids and band-pass filtered (0.1 40 Hz, 12 db/octave), given that for some subjects the low-pass filter was not effective in completely removing the 75Hz artifact. Epochs were time locked to the offer presentation and extracted in the interval from - 200ms to 1000ms. Segments with excessive blinks and/or artefacts (such as excessive muscle activity) were eliminated off-line before data averaging. The lost data (due to artefacts) were equal to 7%. A 200 ms pre-stimulus baseline was used in all analyses. 3. Results 3.1. Behavioral results A 2 2 ANOVA (Congruence [Congruent; Incongruent] Load [Low; High]) revealed a main effect of the load on the piloting performance, the participants were better at aircraft targeting for the low load condition compared to high load condition [F(1, 14) = 8.15, p <.05, ηp 2 =.37; see Figure 3]. On the contrary, we found no effect of congruence, [F(1, 14) = 1.52, p =.22], neither Load Congruence interaction [F(1, 14) = 1.08, p =.37] on the piloting performance.

5234 Mickaël Causse et al. / Procedia Manufacturing 3 ( 2015 ) 5230 5236 3.2. NASA-TLX questionnaire Fig. 3. Mean accuracy for low and high load conditions. Error bars indicate S.E.M. A 2 2 ANOVA (Congruence [Congruent; Incongruent] Load [Low; High]) showed a significant difference between the low load and the high load conditions on the mental demand dimension of the NASA-TLX (Figure 4), [F(1, 12) = 40.53, p <.001, ηp 2 =.77]. Participants estimated the high load condition as more mentally demanding (M = 73.5, SD = 4.2) compared with the low load condition (M = 34.6, SD = 6.2). 3.3 EEG/ERP P300 analyses The P300 amplitude (more precisely the P3b) was assessed in terms of mean amplitude in the 400 450 ms time window (Figure 5). A 3 2 2 (Electrode [Fz, Cz, Pz] x Congruence [congruent, incongruent] x Load [low, high]) ANOVA was conducted. Tukey s HSD tests were used for post-hoc contrasts. The analysis revealed a main effect of load [F(1, 14) = 5.18, p <.05, ηp 2 =.27], with a greater P3b amplitude in low load (M = 3.96 μv, SD = 7.52) than in high load (M = 1.27 μv, SD = 6.28). The analysis also revealed a significant Electrode Congruence interaction [F(2, 28) = 4.24, p <.05, ηp 2 =.23], with a greater P3b amplitude for incongruent trials (M = 1.40 μv, SD = 7.21) than for congruent trials (M = - 61 μv, SD = 7.89, p <.05) at Fz but not at Cz (incongruent trials: M = 2.38 μv, SD = 6.57; congruent trials: M = 2.11 μv, SD = 7.80, p =.99) and at Pz (incongruent trials: M = 5.13 μv, SD = 4.95; congruent trials: M = 5.29 μv, SD = 7.00, p =.99). Fig. 4. Mean index of perceived mental demand for low and high load conditions. Error bars indicate S.E.M.

Mickaël Causse et al. / Procedia Manufacturing 3 ( 2015 ) 5230 5236 5235 * * * * Fig. 5. Grand average ERP waveforms A) for low load (dotted blue line) and high load (black line); B) for congruent (black line) and incongruent trials (dotted red line) at Fz, Cz and Pz electrodes. The horizontal axis denotes time in milliseconds (ms), with the P300 time windows used for the indicated analysis, and the vertical axis denotes amplitude in microvolts (μv). Negative is plotted down; Zeros on the timeline indicate the onset of both the written instruction and the auditory distractor. 4. Discussion In the present study we investigated how working memory load impacted the processing of important visual instruction during a dynamic piloting task. We also investigated how concurrent auditory task-irrelevant stimuli (distractors) interfered with the piloting task whether they were congruent or not with the written instruction. Finally, we investigated how these two factors influence brain activity observed through the amplitude of the P3b component. The higher level of difficulty (n-back 2) provoked both a decrease in piloting performance and an increase in subjective workload (i.e., NASA TLX questionnaire). The manipulation of the load in working memory was effective in that higher memory load made the task more difficult to perform; in particular, to correctly maintain and manipulate the instructions in working memory. At an electrophysiological level, the ERPs revealed that the amplitude of the P3b was higher in the low load condition than in the high load condition. The P3b has been regarded as a sign of processes of memory access that are evoked by evaluation of stimuli in tasks that require some form of action like a covert or overt response [14], ecologically close to instruction or alerts occurring in a cockpit. Diminished amplitude of the P3b response to visual and auditory instructions (we cannot dissociate ERP related to written instruction from ERP related to auditory distractors) is expected when the task at hand requires more processing or working memory [15]. Taken together, these behavioral and electrophysiological results demonstrate that the increase in cognitive load may have led to a reduced openness of the attentional system, and at least partially disrupted the encoding and maintenance of the relevant visual information (i.e., written instruction).

5236 Mickaël Causse et al. / Procedia Manufacturing 3 ( 2015 ) 5230 5236 Our EEG results also demonstrated that P3b was sensitive to the congruency/incongruence between the written instruction and the auditory distractor. Incongruent distractors elicited larger P300 for the Fz electrode. However it did not result in a significant behavioral interference. As participants were required to make a response in all trials to target a new aircraft, it is likely that they put more effort in the evaluation of the relevant stimuli the written instruction to perform the correct action. This increase in P3b amplitude with incongruent auditory distractors seems to emphasize that more resources were recruited to cope with the interference. This hypothesis is supported by the fact that the piloting performances were not impacted by the interference. In conclusion, our findings suggest that P3b can be an efficient tool to measure cognitive load in ecological settings. In the present study we adapted the auditory-visual interference to a simple piloting task. In this context, cognitive load tracked by P3b was load on memory induced by different task difficulties and effort of resisting to irrelevant auditory stimuli. On one hand, P3b amplitude decreased when participants resources allocated to the processing of instruction diminished under load, which resulted in diminished performance. On the other hand, P3b amplitude increased when participants resources allocated to the processing of instruction increased to resist to interference, which resulted in preserved performance. References [1] Hughes RW, Hurlstone MJ, Marsh JE, Vachon F, Jones DM (2013) Cognitive control of auditory distraction: impact of task difficulty, foreknowledge, and working memory capacity supports duplex-mechanism account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 39: 539. [2] Spence C, Read L (2003) Speech shadowing while driving on the difficulty of splitting attention between eye and ear. Psychological science 14: 251-256. [3] Wood NL, Cowan N (1995) The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: attention and memory in the classic selective listening procedure of Cherry (1953). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124: 243. [4] Fuchs PA, Plack CJ, Moore DR, Rees A, Palmer A (2010) The Oxford Handbook of Auditory Science: The Auditory Brain: Oxford university press. [5] Brand-D Abrescia M, Lavie N (2008) Task coordination between and within sensory modalities: Effects on distraction. Perception & Psychophysics 70: 508-515. [6] Santangelo V, Olivetti Belardinelli M, Spence C (2007) The suppression of reflexive visual and auditory orienting when attention is otherwise engaged. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 33: 137. [7] Sinnett S, Costa A, Soto-Faraco S (2006) Manipulating inattentional blindness within and across sensory modalities. The quarterly journal of experimental psychology 59: 1425-1442. [8] Berti S, Schröger E (2003) Working memory controls involuntary attention switching: evidence from an auditory distraction paradigm. European Journal of Neuroscience 17: 1119-1122. [9] Berti S (2013) The role of auditory transient and deviance processing in distraction of task performance: a combined behavioral and eventrelated brain potential study. Frontiers in human neuroscience 7. [10]. Munka L, Berti S (2006) Examining task-dependencies of different attentional processes as reflected in the P3a and reorienting negativity components of the human event-related brain potential. Neuroscience letters 396: 177-181. [1]. Lavie N (2005) Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load. Trends in cognitive sciences 9: 75-82. [12] Polich J (2007) Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical neurophysiology 118: 2128-2148. [13] Hart S, Staveland L (1988) Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of empirical and theoretical research. In: Hancock PA, Meshkati N, editors. Human mental workload. Amsterdam: North Holland Press. pp. 139 183. [14]Donchin E, Kramer A, Wickens C (1986) Applications of event-related brain potentials to problems in engineering psychology. Psychophysiology: Systems, Processes, and Applications New York: Guilford Press. [15] Giraudet L, St-Louis M-E, Scannella S, Causse M (2015) P300 event-related potential as an indicator of inattentional deafness? PLoS one 10: e0118556-e0118556.